Introduction

Urban areas create unprecedented pressures for the wildlife that dwell in them (Sih et al. 2016). Among those pressures is noise pollution, which affects how animals communicate (Brumm and Slabbekoorn 2005), form pair bonds (Swaddle and Page 2007a), forage and protect themselves against predators (Wale et al. 2013; Jung et al. 2020; Sweet et al. 2022), and use space (McClure et al. 2013; Chen and Koprowski 2015; Jung et al. 2020; Liu et al. 2020). Given the known effects of noise on individual and group-driven behaviors (Bruintjes and Radford 2013), we suspect these effects of noise compound to change how individuals and groups interact with each other at the social network level (i.e., social behavior).

Some avian species alter their habitat use and spatial distribution along noise gradients, by choosing to settle in quieter areas (Injaian et al. 2018a; Gomes et al. 2021) or decreasing abundance as noise levels increase (McClure et al. 2013; Chen and Koprowski 2015). When given the choice between a high-amplitude noise environment and a quieter, low-amplitude noise environment in captive settings, previous work demonstrates that birds will either spend more time in the low-amplitude environment (Liu et al. 2020) or change their behavior in the high-amplitude noise environment (Kight and Swaddle 2015; Derryberry et al. 2017; Evans et al. 2018). Changes in avian behavior in response to noise included altering the frequency or amplitude of songs, or even changing the timing of dawn chorus to avoid commuter traffic (Fuller et al. 2007; Kight and Swaddle 2015; Derryberry et al. 2016, 2020; Luther et al. 2016; Zwart et al. 2016). Traffic noise is a prevalent source of noise pollution in urban areas; however, there is spatial and temporal variation in the intensity of traffic noise (e.g., varying road traffic on highways vs backroads, times of rush-hour traffic). To date, many studies of noise pollution have compared extreme ends of an amplitude gradient (i.e., noise-added compared with no-noise added; cf. (McClure et al. 2013; Ware et al. 2015). However, given the spatial and temporal heterogeneity of traffic noise, it is important to understand how variation in noise intensity influences wildlife and their behavior. Such scenarios may better represent the variation in exposure to noise that urban-dwelling species experience.

For animals that remain in high-amplitude noise conditions, noise can mask acoustic signals and disrupt communication if there is a frequency and/or temporal overlap between the noise and the signals. Tactics to compensate for masking effects in noise include adjusting vocal frequency and/or vocal performance (rate of sounds being produced given their frequency bandwidth) and/or amplitude to increase detection (Brumm 2004; McMullen et al. 2014; Kight and Swaddle 2015; Luther et al. 2016 Derryberry et al. 2020) or vocalizing at different times of day (Fuller et al. 2007; Cartwright et al. 2014). Physiologically, noise can alter baseline stress hormones and/or stress response in the individuals exposed to it, although the changes to glucocorticoid profiles appear to be species-dependent (Crino et al. 2013; Angelier et al. 2016; Davies et al. 2017; Injaian et al. 2018b; Zollinger et al. 2019). Additionally, noise creates cross-sensory interference and can affect visual and olfactory processing (Chan et al. 2010; Kunc et al. 2014; Morris-Drake et al. 2016; Halfwerk and Van Oers 2020; Zhu et al. 2022).

Disruptions to normal social behavior can have wide implications for other processes, as social behavior can modulate territory defense (Nowicki et al. 1998), mating decisions (Poesel et al. 2006; Baldassarre et al. 2016), and how information spreads within groups (Urban et al. 2011). Additionally, individuals within a population could respond to disturbance differently depending on their personality traits (e.g., explorer vs sedentary, aggression levels) (Grunst et al. 2019; Harding et al. 2019). Changes to individuals can have group-level consequences for social networks, including affecting the connectivity among individuals in the network or the group’s ability to access resources (Balasubramaniam et al. 2021; Wiśniewska et al. 2022). Changes to sensory information processing and communication could therefore influence social dynamics independently of conspecific density and space use.

