Skip to main content
Log in

Young children category learning: a training study

  • Published:
European Journal of Psychology of Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

From an integrative approach, this work focuses on the role of conceptual mechanisms, such as comparison and conceptual-based inference, and sociopragmatic support in young children's taxonomic categorization. “Experiment 1” assessed whether 3-, 4-, and 6-year-old children succeed in detecting taxonomic relations on their own. A clear developmental trend was found: 6-year-olds succeeded, whereas 4- and 3-year-olds relied primarily on perceptually based categories. “Experiment 2” assessed if 3-year-olds are able to change their perceptual response into a taxonomic categorization as a function of the co-occurrence of contingent category information and feedback in an interactive process with an adult (experimenter). A pretest–posttest training study compared 3-year-olds' performance in four conditions: comparison, conceptual-based, information-only, and feedback-only. A perceptual–totaxonomic shift was found only in the comparison and conceptual-based training groups. Children who only received either category information or corrective feedback did not make such a shift. The results show that social interaction with supportive adults is a mechanism that drives conceptual understanding in early childhood.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baldwin, D. A. (1992). Clarifying the role of shape in children’s taxonomic assumption. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 54, 392–416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, P., & Mandler, J. M. (1989). Taxonomies and triads: Conceptual organization in one-to two-year olds. Cognitive Psychology, 21, 156–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benelli, B. (1988). On the linguistic origin of superordinate categorization. Human Development, 31, 20–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blaye, A., & Bonthoux, F. (2001). Thematic and taxonomic relations in preschoolers: The development of flexibility in categorization choice. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 19, 395–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Booth, A., Waxman, S. R., & Huang, Y. T. (2005). Conceptual information permeates word learning in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 41(3), 491–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowerman, M. (1976). Semantic factors in the acquisition of rules for word use and sentence construction. In D. M. Morehead & A. E. Morehead (Eds.), Normal and deficient child language (pp. 99–179). Baltimore: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S., Goodnow, J. J., & Austin, C. A. (1956). A study of thinking. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Callanan, M. A. (1985). How parents label objects for young children: The role of input in the acquisition of category hierarchies. Child Development, 56, 508–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Callanan, M. (1991). Parent–child collaboration in young children’s understanding of categories hierarchies. In S. A. Gelman & J. P. Byrnes (Eds.), Perspectives on language and thought. Interrelations in development (pp. 440–484). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Chipman, S. F., & Medelson, M. J. (1979). Influence of six types of visual structure on complexity judgments in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 356–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, E. (1973). What’s in a word? On the child’s acquisition of semantics in his first language. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language (pp. 65–110). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, E. (1988). On the logic of contrast. Journal of Child Language, 15, 317–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, L. B., & Oakes, L. M. (1993). How infants perceive a simple causal event. Developmental Psychology, 29, 421–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fenson, L., Cameron, M. S., & Kennedy, M. (1988). Role of perceptual and conceptual similarity in category matching at age two years. Child Development, 59, 897–907.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garton, A. F. (2001). Peer assistance in children’s problem solving. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 19, 307–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, S. A., & Coley, J. D. (1990). The importance of knowing a dodo is a bird: Categories and inferences in two-year-old-children. Developmental Psychology, 26, 796–804.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, S. A., Colley, J. D., Rosengren, K. S., Hartman, E. H., Pappas, A., & Keil, F. C. (1998). Beyond labeling: The role of maternal input in the acquisition of richly structured categories. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, vol. 63, no. 1.

  • Gentner, D. (1978). What looks like a jiggy but acts like a zimbo? A study of early word meaning using artificial objects. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, IS, 1–6.

  • Gentner, D. (1988). Metaphor as structure mapping: the relational shift. Child Developoment, 59, 47–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D., & Imai, M. (1995). A further examination of the shape bias in early word learning. Proceedings of the Twenty-Sixth Annual Child Language Research Forum, 167–176.

