Abstract
Different movement patterns have evolved as a response to predictable and unpredictable variation in the environment with migration being an adaptation to predictable environments, nomadism to unpredictable environments and partial migration to a mixture of predictable and unpredictable conditions. Along different movement patterns, different cognitive abilities have evolved which are reviewed and discussed in relation to an organism’s ability to respond to largely unpredictable environmental change due to climate and human-induced change, and linked to population trends. In brief, migrants have a combination of reliance on memory, low propensity to explore and high avoidance of environmental change that in combination with overall small brain sizes results in low flexibility to respond to unpredictable environmental change. In line with this, many migrants have negative population trends. In contrast, while nomads may use their memory to find suitable habitats, they can counteract negative effects of finding such habitats disturbed by large-scale exploratory movements and paying attention to environmental cues. They are also little avoidant of environmental change. Population trends are largely stable or increasing indicating their ability to cope with climate and human-induced change. Cognitive abilities in partial migrants are little investigated, but indicate attention to environmental cues coupled with high exploratory tendencies that allow them a flexible response to unpredictable environmental change. Indeed, their population trends are mainly stable or increasing. In conclusion, cognitive abilities have evolved in conjunction with different movement patterns and affect an organism’s ability to adapt to rapidly human-induced changes in the environment.
Similar content being viewed by others
Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.
Introduction
Many bird species undertake large-scale movements to escape unfavourable conditions and/or utilise high-quality resources elsewhere. These movements can take the form of migration (to and fro migrations between breeding and wintering sites), partial migration when not all individuals in a population migrate and nomadism with birds following high-quality and abundant resources (Dean 2004). As a whole, large-scale movements are a global widespread phenomenon, and about 20 % of all bird species are migratory (Somveille et al. 2015). Three major Holarctic migration systems can be distinguished spanning all continents, the Nearctic–Neotropical system, the Palearctic–African system and the Palearctic–Asian system (Rappole and Jones 2003). Likewise, partial migration occurs worldwide (Jahn et al. 2012), but is particularly common in Australia with about 36 % of its bird species being partial migrants (Chan 2001). Nomadism is mainly linked to semi-arid and arid environments worldwide (Dean 2004) with nomadism accounting for about 10 % of all bird species in southern Africa (Dean 1997) and 26 % of all bird species in Australia (Smith 2015).
Climate change and human-induced activities result in rapidly changing environments. These changes are often unpredictable with extreme interannual variation in, for example, temperatures (e.g. Jalili et al. 2010). How bird species involved in large-scale movement fare in these ever-changing environments is of conservation importance. While many factors contribute to population decline including habitat loss, habitat change, fragmentation, pollution and others, a large part of a species’ reaction to environmental change is due to its ability to counteract these effects, that is, its ability to utilise new or different resources, adapt to other habitats or overcome gaps in its distribution. In this context, the paper will explore whether cognitive abilities play an important role in mitigating environmental change through flexible behavioural responses.
This article will review cognitive abilities of birds adopting different movement strategies and discuss them in relation to population developments and a species’ cognitive preparedness for an ever-changing world. I will first outline under which environmental conditions the different movement strategies evolved and then discuss cognitive adaptations and their consequences.
Adaptation to variation in the environment
Migration has evolved as an adaptation to predictable seasonal environments (Dean 2004; Mettke-Hofmann 2014; Somveille et al. 2015) and often has an endogenous component determining the onset, direction and duration of migration (Gwinner 1986). Many migrants show morphological (e.g. longer wings), physiological (e.g. migratory fattening) and behavioural (e.g. nocturnal restlessness) adaptations to migration (Gwinner 1986; Leisler and Winkler 2003).
Partial migration describes populations where some individuals migrate and others do not (Lundberg 2013), and is an adaptation to seasonal environments with neither particularly harsh nor particularly benign winter conditions such as found at intermediate latitudes but with high variability (i.e. less predictability) in winter survival (Lundberg 1988). Whether an individual migrates can be genetically fixed or environmentally dependent and vary between years (Lundberg 1987). The proportion of birds migrating is often affected by environmental factors such as population density and resource availability with larger numbers migrating in years of high density and low resource availability (Nilsson et al. 2008).
Nomadism, in contrast, has evolved as an adaptation to unpredictable environmental conditions (Dean 1997). Nomads track superabundant but spatiotemporal unpredictable resources (Runge et al. 2015) and can often breed throughout the year whenever conditions are favourable (Jonzen et al. 2011).