Our knowledge regarding the effects of disturbance on social networks is expanding, receiving more attention in recent years. For example, mixed-species groups of large herbivores increase social affinity in response to high rainfall levels (Meise et al. 2019), and simulation-based models reveal that social connectivity of elephant herds decreases in response to targeted poaching of larger, older individuals (Wiśniewska et al. 2022). In free-living social birds, the experimental introduction of traffic noise in an otherwise quiet area resulted in decreased social interactions within and among cooperatively breeding groups (Hawkins et al. 2020).

In this study, our main goal was to investigate how noise could change characteristics of social behavior, including individual sociality and group-level social clustering in birds chronically exposed to elevated ambient noise levels—conditions that urban-dwelling birds would experience. Since birds might disperse away from the source of noise, we also investigated how the spatial distribution of the birds changed across a noise gradient. We created an experimental sound gradient of traffic noise across aviaries in which the amplitude of the noise varied from high noise (~ 80 dBA SPL) to lower noise (~ 70 dBA SPL) within a free-flight aviary (Fig. 1). Using flocks of captive-bred zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata), we measured changes in individual social connectivity and whole-flock clustering before, during, and after introducing traffic noise. We also recorded how birds used the space across the sound gradient to determine if birds preferred using the quieter side of the aviary during the noise playback.

Zebra finches maintain their highly social flocking nature when housed in free-flight aviaries (Zann 1996). They quickly form pair bonds within the flock and are rarely without their social partner. Birds engage in flock activities such as feeding, watering, and allopreening (Zann 1996). Zebra finches rely on auditory cues for individual recognition (Zann 1996), making their social system particularly vulnerable to changes in noise conditions. Previous work on this species has demonstrated that noise inhibits cognitive performance and social learning (Osbrink et al. 2021), changes vigilance behavior while foraging (Evans et al. 2018), and decreases pair bond preferences (Swaddle and Page 2007b), which suggests larger changes could be occurring at the social network level in response to noise disturbance.

We hypothesized birds would change how they used aviary space in response to noise and predicted that they would increase time spent in the quieter end of the noise gradient at higher densities than before the noise treatment. We therefore hypothesized increased densities of conspecifics would affect individual social connectivity and whole-flock clustering predicting an increase in both connectivity and clustering with the onset of noise.

Methods

We used a large colony of outbred, domesticated zebra finches that were housed in outdoor aviaries at William & Mary (Williamsburg, Virginia, USA, 37.3° N, 76.7° W). Zebra finches used in this study were raised in relatively noisy conditions, as their parents had been for at least eight generations, due to the incidental effects of having a captive breeding colony in indoor rooms with a relatively old air conditioning system and highly reflective concrete walls and floors. Typical noise levels in indoor breeding rooms were between 55 and 86 dBA SPL, which varies based on air conditioner ventilation bouts and fluctuations in group vocalization among the colony. All procedures used on animals were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at William & Mary (IACUC-2015-11-16-10733-jpswad).

We conducted a total of 11 trials between May and October 2017 in two large outdoor aviaries. Each aviary consisted of three conjoined rooms (3 × 3 × 6 m each, a collective size of 9 × 3 × 6 m) among which birds could freely move (Fig. 1). Birds had ad libitum access to a nutritionally balanced diet of millet mix (Volkman Super Finch Blend), water, and cuttlefish bone. Feeders were placed in rooms 1 and 3 of each aviary (Fig. 1). For each trial, one of the two aviaries received a high-amplitude treatment where either room 1 or 3 had a speaker play traffic noise, creating an amplitude gradient across the three rooms where the furthest room from the speaker was the quietest. This allowed the birds to reduce their noise exposure, but not entirely escape it, which is ecologically relevant to urbanized areas. The noise continued across our facilities to the second aviary at a lower amplitude, with the loudest of the three rooms ~ 20 dBA quieter than the loudest room in the high-noise treatment; this aviary served as our “lower” noise treatment. The lower noise treatment only had a small increase in sound pressure (1–2 dB SPL) during playback from the high-noise treatment, as the traffic sounds were quiet relative to the background noise of the facility. However, the representation of traffic sounds was palpable even at this distance and novel to the lower noise birds, as the aviary is not located near any roads.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Plan view of aviary. Thick gray bars indicate wooden perches. The three cages were connected by one long perch, and central walls were covered in noise-absorbing foam. A 1-m-long high perch and a 0.5-m-long low perch were placed in rooms 1 (R1) and 3 (R3). F indicates food and water station (0.5-m diameter tray), and S indicates speakers (speakers not drawn to scale). We had two identical aviaries with this set-up to serve as high-amplitude noise and low-amplitude noise treatments, we alternated treatments throughout trials. For the high-noise treatment, the two speakers in either R1 or R3 (alternating between trials) would play traffic noise and the two speakers in the opposite room would remain inactive, creating a sound gradient across this structure

