Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

An assistive technology based on Peirce’s semiotics for the inclusive education of deaf and hearing children

  • Long Paper
  • Published:
Universal Access in the Information Society Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In the contemporary educational scenario, heterogeneous classrooms have students with different communication needs. Whether we consider the inclusive education area, one of the most relevant drawbacks is that deaf children have needs that are normally ignored for not fitting with the pattern of other students. Furthermore, a few works have performed scientific researches that deeply investigate the application of models and technologies to tackle differences between deaf and hearing children. Therefore, this work proposes a bilingual literacy model based on Peirce’s semiotics for inclusive education that utilizes software technology to improve the communication process between deaf and hearing children in classrooms. An experiment was conducted to observe the level of communication during games played in class and to investigate the quality of interaction by considering the presence of the Portuguese and the Brazilian Signs Language. We also identified three communication categories between deaf and hearing children: (i) Sign Communication; (ii) Common Vocabulary; and (iii) Formal Signaled Communication. We developed and implemented a semiotic model that supports the learning of Portuguese and Libras alphabets, as well as the process of communication between deaf and hearing children.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9
Fig. 10
Fig. 11
Fig. 12
Fig. 13
Fig. 14
Fig. 15
Fig. 16

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author [Denys F.S. Rocha] on reasonable request.

References

  1. Adoyo, P.O.: Educating deaf children in an inclusive setting in kenya: challenges and considerations. Electron. J. Incl. Educ 2(2), 8 (2007)

    Google Scholar 

  2. de Azevedo Fronza, C., Karnopp, L.B., Tammenga-Helmantel, M.: Deaf education in Brazil: Contexts, challenges, and perspectives. Deaf Education Beyond the Western World: Context, Challenges, and Prospects p. 343 (2019)

  3. Belsis, P., Gritzalis, S., Marinagi, C., Skourlas, C., Vassis, D.: Secure wireless infrastructures and mobile learning for deaf and hard-of-hearing students. In: Informatics (PCI), 2012 16th Panhellenic Conference on, pp. 369–374. IEEE (2012)

  4. Bersch, R.: Assistiva - tecnologia e educação. http://www.assistiva.com.br/tassistiva.html (2017). Accessed 11 Dec, 2017

  5. Brito, F.B.D., Prieto, R.G.: we did it ourselves: the deaf social movement and the quest for the legal recognition of the libras sign language in Brazil. Disabil. Stud. Q. (2018). https://doi.org/10.18061/dsq.v38i4.6241

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Broderick, A.: Equality of what? the capability approach and the right to education for persons with disabilities. Social Incl. 6, 29–39 (2018)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Constantinou, V., Ioannou, A., Klironomos, I., Antona, M., Stephanidis, C.: Technology support for the inclusion of deaf students in mainstream schools: a summary of research from 2007 to 2017. Univ. Access Inf. Soc. 19(1), 195–200 (2020)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Dawkins, R.: From the perspective of the object in semiotics: Deleuze and peirce. Semiotica 2020(233), 1–18 (2020)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Drigas, A., Kouremenos, D., Vrettaros, J.: Teaching of english to hearing impaired individuals whose mother language is the sign language. In: World Summit on Knowledge Society, pp. 263–270. Springer (2008)

  10. Eco, U.: Semiotics and the Philosophy of Language. Indiana University Press, Bloomington (1986)

    Google Scholar 

  11. Edmondson, S., Howe, J.: Exploring the social inclusion of deaf young people in mainstream schools, using their lived experience. Educ. Psychol. Pract. 35(2), 216–228 (2019)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Erton, İ: The essence of semiotics as a mediator of communication and cognition. Int. Online J. Edu. Teach. 5(2), 267–277 (2018)

    Google Scholar 

  13. Espinheira, P.L., de Oliveira Silva, A.: Residual and influence analysis to a general class of simplex regression. TEST 29(2), 523–552 (2020)

    Article  MathSciNet  MATH  Google Scholar 

  14. Florencio Fernandes Moret, M.C., Rossarolla, J.N., Rodrigues Mendonca, J.G.: Bilingual proposal in deaf education: educational practices in literacy process. Revista Ibero-americana De Estudos Em Educacao pp. 1792–1801 (2017)