Human-induced changes are from an animal’s perspective largely unpredictable in space and time, and climate change also results in increased uncertainty and variability in the environment (extreme weathers occur more often; Cormont et al. 2011). I, therefore, predict that nomadic species may be better equipped to withstand human-altered habitats as they are adapted to track unpredictable resources. Partial migrants also respond to environmental cues and may be able to buffer against environmental change, whereas migrants, particularly long-distance migrants, have evolved to cope with highly predictable (seasonal) changes and may be least adapted to unpredictable change.
Cognitive abilities of birds with different movement strategies
This section will review what is known about cognitive adaptations to different movement strategies.
Migratory species
Many migrants return to their breeding ground and often even the same territory year after year (e.g. Blums et al. 2002; Olalla-Kerstrupp et al. 2015). This requires a long-lasting memory for at least the period covering the non-breeding season. Indeed, long-distance migratory garden warblers (Sylvia borin) have been found to remember a room with food for at least 12 months, whereas closely related but resident Sardinian warblers (Sylvia melanocephala) remembered the same room for only 2 weeks (Mettke-Hofmann and Gwinner 2003). This long-lasting memory in migrants may even allow remembering high-quality stopover or wintering sites until the next year in accordance with winter site fidelity shown in many species (Rappole and Jones 2003; Paruk et al. 2015). Similarly, in dark-eyed juncos (Junco hyemalis) and white-crowned sparrows (Zonotrichia leucophrys), individuals of a migratory subspecies had a better spatial memory for previously visited food locations than individuals of a resident subspecies (Cristol et al. 2003; Pravosudov et al. 2006). The better spatial memory in the migrants was reflected in the hippocampal formation which is an important brain region for processing spatial information (Healy et al. 1994). The migrants among the dark-eyed juncos had more densely packed neurons in this region, and the migrants among the white-crowned sparrow had a larger hippocampal formation and also showed increased neurogenesis in this region than their resident counterparts (Cristol et al. 2003; Pravosudov et al. 2006; LaDage et al. 2011). Likewise, in the migratory garden warblers, relative hippocampal volume increased in migratory-experienced individuals as compared to migratory-naïve ones which indicates that the birds collected spatial information while on migration (Healy et al. 1996). The same study showed that hippocampal volume did not change in the resident Sardinian warbler. A recent study on two sandpiper species with different demands on visuospatial learning during migration also revealed a larger hippocampus and a larger number of microglia cells therein in the species with more demand on remembering visual cues during overland migration (Actitis macularia) as compared to Calidris pusilla with more non-stop flight over the Atlantic Ocean (Diniz et al. 2016). Overall, migrants seem to have a better spatial memory as an adaptation to their to and fro migration than residents.
While migrants have a larger hippocampus than residents, their overall brain size is smaller than in residents. Sol et al. (2010) found among 600 passerines that relative brain size decreased with migratory distance. Moreover, analyses indicated that migration selected for smaller brains, that is, migratoriness evolved from large-brained species, and then, selection favoured smaller brains (Sol et al. 2010). Similar results were found within species; in lark sparrows (Chondestes grammacus), migratory populations have smaller brains than resident populations (Fuchs et al. 2015). The reduced brain size may be an adaptation to reduce energy consumption (Winkler et al. 2004) as brains consume relatively more energy than other body parts (Laughlin et al. 1998). Alternatively, or in addition to this, the smaller brains are often linked to a flatter skull in migrants which may make the head more aerodynamic (Winkler et al. 2004).
The smaller brain size in migrants was also linked to less innovative behaviour in this group, particularly in long-distance migrants, as compared to short-distance migrants and residents (Sol et al. 2005). This indicates lower flexibility in behaviour in migrants as suggested by Sol (2003) and Winkler et al. (2004). Migrants are also less explorative than residents. Migratory garden warblers responded less to changes in their environment by taking longer to approach and investigate a novel object in their familiar environment than resident Sardinian warblers (Mettke-Hofmann et al. 2005a). They were, however, more likely to enter an unfamiliar environment, but once they were in this environment, they spent less time exploring it than the Sardinian warblers (Mettke-Hofmann et al. 2009). Moreover, the migrants covered more space per minute (Mettke-Hofmann and Gwinner 2004). These findings indicate that migrants are less hesitant to enter a novel environment which may be a prerequisite when encountering unfamiliar areas on migration, but that they do not invest a lot of time in exploring an unfamiliar environment. The exploration patterns reflect more superficial spatial exploration to get a rough overview about resources. These results are backed up by field studies showing that migrants move only over short distances in a straight line (i.e. covering unfamiliar territory rather than meandering around) and invest little in exploratory movements at stopover sites (Aborn and Moore 1997; Paxton et al. 2008). Migrants remain at each site for only relatively short periods of time (days to months depending whether it is a breeding, stopover or wintering site) and have to consider costs and benefits of exploration. While they have to collect information about suitable foraging sites and predation risk, they may keep this to a minimum as they cannot use this information in the long term (Mettke-Hofmann and Gwinner 2004; Mettke-Hofmann et al. 2012).