We took sound pressure readings using an EXTECH General Purpose Sound Level Meter and stood at the feeder locations in rooms 1 and 3 (Fig. 1) on day 2 of each experimental phase for each trial. The sound meter was calibrated with an EXTECH 407744 Sound Level Calibrator, which calibrates the meter with a 1 kHz sine wave at 94 dB SPL with an accuracy of ± 0.8 dB standard error. With no noise playing, peak amplitude of ambient noise pressure in the outdoor aviaries averaged 60.62 ± 0.52 standard error dBA SPL; this average was calculated by using all measurements from rooms 1 and 3 in both high- and lower-amplitude treatments from the before and after phases when there was no noise playback (n = 85). In the high-amplitude aviary during traffic noise playback, peak amplitude in the room with noise playback averaged 80.09 dBA SPL ± 0.99 SE and 70.94 dBA SPL ± 2.94 SE in the room furthest from the playback speaker (n = 11). In the lower-amplitude aviary during the traffic noise playback, peak amplitude averaged 62.53 dBA SPL ± 0.75 SE in the room closest to the high-amplitude aviary and 61.96 dBA SPL ± 0.98 SE in the furthest room (n = 11).

We used a pseudo-randomized blocking procedure to determine the aviary and room that would receive sound treatment; thus, we used both aviaries for high noise and low noise equally and alternated which room (1 or 3) had noise for each trial. This was to account for any preexisting preferences for rooms or aviaries that the birds may have, independent of our experimental manipulation.

For each trial, we released 12 birds (six adult male, six adult female) marked with individual color bands into the high-amplitude aviary and a separate 12 birds in the lower-amplitude aviary and gave them 72 h to habituate to the enclosure and establish their social groups with no noise playback. Of the 12 birds in each aviary, males and females were unfamiliar with each other, but birds could have been familiar with each other within the sexes, as individuals were previously housed with approximately 100 others in same-sex free-flight conditions. All birds were wild-type coloration (to the human eye) to control for any social preferences for unique colorations sometimes found in captivity (e.g., fawn, black-cheeked, white). Each bird was used only once and did not appear in subsequent trials. Hence, we used 264 birds over the course of 11 trials (24 birds per trial).

Following the initial 72-h habituation period, each trial consisted of three additional 72-h experimental phases: (i) “before” which was the phase before the experimental noise manipulation, (ii) “noise” when the high-amplitude aviary received traffic noise playback (which slightly reached our lower-amplitude aviary), and (iii) “after” which was the time period immediately following the termination of the active playback in the noise phase (Table 3). During the noise phase, speakers directed towards the opposite wall of their placement played a looped audio recording of highway traffic noise that varied in maximum dBA SPL (~ 65–80 dBA) depending on the number of cars and trucks passing on the highway throughout the recording (YouTube 2014) and played in either room 1 or 3 of the aviary depending on trial (Fig. 1). We had four speakers in each aviary set up (two in each of the far rooms) but only one amplifier set to a consistent volume that we moved between aviaries/rooms depending on which was getting the noise treatment in that trial to ensure consistency.