  15. Ghanouni, P., Jarus, T., Zwicker, J.G., Lucyshyn, J., Chauhan, S., Moir, C.: Perceived barriers and existing challenges in participation of children with autism spectrum disorders:“he did not understand and no one else seemed to understand him’’. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 49(8), 3136–3145 (2019)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Guimarães, C., Antunes, D.R., García, L.S., Peres, L.M., Fernandes, S.: Pedagogical architecture–internet artifacts for bilingualism of the deaf (sign language/portuguese). In: System Sciences (HICSS), 2013 46th Hawaii international conference on, pp. 40–49. IEEE (2013)

  17. Haug, P.: Understanding inclusive education: ideals and reality. Scand. J. Disabil. Res. 19(3), 206–217 (2017)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Mehrabian, A.: Web-based distance learning system for opportunities for deaf students. Age 12, 1 (2007)

    Google Scholar 

  19. Melas, V., Salnikov, D.: On asymptotic power of the new test for equality of two distributions. In: International conference on stochastic methods, pp. 204–214. Springer (2020)

  20. Mich, O., Pianta, E., Mana, N.: Interactive stories and exercises with dynamic feedback for improving reading comprehension skills in deaf children. Comput. Educ. 65, 34–44 (2013)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Peirce, C.S.: Semiótica, 4 edn. Perspectiva (2012). Translated from Teixeira Coelho, 1st reprint

  22. de Quadros, R.M., Stumpf, M.R.: 16. recognizing brazilian sign language: legislation and outcomes. In: The Legal Recognition of Sign Languages, pp. 254–267. Multilingual Matters (2019)

  23. Rocha, D.F., Bittencourt, I.I., Dermeval, D., Isotani, S.: Uma revisão sistemática sobre a educação do surdo em ambientes virtuais educacionais. In: Brazilian Symposium on Computers in Education (Simpósio Brasileiro de Informática na Educação-SBIE), vol. 25, p. 1263 (2014)

  24. Sager, F., Sperb, T.M., Roazzi, A., Martins, F.M.: Avaliação da interação de crianças em pátios de escolas infantis: uma abordagem da psicologia ambiental. Psicologia: reflexão e crítica. Porto Alegre. 16(1), 203–215 (2003)

  25. Santaella, L.: O que é semiótica. Brasiliense (2017)

  26. Silva, O.G., Nogueira, A.F.d.S.: Comparing grammatical aspects of portuguese and libras. In: Colloquium of Letters FALE/CUMB, Federal University of Pará. Federal University of Pará (2014)

  27. da Silva, S.G.d.L., do Nascimento, S.P.d.F.: Reading strategies in portuguese teaching as a second language for deaf students. In: Brazilian Sign Language Studies, pp. 305–320. De Gruyter Mouton (2020)

  28. Snoddon, K.: Sign language planning and policy in ontario teacher education. Lang. Policy 20(4), 577–598 (2021)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Wauters, L.N., Knoors, H.: Social integration of deaf children in inclusive settings. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 13(1), 21–36 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Weaver, K.A., Starner, T., Hamilton, H.: An evaluation of video intelligibility for novice american sign language learners on a mobile device. In: Proceedings of the 12th international ACM SIGACCESS conference on Computers and accessibility, pp. 107–114. ACM (2010)

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the Center of Excellence in Social Technologies (NEES), our families for their support, and some specific members who contributed significantly to this work, such as Josmário Albuquerque, Jário José and Daniel Borges. The principal researcher of this work gives a special thanks to his fiancee Floriza Abreu, and devotes this work to his father Antonio Carlos R Rocha.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rafael de Amorim Silva.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix

Appendix

In this section, the analysis of the results obtained with the controlled experiment will be presented. For this purpose, a proper statistical tool was used to treat and to understand the data. The first method being was the Anderson–Darling Test (ad) [19], which tests whether the data comes from a normal population. Therefore: Anderson–Darling test (ad): \(F(x) = P(X \le x)\) being the cumulative distribution function of a normal population. G(x) being the empirical data distribution function (EDF), which can be defined as the cumulative distribution function of the relative frequencies. The test evaluates if \(G(x) \approx F(x)\). So, our hypotheses are:

  1. H0:

    data comes from a normal distribution

  2. H1:

    data does not come from a normal distribution

It is noticeable from Table 3 that more than half of the p-values are smaller than 5%, indicating that we rejected the normality hypothesis for these variables at a 5% level. In this sense, we can use non-parametric tests to compare if the variables of the sample without technology and sample with technology and teacher come from the same probability distribution. For this purpose, we will use the Wilcoxon test [19] for two populations. The test hypotheses are described below, where \(F_x\) represents the probability distribution of the random variable X and \(F_y\), represents the probability distribution of the random variable Y. The test is performed considering a level of significance of \(\alpha = 0.05 = 5\%\).