  • Gentner, D., & Namy, L. L. (1999). Comparison in the development of categories. Cognitive Development, 14, 487–513.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D., & Rattermann, M. (1991). Language and the career of similarity. In S. A. Gelman & J. P. Byrnes (Eds.), Perspectives on thought and language: Interrelations in development (pp. 225–277). London: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Bailey, L. M., & Wenger, N. R. (1992). Young children and adults use lexical principles to learn new nouns. Developmental Psychology, 28(99–1), 08.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graham, S. A., & Diesendruck, G. (2010). Fifteen-month-old infants attend to shape over other perceptual properties in an induction task. Cognitive Development, 25, 111–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, S. A., Kilbreath, C. S., & Welder, A. N. (2004). Thirteen-month-olds rely on shared labels and shape similarity for inductive inferences. Child Development, 75, 409–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, T. R. (1994). What kindergartners know about class inclusion hierarchies? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 57, 72–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Imai, M., Gentner, D., & Uchida, N. (1994). Children’s theories of word meaning: The role of shape similarity in early acquisition. Cognitive Development, 9, 45–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keil, F. C. (1989). Concepts, kinds, and cognitive development. Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keil, F. C. (1999). Conceptual change. In R. A. Wilson & F. C. Keil (Eds.), The MIT encyclopedia of the cognitive science (pp. 179–182). Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keil, F. C., & Batterman, N. (1984). A characteristic-to-defining shift in the development of word meaning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 221–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kemler Nelson, D. G. (1995). Principle-based inferences in young children’s categorization: Revisiting the impact of function on the naming of artifacts. Cognitive Development, 10, 347–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kotovsky, L., & Gentner, D. (1996). Comparison and categorization in the development of relational similarity. Child Development, 67, 2797–2822.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. S. (1982). Commensurability, comparability, communicability. In P. D. Asquith & T. Nickles (Eds.), PSA 1982 (Vol. 2, pp. 669–688). East Lansing: Philosophy of Science Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landau, B., Smith, L. B., & Jones, S. (1988). The importance of shape in early lexical learning. Cognitive Development, 3, 299–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landau, B., Smith, L. B., & Jones, S. (1998). Object shape, object function, and object name. Journal of Memory & Language, 38, 1–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Madole, K. L., & Oakes, L. M. (1999). Making sense of infant categorization: Stable processes and changing representations. Developmental Review, 19, 263–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, J. M. (1992). How to build a baby II: Conceptual primitives. Psychological Review, 99, 587–604.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mandler. J. M. (1998). Representation. In D. Kuhn, & R. S. Siegler (Eds.). W. Damon (Series Ed.), Hadbook of child psychology: vol 2. Cognition, Psychology, and Language (pp. 199-309). New York: Wiley.

  • Mandler, J. M. (2000). Perceptual and conceptual processes in infancy. Journal of Cognition and Development, 1, 3–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, J. M. (2004). The foundations of mind: Origins of conceptual thought. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman, E. M. (1989). Categorization and naming in children: Problems of induction. Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman, E. M., & Hutchinson, J. E. (1984). Children’s sensibility to constraints on word meaning: Taxonomic versus thematic relationship. Cognitive Psychology, 16, 1–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markman, E. M., & Wachtel, G. F. (1988). Children’s use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meaning of words. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 121–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Namy, L. L., & Gentner, D. (2002). Making a silk purse out of two sow’s ears: Young children’s use of comparison in category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 131, 5–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (1973). Some evidence for the cognitive primacy of categorization and its functional basis. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 19(2), 1–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (1985). Making sense: The acquisition of shared meaning. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (1996). Language in cognitive development. The emergence of the mediated mind. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

  • Nelson, K., & Nelson, A. J. (1990). Category production in response to script and categories cues by kindergarten and second grade children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 11, 431–446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Opfer, J. E., & Siegler, R. S. (2004). Revisiting preschoolers living things concept: A microgenetic analysis of conceptual change in basic biology. Cognitive Psychology, 49, 301–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1959). La gene`se des structures logiques e´le´mentaires [The origin of elementary logical structures]. Neuchaˆtel: Delachaux & Niestle´.

  • Quinn, P. C. (2002). Category representation in young infants. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 66–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rakison, D. (2000). When a rose is just a rose: The illusion of taxonomies in infant categorization. Infancy, 1(1), 77–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rakison, D. H., & Butterworth, G. E. (1998). Infants’ use of object parts in early categorization. Developmental Psychology, 34, 49–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B., Ellis, S., & Gardner, W. (1984). Adjustment of adult–child instruction according to child’s age and task. Developmental Psychology, 20, 193–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, L. B., Jones, S. S., & Landau, B. (1992). Count nouns, adjectives, and perceptual properties in children’s novel word interpretations. Developmental Psychology, 28, 273–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waxman, S. R. (1990). Linguistic biases and the establishment of conceptual hierarchies: Evidence from preschool children. Cognitive Development, 5(2), 123–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waxman, S. R., & Gelman, R. (1986). Preschoolers’ use of superordinate level relations in classification and language. Cognitive Development, 1, 139–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waxman, S. R., & Kosowski, T. (1990). Nouns mark category relations: Toddlers’ and preschoolers’ word-learning biases. Child Development, 61(5), 1461–1473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waxman, S. R., & Markow, D. B. (1993). Words as invitations to form categories: Evidence from 12-month-old infants. Cognitive Psychology, 29, 257–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waxman, S. R., Senghas, A., & Benveniste, S. (1997). A cross-linguistic examination of the noun-category bias: Its existence and specificity in French- and Spanish-speaking preschool-aged children. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 183–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winer, G. (1980). Class inclusion reasoning in children: A review of the empirical literature. Child Development, 51, 309–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yu, Y., & Nelson, K. (1993). Slot-filler and conventional category organization in young Korean children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 16, 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a Doctoral Fellowship from the National Research Council of Argentina to the first author and by Grant PIP 1099 from the National Research Council and Grant PICT 01754 from the National Agency of Scientific Promotion to the second author. We are grateful to the institutions and children who participated in this research. We also want to thank Georgene Troseth and the anonymous reviewers for their comments and suggestions on earlier drafts.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Olga Alicia Peralta.