Observation periods took place at either 9:00 EST or 12:00 EST and would last for roughly 2 h (1 h and 36 min of focal-follow time), with time in between as observers searched for the next focal-follow bird. We alternated which aviary had the morning (9 a.m.) or afternoon (12 p.m.) observation daily, so at the end of the experiment, each aviary had an equal number of morning and afternoon observation periods. Speakers were turned on at the conclusion of the 72-h before phase (~ 14:00 EST), played traffic noise continuously day and night, and then turned off at the conclusion of the 72-h noise phase.

We conducted 8-min focal-follows of each bird and defined interactions with an ethogram (Table 1) to collect behavioral data. Social interactions for the social network were defined by proximity (0.5 m horizontal distance). We chose this distance as our shortest perches were 0.5 m, as was the food/water tray available in rooms 1 and 3 of the aviary set-up (Fig. 1). Each data collection period consisted of focal-follows of every bird in a randomized order in both treatments (experimental and reference) daily throughout a trial. During a focal-follow, one focal bird would be observed for 8 min, and only interactions with that focal bird were recorded. This process was repeated for each of the 12 birds in each data collection period. We had three different observers for data collection periods (only one observer was present at each data collection period), and these observers trained together on a pilot flock to determine whether observer bias existed. Using the pilot flock, the three observers that were present together focal-followed the same individuals at the same time to record interactions. Observers compared our recorded interactions, calculated social network metrics, and found minimal differences among our datasets. We confirmed no significant effect of observer using intraclass correlation coefficients. The three observers also rotated which flock they were observing every data collection period, so all three observed an equal number of high-noise or low-noise flocks across the two different aviaries and across experimental phases. Over the course of the study, 260 birds were observed for a total of 312 h of data collection (total birds had been 264, but four birds died early in their trials and were thus excluded from the data).

Table 1 Ethogram used for collecting behavioral and interaction data

We constructed social networks using proximity data, and an interaction is defined as an individual within 0.5 m horizontally of other individuals (e.g., birds were on the same perch or using the same resource), but only with non-aggressive sub-behaviors (shown in white, Table 1), as aggressive sub-behaviors (shown in gray, Table 1) tended to be the displacement of another individual to keep them away from their social group. The detailed non-aggressive sub-behaviors described in our ethogram were for a different project; for our purposes, we were interested in the animals as a group, and so our intent was not to separate these sub-behaviors when constructing our social network.

We then transformed interaction data into association matrices using the simple ratio index (SRI): \(\frac{x}{x+{y}_{a}+{y}_{b}+{y}_{ab}}\) where x is the total number of observations where “a” and “b” are associated, ya is the number of times the focal individual (a) was observed, yb is the number of times the interacting individual (b) was observed, and yab is the total observations where the focal individual and interacting individual were each observed, but not together. An individual’s total number of observations (ya and yb) were calculated by including any appearance of the individual in a focal-follow recording (either as an interacting bird or during its own focal-follow) and summed once focal-follows were completed for all 12 birds. The SRI value represents the strength of the relationship between individuals (or the “edges” between the “nodes” in a social network), weighted by the frequency of interactions. All SRI values are from 0 to 1, 0 being the two individuals never interacted and 1 being that every time individual “a” is observed, “b” is interacting with it. An individual’s sociality can be measured using weighted degree, which is the sum of all SRI values associated with that individual (or the sum of all edge values attached to the node in a social network). Thus, weighted degree accounts for both the total number of social partners that individual has and the frequency of interactions with those social partners.

At the group level, we measured global clustering coefficient which measures how connected the overall social network is by summing the number of closed triplets (a group of 3 birds all connected to each other) divided by the number of triplets of birds that are connected to each other in some way (either as a closed triangle or an open triplet with one or two edges) (Whitehead 2008). This generates a proportion between 0 and 1, where 0 is no three birds are all connected to each other, and 1 is every possible triplet in the network is closed. Weighted degree and global clustering coefficient are repeatable and consistent across time in other studies that lack an experimental intervention (Jacoby et al. 2014), making them suitable candidate metrics to study the effects of noise disturbance on individual and group sociality, as any observed changes in these metrics could be attributed to our noise treatment. We calculated individual weighted degree and flock global clustering coefficient at the end of each experimental phase (72-h period; Before, Noise, and After).