Several studies found that migrants use social information to get information about habitats quickly which may make up for the lower individual exploration. For example, migrants initially join flocks at stopover sites before foraging on their own (in insectivorous birds) which may serve information gathering by reducing uncertainty and risks associated with lack of information (Nemeth and Moore 2007). Likewise, some migrants use resident species as an indicator for high-quality breeding habitats and settlement decisions (heterospecific attraction hypothesis; Moenkkoenen et al. 1997). Moenkkoenen et al. (1999) suggested that this requires high cognitive abilities, for example, to recognise suitable species which has been shown in some migrants (Moenkkoenen et al. 1996).
While migrants show little spatial neophobia, that is, hesitancy to enter unfamiliar environments (Mettke-Hofmann et al. 2009), they are highly avoidant of changes in their familiar environment. A study comparing eight species/populations of sympatrically occurring New World blackbirds (Icteridae) during the non-breeding season showed strong avoidance reactions to feeding sites with novel objects placed around in migratory birds as compared to resident birds (Mettke-Hofmann et al. 2013). The same strong neophobia reaction was found in migratory garden warblers in comparison with resident Sardinian warblers (Mettke-Hofmann et al. 2005a). As migrants spend only limited time in each area, they may be more cautious about any changes as they do not know the risks/dangers associated with change, whereas residents may have a better knowledge whether, for example, a human-induced change is dangerous such as machinery placed in a field for future use.
Recent research has shown that individuals of a given species often differ consistently from each other in their response to environmental challenges (termed personality; for example, Koolhaas et al. 1999) which on the population level increases flexibility. Marchetti and Zehtindjiev (2009) suggested that in long-distance migratory sedge warblers (Acrocephalus schoenobaenus), personality can explain differences in behaviour during migration along a fear axis. While they did not test for within context repeatability, they found correlations across contexts that could best be explained with personality rather than other factors. Birds were captured during autumn migration and tested for their time to start foraging, migratory orientation and exploration of an unfamiliar room. Calmer birds (less escape movements) explored the cage more and were faster to accept food than birds moving more. Also, lean birds explored more. They suggested that the calm birds represent a reactive coping style with being more flexible and putting on less fat as they readily explore new environments. More nervous birds (more escape movements) represent a more proactive coping style with little flexibility but more reliance on fat reserves. Moreover, it has been shown in several bird species that individuals of the same species consistently differ in their timing of migration (Vardanis et al. 2016); that is, some individuals always migrate early, whereas others late. On the population level, this again provides flexibility to respond to environmental change. Interestingly, in black kites (Milvus migrans), timing of migration only became highly repeatable in adults, whereas juveniles progressively departed earlier each year (Sergio et al. 2014). Moreover, only early migrating individuals in their respective age class that were also able to advance their departure date in the first years survived and reproduced best, whereas individuals not able to advance their departure finally disappeared from the population. Unfortunately, it is unclear which factors (learning ability, body conditions or others) allowed earlier departures.
Partially migratory species
Much less is known about cognitive abilities in partial migrants, but what is known contrasts in part with results just presented for obligate migrants. In partial migrants, not all individuals in a population migrate and whether to migrate individuals decided each year anew (Lundberg 2013). Partial migration should not be confused with species consisting of resident and migratory populations that have separate distributions where all individuals in each population express the same movement strategy. A study on partially migratory blue tits (Cyanistes caeruleus) showed that migrating individuals were more explorative of a novel object in their familiar environment than individuals that remained resident (Nilsson et al. 2010). Partial migrants respond much more strongly to environmental cues and may sample their environment for winter settlement suitability while on migration which may explain their higher exploration (Nilsson et al. 2010). There is also some evidence for a migratory versus resident personality in blue tits; individuals with a strong migratory propensity may initiate migration earlier and also start foraging in an unfamiliar environment earlier indicating that they settle in faster than individuals with a more resident personality, though no differences were found regarding neophobia (Nilsson et al. 2016). No other studies on personality are available for partially migratory birds; however, there are several studies in fish that indicate differences in personality between resident and migratory individuals. Migratory individuals in partially migratory fish were bolder (emergence from a hideout) and took more risk in an unfamiliar environment than resident individuals (Chapman et al. 2011). This again indicates that the migratory individuals among partial migrants are more open to novelty and invest in sampling unfamiliar environments than the resident individuals of the population. Nothing is known about other cognitive abilities in partially migratory birds. However, one may hypothesise that the migratory individuals have a better spatial memory than the resident individuals in the population to remember suitable wintering sites to the next season.