Additional information

Olga Alicia Peralta. Professor of Educational Psychology-Scientific Researcher Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (CONICET) (National Research Council) Argentina Address: Instituto Rosario de Investigación en Ciencias de la Educación (IRICE) (Rosario Institute for Research in Sciences of Education) 27 de Febrero 210 bis, 2000, Rosario, Argentina. Phone: +54-341-4821770, extension 204. E-mail: peralta@irice-conicet.gov.ar

Current themes of research:

Symbolic and conceptual development in young children. Social interaction. Cognitive development.

Most relevant publications in the field of Psychology of Education:

PERALTA, O. A. (1995). Developmental changes and socioeconomic differences in mother–infant picturebook reading, European Journal of Psychology of Education, 3, 261–272.

PERALTA, O. A. (1997). Estilos de interacción cognitiva materno-infantil en una situación de resolución de problemas en función del nivel socioeconómico y de la edad del niño. (Mother–child cognitive interaction styles in a problem-solving situation as a function of socioeconomic status and child’s age). Infancia y Aprendizaje, 80, 85–90.

PERALTA, O.A. and SALSA, A. (2003). Instruction in early in early comprehension and use of a symbol-referent relation. Cognitive Development, 1 (2), 269–284.

SALSA, A. and PERALTA, O. A. (2007). Routes to Symbolization: Intentionality and Correspondence in Early Understanding of Pictures. Journal of Cognition and Development, 8 (1), 79–92. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. NJ-London.

PERALTA, O. and MAITA, M. (2010). El Impacto de la Instrucción en la Comprensión Temprana de un Mapa como Objeto Simbólico (The impact of Instruction in the early comprehension of a map as a symbolic object). Infancia y Aprendizaje. Madrid.

PERALTA, O. y SALSA, A. (2011) Instrucción y desarrollo en la comprensión temprana de fotografías como objetos simbólicos (Instruction and development in early comprehension of photographs as symbolic objects). Anales de Psicología, 27 (1), 118–125. Murcia.

MAITA, M. y PERALTA, O. (in press). La enseñanza materna de la función simbólica de un objeto (Maternal teaching of a symbolic function of an object). Cultura y Educación., 24 (3).

Andrea Sabina Taverna. Post Doctoral Student Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (CONICET) (National Research Council) Argentina Address: Instituto Rosario de Investigación en Ciencias de la Educación (IRICE) (Rosario Institute for Research in Sciences of Education) 27 de Febrero 210 bis, 2000, Rosario, Argentina. Phone: 54 341 4821770 extension 204. E-mail: taverna@irice-conicet.gov.ar

Current themes of research:Conceptual development in young children. Social interaction. and Cognitive development.

Most relevant publications in the field of Psychology of Education:

TAVERNA, A. and PERALTA. O. (2009). Desarrollo conceptual: Perspectivas actuales en la adquisición temprana de conceptos. Conceptual development: current approaches to children’s early concepts acquisition. Psykhe, 18 (1), 49–59.

TAVERNA, A. and PERALTA. O. (2010). Rutas hacia la Formación del Significado de las Palabras. Naturaleza, Experiencia, Construcción y Contexto (Routes towards word meaning. Nature, experience, construction and context). Estudios de Psicología.

TAVERNA, A. y PERALTA, O. (2012). Comparación e Inferencia en la Categorización de Artefactos No-familiares. Un Estudio con Niños Pequeños (Comparison and inference in the categorization of non-familiar artifacts). Psykhé, 21 (1), 21–36.

Appendices

Appendix A

Table 2 List of items in “Experiment 2

Appendix B

Table 3 List of vignettes in “Experiment 2

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Taverna, A.S., Peralta, O.A. Young children category learning: a training study. Eur J Psychol Educ 28, 599–617 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-012-0130-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-012-0130-9

Keywords

Navigation