To determine space use, we used a digital data collection sheet that logged time stamps for each entry. A time stamp was recorded at the start of the trial, and additional time stamps would record the exact time a bird switched rooms. These time stamps allowed us to determine the total number of seconds a focal bird spent in each of the three rooms in an aviary. We then calculated the relative proportion of time spent in each room for the focal bird for each phase (Before, Noise, After) by calculating the total seconds spent in each room and dividing this by 1440 s (total 24 min) because each individual had three 8-min focal-follow periods for each experimental phase. This resulted in a percentage value representative of the relative amount of time a bird spent in each room throughout the experiment. For our statistical analysis, we only included the proportion of time spent in the room that had the speakers playing in the Noise phase in the high-noise aviary. For the low-noise aviary, we did this same calculation for the room in closest physical proximity to the high-noise aviary.

Statistical analyses

We wrote a custom Python script to manipulate data which converted raw interaction data to a matrix of association indices. To process social network metrics, we used the “igraph” package in R (Csárdi and Nepusz 2006; R Core Team 2016). To analyze spatial use of the aviary with the onset of noise in our high-noise treatment, we calculated the difference in the proportion of time birds spent in the room with active playback from before to noise periods for the high-noise and low-noise treatments. For the lower noise treatment, we did this calculation for the room in closest proximity to the high-noise aviary. We used generalized linear mixed models (GLMMs) for each of our response variables, weighted degree (gamma model), GCC (Gaussian model), and space use (zero-inflated beta model) using the “lme4” package in R (Bates et al. 2015; R Core Team 2016). Our fixed effects were Treatment which had two levels (low noise and high noise), Phase, which had three levels (before, noise, after), and their interaction. Individual bird identity nested within trial was a random effect for all three models. For GCC, we used a logit transformation to fit a normal distribution, as values were proportions bound between 0 and 1. We obtained p-values for predictors using Wald tests.

Sampling bias

We recognize the risk of sampling bias when collecting social network data and did our best to mitigate potential risks. For example, an observer misidentifying an individual could generate a social network that fails to reveal the actual social structure, particularly with global metrics like global clustering coefficient (Davis et al. 2018). Therefore, if an observer was unsure about the identity of an individual, they were trained to skip this social interaction rather than attempt to guess at the individual’s identity, as missed interactions have less impact on overall network structure than misidentified interactions (Davis et al. 2018). It is worth noting that local metrics, such as weighted degree used in this study, are more robust to observer error. All observers in this study trained on a pilot flock of birds to practice taking observations before the experiment began, and we feel confident in their ability to accurately re-sight color bands to record proximity interactions.

Results

Changes to individual sociality (weighted degree)

Sociality was significantly influenced by Treatment (p < 0.001), phase (p < 0.001), and their interaction (p < 0.001). The effect of Treatment (β1 = 0.19656 ± S.E. = 0.070) indicates that on average, weighted degree increased more in the high-noise treatment compared to the low-noise treatment. The Noise phase increased weighted degree of birds compared to the Before phase (Noise, β2 = 0.12364 ± 0.058) meaning birds had on average more social partners and more interactions with their social partners during Noise in both treatments. Following the termination of the noise treatment in the After phase, weighted degree decreased to levels below, but not significantly different from, that of their original state in the Before phase (After, β3 =  − 0.10323 ± 0.053). These effects, as well as the interaction effects, are given in Table 2. These results reveal that birds experienced change in sociality directly related to the Noise phase of the experiment and returned to a similar social state as Before following the termination of noise in the After phase (Fig. 2).