Nomadic species
Finally, nomadic species show cognitive adaptations to their lifestyle. Movement patterns of some nomadic species indicate long-term memories for previously visited locations. For example, when current conditions deteriorated, grey teals (Anas gracilis) did not move to the next available wetland, but sometimes passed suitable sites to visit a remote wetland they may have visited earlier (Roshier et al. 2006). Likewise, snail kites (Rostrhamus sociabilis) are assumed to have a long-lasting memory for wetlands (Bennetts and Kitchens 2000). In contrast, memory was not considered to be important in two seedeater species (Sporophila) as seeding cycles of bamboo plants (the main food of the species) were longer than the average lifespan of the birds. While these plants have predictable cycle intervals, plants are not synchronised making the spatiotemporal occurrence of seeding unpredictable and with the very long cycle intervals, often around 20 years, a bird’s life is too short to learn about the cycle length of particular plants (Areta et al. 2013).
Snail kites were shown to have higher movement patterns during good food conditions which were interpreted as exploration movements and may serve to increase knowledge about the environment as lakes dry out every 5–10 years (Bennetts and Kitchens 2000). Conducting such flights during good periods reduces the costs of exploration. In contrast, on a local scale (exploration of a neighbouring aviary), nomadic parrot species showed less spatial exploration and also explored changes in their familiar environment less than closely related resident species (Mettke-Hofmann et al. 2005b, 2012). Like in migratory species, short residency times may make extensive local exploration and changes therein too costly. However, nomadic species seem to have similarly sized brains as residents. Based on data of movement patterns in parrots (Mettke-Hofmann et al. 2002), brain sizes (Iwaniuk et al. 2005) were compared between pairs of closely related species (one a nomad, the other a resident; when more than one species of a particular movement type was available within a group, the mean was used) resulting in nine pairings. Results indicate that residents and nomads have similar brain sizes (paired t test: n = 9, df = 8, t = 0.927, p = 0.381; Table 1). This may give nomadic species the necessary flexibility to respond to unpredictable environmental change.
Unlike migrants, nomadic species seem to be less avoidant of changes in their familiar environment. Data on neophobic reactions of over fifty parrot species (Mettke-Hofmann et al. 2002) were reanalysed by planned comparisons of closely related species by excluding phylogenetic groups consisting of purely resident or nomadic species. Additionally, a paired comparison (one a nomad, the other a resident; see above) was conducted which resulted in 11 pairings. This way, taxon-specific effects in neophobia reactions were avoided though phylogeny did not play a role in the full data set (Mettke-Hofmann et al. 2002). While overall residents and nomads did not differ systematically in their neophobic reaction (latency to forage beside a novel object; t test: n = 29, df = 27, t = 1.213, p = 0.236), in all pairings, nomads were less neophobic than closely related resident parrot species (paired t test: n = 11, df = 10, t = −3.132, p = 0.011). This is an interesting finding as it indicates that nomads are not afraid of changes in their environment which is in strong contrast with neophobic reactions in migrants.
Nomadic species seem to pay attention to a variety of environmental cues to decide about where to go when conditions deteriorate, whereas migratory species generally follow endogenous programmes (Gwinner 1986) though partial migrants also use local cues to decide about when to migrate (Nilsson et al. 2006). At least for nomadic wetland birds, it has been suggested that they use visual cues such a cloud formation, or changes in temperature and pressure gradients that predict rainfall (Simmons et al. 1998) for decision-making whether and where to move. Roshier et al. (2006) suggested that primary productivity of algae and other micro-organisms would lead to a distinctive olfactory signal which could be picked up by nomadic species over large distances. There are no personality studies available in relation to movements in nomads.