Table 3 Anderson darling normality test

Our main goal is to test the equivalence of the populations regarding the samples without technology and with technology and teacher, therefore we will have the hypothesis of the type:

  1. H0:

    \(F_\text {UseOfSign-WithoutTechnology} = F_\text {UseOfSign-WithTechnologyTeacher}\)

  2. H1:

    \(F_\text {UseOfSign-WithoutTechnology} \ne F_\text {UseOfSign-WithTechnologyTeacher}\)

Taking as basis Table 4, we noticed that only the Use of Signs samples—Without and With Technology (use of signs by the students) and the Use of Libras—Without and With Technology samples (use of Libras among students) are considered to come from different populations. The p-values are equal to 0.002364 and 0.001366, respectively, and smaller than 0.05.

We can conclude from these samples that the use of Libras signs and signs created among children before the use of the technology and after the use of technology and assistance from the teacher are statistically different at a 5% level. The same conclusion is true when using only Libras signs. We will perform some graphical analysis to validate the conclusions above.

Table 4 Wilcoxon test—group without technology and group with technology and teacher

In the current study, the involved variables are:

  • Common Communication between the children, using standard language or any type of language. Ex: notes; group games; gestures; etc;

  • Port Use of written or spoken Portuguese;

  • Libras Use of standard Libras signs;

  • PortLibras Use of Portuguese languages and Libras in the same time frame;

  • WithoutCod If there was communication without the use of a standard language or code;

  • Deaf If there was interaction with deaf students;

  • Hearing If there was interaction with hearing students;

  • DeafHearing If there was interaction with deaf and hearing students in the same time frame.

One of the objectives of the study is to check if the model and technology used to stimulate the use of Libras signs and the Signs inherent to the children. Since both are variables that present characteristics of the Poisson distribution, it is possible to think of a Generalized Linear Model, based on the Poisson distribution and with logarithmic connection function.

The methodology adopted in the data analysis considers generalized linear models (GLM) proposed by P. McCullagh and J. A. Nelder, Generalized Linear Models, Chapman and Hall, London, 1989. Furthermore, every regression model in fact explains the mean of the response variable, instead of the response variable itself, as presented as follows.

The classical regression model considers that

$$\begin{aligned} Y = X\beta + \epsilon \end{aligned}$$

where \(\epsilon \) is a random error that have to satisfy \(\mathrm{{E}}(\epsilon ) = 0\), that is, the mean of error must be zero. Here, \(\mathrm {E}(\cdot )\) represents the mathematical expectation of a random variable, or expected value, or population mean of the random variable. The consequence of this assumption is that

$$\begin{aligned} \mathrm {Y} = X\beta + \mathrm {E}(\epsilon ) \Rightarrow \mu = X\beta \end{aligned}$$

in which \(\mu \) is the mean of response variable. In time series the same occurs, however the practitioners do not know about this mathematical particularity. Our regression model considers that the response has gamma distribution instead of the normal distribution, and for this reason the model becomes

$$\begin{aligned} g(\mu ) = X\beta \end{aligned}$$

since \(\mu \in (0,\infty )\) and the function \(g(\mu ) = \log (\mu )\) is appropriated to become the regression similar to normal models in which \(\mu \in (-\infty ,\infty )\).