Table 2 Results of gamma GLMM for weighted degree
Fig. 2
figure 2

Average weighted degree with corresponding ± one standard error bars of high-noise birds (n = 131) and low-noise birds (n = 129) throughout eleven trials (raw data)

Changes to flock (global clustering coefficient)

Global clustering coefficient (GCC), or overall connectedness of the network, was significantly influenced by Phase (F2,40, p = 0.013), but not by Treatment (F1,20, p = 0.054) or by their interaction (F2,40, p = 0.717). Individual factors on their own were not statistically significant, but the overall impact of Phase did demonstrate changes in clustering patterns associated with the onset of the Noise treatment (Table 3, Fig. 3).

Table 3 Results of LMM for GCC
Fig. 3
figure 3

Average global clustering coefficient (logit transformed), with corresponding ± one standard error bars, of high-noise flocks (n = 11) and low-noise flocks (n = 11) throughout trials (raw data)

Changes to space use

Our zero-inflated beta model results in two models, a conditional model that predicts the actual non-zero value of the proportion of time spent in the room of interest and a zero-inflation model that predicts the likelihood of observing a zero in a particular condition (Treatment and/or Phase). In biological context, a zero occurs when no individuals use the room of interest during the entire observation period. Our conditional model demonstrated that changes in space use for the room that received noise treatment were significantly influenced by the interaction of Treatment and Phase (F2,474, p = 0.001) (Table 4). Additionally, zero-values were more likely to occur in the After phase or when considering the interaction between Noise*Treatment and After*Treatment (Table 5). Post hoc paired Wilcoxon rank-sum tests reveal that the significant interaction results in the conditional model were driven by the change between Before and Noise and, interestingly, in opposite directions dependent on treatment. Birds in the high-noise group increased their use of the room that received noise (p < 0.001), and birds in the low-noise treatment decreased their use of the room closest to the noise (p < 0.01) (Fig. 4). This differential response (positive response in high noise, negative response in low noise) led to our mixed model failing to detect the effect of Phase alone (F2,474, p = 0.956), as the mean difference was averaged between the two.

Table 4 Conditional model output for zero-inflated beta GLMM for space use
Table 5 Zero-inflated model output for zero-inflated beta GLMM for space use
Fig. 4
figure 4

Changes in the average proportion of time spent in the room that received noise treatment (high noise) or the room closest adjacent to the noise treatment (low noise) during the Noise phase throughout the trials for high-noise birds (n = 131) and low-noise birds (n = 129) with corresponding ± one standard error bars (raw data)

Discussion

We found that individual sociality (weighted degree) and flock clustering (GCC) both increase with the onset of introduced traffic noise, although the mechanism driving these social changes remains to be discovered. We also observed that removing the noise source led to the birds becoming less social (decrease in weighted degree) than in their pre-disturbed state. Changes in social behavior occurred in both treatment groups (high and lower amplitude), revealing that even in low levels of traffic noise, birds adjusted their social behavior following some exposure to traffic noise sounds. However, the high-amplitude treatment birds experienced a more pronounced response in weighted degree compared to birds in the lower-amplitude groups. We recognize that a no-noise condition would have been ideal for this experiment, but this was not feasible with our aviary resources, as the proximity of aviaries was such that we could not isolate the traffic noise sounds to the high-amplitude playback only. Therefore, we are not able to reject the possibility that the observed pattern is a result of the flock’s social progression over time; however, given the significantly steeper rate of change in the high-amplitude conditions (Figs. 2 and 3), we can conclude that the amplitude of traffic noise exposure playback affected social behavior.

Although we initially predicted that observed changes in social behavior would be driven by changes in space use, we observed conflicting results in changes in space use that were dependent on noise treatment. Unexpectedly, birds that received a high-amplitude treatment spent a higher proportion of time in the playback room during noise than before playback. Birds that received the lower-amplitude treatment decreased their use of the room in closest physical proximity to the high-noise treatment with the onset of noise, which was more in line with our initial predictions. Because our social responses (weighted degree and GCC) between treatment groups were positively correlated with experimental phase in both treatments, but we observed a divergent response dependent on treatment, we propose that changes in social behavior must be driven by alternative mechanisms.

In free-living conditions without space constraints, the densities of birds often decrease in areas with introduced experimental noise playback (Francis et al. 2009; McClure et al. 2013; Mahjoub et al. 2015; Swaddle et al. 2016; Gomes et al. 2021). However, this response is species-dependent with species such as house finches (Carpodacus mexicanus) and black-chinned hummingbirds (Archilochus alexandri) increasing densities in noise sites (possibly due to the dispersal of nest predators) (Francis et al. 2009) or not demonstrating changes in their distributions in response to noise at all, as is the case with red-backed fairywrens (Hawkins et al. 2020). Our study was spatially constrained, with captive birds unable to fully escape noise playback.

In a similar captive study with domesticated zebra finches, birds spent less time in the highest amplitude space during acute playbacks of traffic noise (30 min) (Liu et al. 2020). The birds in our study were exposed to chronic traffic noise over 72 h, and the first measurement of changes in space use occurred after approximately 20-h habituation period to the onset of noise. As social interactions and relationships develop over longer time intervals, we therefore suggest that changes in space use are not the most likely mechanism for changes in social behavior in response to noise.

Despite the known interference of noise with communication regarding signal transmission, we observed increased social behavior in high-amplitude and lower-amplitude conditions. Potential mechanisms for increased flock clustering and individual sociality could be increased perceived predation risk (Mahjoub et al. 2015) as noise decreases detection ability for low-frequency predator cues (Jung et al. 2020), and thus, increased flock density could serve as an antipredatory strategy (Peacor and Lansing 2003; Owens et al. 2012). Indeed, increased vigilance behavior in experimental noise playback has been documented in mammals, birds, and fish (Voellmy et al. 2014; Meillere et al. 2015; Kern and Radford 2016). Free-living dwarf mongoose (Helogale parvula) and captive white-crowned sparrows (Zonotrichia leucophrys) decreased feeding duration and increased vigilance decreased time spent foraging and increased time spent scanning or vigilant (Ware et al. 2015; Kern and Radford 2016). Song sparrows (Melospiza melodia) increased vigilance or decreased foraging in playbacks of high noise from both natural (rivers) and anthropogenic (road) sources (Sweet et al. 2022). Female house sparrows had shorter flushing distances when researchers approached nest boxes in areas with traffic noise playback (Meillere et al. 2015). Three-spined sticklebacks reacted more quickly to a visual predator cue during playback of boat noise (Voellmy et al. 2014). Across avian species, foraging and non-foraging group sizes decrease with relaxed predation risk (Beauchamp 2010), so our result of increased social connectivity in response to increased predation risk is consistent with this pattern.

Alternatively, or in addition to increased perceived predation risk, it is possible that the increased social response is acting as a social buffer to a stressor. Research on the effects of noise on glucocorticoid profiles of birds has yielded mixed results. For baseline corticosterone, house wrens from rural areas exposed to noise had elevated CORT and their urban counterparts did not, but captive zebra finches exposed to noise had lower CORT than their no-noise counterparts (Davies et al. 2017; Zollinger et al. 2019). Nestling, but not adult, tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) exposed to noise in field conditions also had elevated baseline CORT, but nestling mountain white-crowned sparrows had depressed baseline CORT (Crino et al. 2013; Injaian et al. 2018a). However, in nestling house sparrows, there was no apparent effect of exposed traffic noise to baseline CORT or stress response (Angelier et al. 2016). Although the impacts of noise exposure on the glucocorticoid profiles of birds remain subject to further investigation, one explanation for our observed increased social behavior could be social buffering against changes to glucocorticoids. For example, in zebra finches, birds exposed to a mild stressor had reduced stress response if they were in the presence of a familiar conspecific (Emmerson and Spencer 2017). Additionally, we speculate that the observed increase of space use in the noise room for birds in the high-noise treatment could have been driven by a stress-freezing behavior (Korte et al. 2005; De Haas et al. 2012). We observed many of the birds in the high-noise treatment shifted to spending 100% of their time in the noise room with the onset of noise. We recommend further research on how social buffering can possibly mediate the negative effects of noise and how stress response to noise can affect spatial decisions.