The first model to be tested will try to explain the behavior of the random variable Libras by considering the other study variables as covariates (therefore, they are not considered random variables) and, additionally, the two associated groups, namely: Without technology and With technology and teacher. With this goal, we will create a new covariate, which will have values equal to zero for the group Without technology and values equal to one for the group With Technology and Teacher. This variable is known as the Dummy Variable. Here we will represent it by “ID”. After testing some models, we found out that it was more appropriate to explain the average of Libras with:

$$\begin{aligned} \mathrm{log}\mu _{i} = \beta _{1} + \beta _{2}ID_{i} + \beta _{3}\mathrm{Sign}_{i} + \beta _{4}\mathrm{Deaf}_{i}, i = 1, ..., 30 \end{aligned}$$

Note that the data we have are the covariate values: ID, Sign and Deaf and the values of the response variable, for each one of the thirty observations in the sample. The estimation process of the coefficients of the model considers exactly the values of the covariates and the values of y to obtain the estimates of the \(\beta \)’s and consequently the estimates of \(\mu _{1}\), \(\mu _{2}\), \(\mu _{3}\), \(\mu _{4}\), \(\mu _{5}\), \(\mu _{6}\) ... \(\mu _{30}\), where each \(\mu \) represents a different moment of observation.

The method used to obtain an estimate of \(\beta \)’s is the model of maximum likelihood and to test the significance of the \(\beta \)’s the quasi-t tests are used, being asymptotic tests (approximately).

In Table 5 we present the estimates of the \(\beta \)’s if the p-Values associated with the quasi-t tests.

Table 5 Estimates of the \(\beta \)’s if the p Values associated with the Quasi-t tests

Using Table 5 as basis we notice the rejection of all the hypotheses

$$\begin{aligned} H0:\beta _{1} = 0;\, H0:\beta _{2} = 0;\, H0:\beta _{3} = 0;\, H0:\beta _{4} = 0 \end{aligned}$$

Therefore, all the covariates are considered important to build a group model that does not use technology in the same way as the group that used the proposed technology associated with the help of the teacher. This final model is defined as:

$$\begin{aligned} \hat{\mu }= \mathrm{exp}\{-2.99 + 1.08 ID_{i} + 0.68 \mathrm{Sinal}_{i} + 0.10 \mathrm{Deaf}_{i}\} \end{aligned}$$

Another statistical hypothesis that we still must test is: Is the estimated model adjusted to the data of good quality? We can do this based on residue analysis to check if a given model is an adequate representation of the data. The construction of a regression model involves the definition of the distribution of the response variable, choice of the binding function, and choice of covariates. Typically, residues are based on the differences between observed responses (y) and the estimated average \(\hat{\mu }\). For instance, \(r_{i} = y_{i} - \hat{\mu }_{i}\) with the residual being a measure of discrepancy between the actual data and the adjusted model. Here we will use the residual component of the deviation, the one used with the highest frequency for generalized linear models.

An important residual graphic is the one of normal probability with simulated envelope, which can be used even when the empirical distributions of the residuals are not normal ([13]). If the model is suitable for the data, we expect that most of the residues are randomly distributed within the envelope bands. Based on Fig. 17a, we see that this occurs with the proposed use of the Libras model, confirming the quality of the adjusted model.

Given that another important variable in the study is the quantity of signs, it seems plausible to fit a regression model with the purpose of explaining the number of signs performed by the students while providing a contrast between the use of signs without the intervention of technology and the use of signs with the intervention of technology. The estimated model for quantity of signs was \( \hat{\mu }= \mathrm{exp}\{-2.54 + 2.15ID_{i} - 0.10\mathrm{Common}_{i}\} \). From Fig. 17b, we find that the model is well fitted to the data since the residues are mostly inside the envelope and randomly distributed around zero.

Fig. 17
figure 17

Graphic of residuals

We should emphasize the effect of the covariates on the average number of communication made among the students using Libras, and this effect is measured based on the estimates of \(\beta \)’s. If the estimate of a \(\beta \) associated with the given covariate is negative, the estimated value of \(\beta \) will be as larger as the reduction in the average amount of Libras. On the other hand, if the estimated \(\beta \) is positive, it is important to increase the student’s performance the higher this estimate became to be. The opposite happens when the estimate of \(\beta \) is positive. For instance, \(\hat{\beta }_{2} = 1.08\), with this indicating that the increase of one unit in the indicator covariate implies on increase of \((\mathrm{exp}(1.08) -1) \times 100\% = 195\%\) on the average amount of Libras usage. But this is a covariant indicator, which only assumes the value equal to zero (group without technology) and value equal to one (group without technology and teacher). Thus, adding a unit in this covariate implies going from zero to one, or going from the moment without technology to the moment with technology and teacher. If the child is part of the group using technology and teacher, the use of Libras increases by approximately 195%. If the child uses several signs other than Libras for each additional sign used, the average use of Libras increases by 101% and the communication between deaf and hearing students increases in 12% the use of Libras.