As noise is not limited to acoustic interference of communication signals, cross-sensory interference could be another potential mechanism for changes in social behavior (Chan et al. 2010). For example, animals exhibit a dampened response to visual cues in high-noise conditions, such as fewer cryptic prey located by great tits (Parus major) (Halfwerk and Van Oers 2020) and reduced response to attractive visual cues of male serrate-legged small treefrogs (Kurixalus odontotarsus) by females (Zhu et al. 2022). In high-noise environments, dwarf mongooses had delayed response to olfactory cues of predator feces (Morris-Drake et al. 2016). Social behavior is inherently a cross-sensory process (e.g., visual signals like baring teeth in primates, olfactory cues like scent marking in canids and felids, acoustic signals like duetting in songbirds), so there are many avenues in which noise could affect social interactions that are worth exploring in future studies.

Increased social behavior in urban-dwelling birds can have a variety of ecological implications for urban sedentary species. Both social network size and individual sociality affect disease transmission, as individuals with a high weighted degree have a disproportionate effect on the disease transmission throughout a population (Hamede et al. 2009; Rushmore et al. 2013). Thus, denser flocks of birds with more social constituents could alter the rate and prevalence of pathogen transmission. Urban-dwelling birds tend to have increased parasite loads compared to their rural counterparts, and increased social behavior potentially contributes to this pattern (Giraudeau et al. 2014). Possible benefits of increased social behavior include increased transmission of symbiotic microbes that change the gut microflora biodiversity in birds, increasing resistance to disease (Archie and Tung 2015; Levin et al. 2016). Additionally, denser group clustering can increase familiarity between conspecifics, which can decrease individual aggressiveness (Geffroy et al. 2014).

The reduction in social behavior following disturbance in the After phase indicates a level of resistance to disturbance in domesticated zebra finches. While the resulting decreased levels of weighted degree surpassed their baseline levels in the Before phase, it would be relevant to extend the timeline of observations to see if this would return to baseline after more time. Recovering from a disturbed social network implies that remediation strategies for noise pollution could return birds to their natural social state, a phenomenon observed in bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops aduncus) social networks following the implementation of legislation limiting commercial fisheries in their habitat (Ansmann et al. 2012).

Due to the highly social nature of our study system—zebra finches in the wild often live in a range of flock sizes, either smaller or larger than the one we have created here (Zann 1996; McCowan et al. 2015)—our results may not be generalizable to avian species that have different social systems (e.g., solitary birds). Indeed, previous research on a free-living population of red-backed fairywrens, a passerine that forms small familial groups, found that weighted degree decreased with the onset of experimental noise (Hawkins et al. 2020). Additionally, the challenges that free-living birds face, such as predation risk or access to resources, would likely result in selection pressures on populations that influence social dynamics that our captive population did not experience. Notably, previous research on the effects of early-life stress on social network position has demonstrated consistent results between zebra finches in captive studies and studies of wild populations (Brandl et al. 2019). Despite limitations inherent in a captive environment, we feel our results are relevant to interpreting the effects of noise on other highly social species that reside in urban areas in which anthropogenic landscapes fragment habitat and variable gradients of traffic noise are pervasive (e.g., great tits, rock doves (Columba livia), barnacle geese (Branta leucopsis), and gallahs (Eolophus roseicapilla)).

In conclusion, our study provides evidence that traffic noise alters individual sociality and flock clustering in a population chronically exposed to anthropogenic noise. We suggest that the observed changes are driven by factors other than noise-induced constraints on space use and propose hypotheses related to perceived predation risk, social buffering to stress, vigilance, and cross-sensory interference. The rebound of social behavior following the noise introduction suggests that remediation strategies for reducing noise pollution in urban areas stand to benefit urban-dwelling species.