The model \( \hat{\mu }_{i} = \mathrm{exp}(-2.99 + 1.08ID_{i} + 0.68\mathrm{Sign}_{i} \)\( + 0.10\mathrm{Deaf}_{i}) \) also allows to estimate the quantity of Libras signs used for the i-student. For example, let’s consider the student \(i = 23\). The covariates values for this student are \(x232 = 1.0\), since this is a student who has experienced method interference. \(x233 = 4.0\) and \(x234 = 4.0\), we have \(\hat{\mu }_{23} = 3.3 \approx 3.0 \), with the true value of the response being \(y23 = 3.0\) This is a good example of the model being set correctly.

The model interpretation for the quantity of signs, \( \hat{\mu }_{i} = \mathrm{exp}(-2.54 + 2.15ID_{i} + 0.10\mathrm{Port}_{i} + 0.10\mathrm{Common}_{i}) \) is interesting. Notice that \(\hat{\beta }_{2} = 2.15\), with this implying that the student experimenting interference of the associated technology with the teacher increases in \((\mathrm{exp}(2.15)-1) \times 100\% = 758\%\) the average quantity of signs used by these students. On the other hand, in a logical way, the communication made through the Portuguese language decreases in \((1.0 - \mathrm{exp}(-0.10)) \times 100\% = 9.5\%\) the use of signs.

We conclude that both through the applied tests and through the regression model generated to verify possible inconsistencies, the process of communication between hearing and deaf children improved quantitatively, with increased use of signs, and qualitative, because with the use of these signs, the children had a larger common vocabulary, making the communicative process more semantic. We also conclude that the generated regression model is adjusted to reality, so that the experiment can be scalable, or easily reproduced in other schools that have similar conditions, obtaining similar results.

1.1 Qualitative analysis

This section describes the qualitative analysis of the performed experiment. As previously mentioned, our experiment lasted for five days, with sections of thirty minutes per day recorded, and aimed to observe how communication between deaf children and hearings would occur before and after the technological intervention.

Based on these observations, we tried to understand whether there was some kind of communication or interaction pattern between deaf and hearing children, and how these patterns would have occurred. In the first two days, we have wanted the children to get used to the environment since they were isolated from the other children in the school. From the third day, we have observed the behavior of the children to identify whether there was any pattern of communication between them. We summarize the most significant moments in our experiment by observing the possible patterns of communication among the target children. We group these moments into 3 categories of communication to show to the reader the most significant points according to the opinion of the researcher: (i) gesture communication; (ii) common vocabulary; and (iii) Signed Formal Communication.

We have observed that moments of communication between the hearing children and their deaf colleagues had a significant semantic gain, because the communication that happened before through notes and gestures created by the children, now happens with the addition of signs of Libras thus making the possible moments of conversation extending even further. We also noticed that there was a greater engagement of the deaf student in class time, especially in relation to her colleagues. A priori, when we idealized this experiment, we imagined that the deaf student would be isolated at the beginning, and with the gradual learning of the signs language made by her and her colleagues, the number of interactions would increase. However, we were wrong about this hypothesis because from the very beginning the deaf student communicated with her colleagues without any difficulties. However, we realized that the moments of communication were limited due to the lack of vocabulary. We realized then that the improvement provided by the technology and help of the teacher was related to the quality of the moments of communication. With the increase in the repertoire of the Libras signs, the children had a greater understanding of the situation in which they were involved. With this, we feel that the objectives proposed in this research were achieved.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Rocha, D.F.S., Bittencourt, I.I., de Amorim Silva, R. et al. An assistive technology based on Peirce’s semiotics for the inclusive education of deaf and hearing children. Univ Access Inf Soc 22, 1097–1116 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10209-022-00919-2

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10209-022-00919-2

Keywords

Navigation