Cognitive adaptations in a modern world
This section will discuss how birds with cognitive adaptations to different migration strategies fare in our rapidly changing environment where changes are often unpredictable.
Migrants
Migrants have evolved cognitive adaptations to deal with predictable variation in the environment (Fig. 1). This includes a long-lasting memory for high-quality stopover sites, last year breeding territories and former wintering sites. While this is advantageous in a predictable environment, it can be a disadvantage when environments become less predictable due to climate change or human activity (Cormont et al. 2011). For example, stopover and breeding sites may have disappeared, shifted or changed and site fidelity can be, in the best case, a waste of time (as birds return to a site that is less suitable now) or, in the worst case, mean death when habitats may have become unsuitable (Cresswell 2014). The lower exploration propensity in combination with their smaller overall brains also makes migrants less likely to find suitable habitats or adapt to changed habitats. Indeed, migrants largely settle along their migration pathway and underuse suitable habitat further away from this path (Telleria et al. 2008). However, some plasticity has been reported in pink-footed geese (Anser brachyrhynchus) where birds moved on to nearby staging sites after arriving at a former autumn and winter site that had disappeared due to restoration of a lake (Clausen and Madsen 2016). Nonetheless, migrants as a group were found to have the lowest invasion success when introduced to New Zealand (Sol and Lefebvre 2000). Their strong neophobia to changes in the environment keeps them away from possible resources and may restrict them to more pristine areas. While all these cognitive abilities make it difficult for a migrant to respond to environmental change, their ability to use other species as a cue for good habitat conditions (Moenkkoenen et al. 1997; Nemeth and Moore 2007) may buffer these disadvantages to some extent. Moreover, at least some migrants vary in behaviour along personality axes with some individuals being more explorative (Marchetti and Zehtindjiev 2009) which may allow for rapid selection. However, this does not seem enough to counter negative population trends.
Among the European breeding birds (n = 350), population trends of long-distance migrants were significantly more negative than of short-distance migrants and residents, irrespective of breeding or wintering habitat or continent (Sanderson et al. 2006). Similarly, a study by Lloyd-Evans and Atwood (2004) on 78 migratory species in America showed a negative population trend in the majority of the migratory species. Moreover, Holmes and Sherry (2001) showed that particularly long-distance migrants in America (n = 24 species) are declining. Long-distance migrants may be at a particular disadvantage as cues used to initiate spring migration may be misaligned with conditions on the breeding ground as a consequence of climate change (van Turnhout et al. 2010; Cormont et al. 2011), and migration may be overall more endogenously controlled than in short-distance migrants that have the ability to respond to environmental cues more flexibly (Sol et al. 2005). The same pattern of the highest proportion of declining populations in migrants as compared to all other groups was also found in the bird taxa listed in the Online Resources 1.
While migrants span a large range which may increase the risk of encountering habitat change and loss, and may affect population development of migrants more than residents, cognitively they seem to be poorly adapted to respond to those challenges. The combination of low exploration, reliance on memory rather than exploration and strong avoidance reactions to changes makes this group particularly vulnerable to unpredictable environmental change as they are behaviourally less flexible and may have a low propensity of bold individuals.
Partial migrants
The little that is known about cognitive abilities in partial migrants seems to prepare them for unpredictable environments (Fig. 1) which contrasts strongly with what is known from obligate migrants. The high exploration propensity and attention paid to environmental conditions in both, resident and migratory individuals among partial migrants, makes them well adapted to unpredictable environmental change as they can react flexibly to current situations (Chan 2001). Actually, partial migration is assumed to have evolved as a response to though seasonal but unpredictable variation in, for example, population density and food availability across years (Lundberg 1988). Moreover, models predict partial migration to evolve under strong environmental stochasticity (Velez-Espino et al. 2013). The authors conclude that partial migration can serve as a buffer against environmental stochasticity. This seems to be supported by population developments in partial migrants as they remain largely stable or even increase (Online Resource 1) indicating that they cope relatively well with human-altered habitats. An example is partially migratory blue tit populations in Sweden which have increased over the last 30 years (Nilsson et al. 2006). Interestingly, the proportion of migratory blue tits has also increased in contrast to predictions that global warming should reduce the migratory proportion (Berthold 2003) indicating density-dependence in this system (Velez-Espino et al. 2013). Moreover, their ability to cope with unfamiliar environments is further supported by their high invasion success (Sol and Lefebvre 2000). The possible existence of migratory and resident personality types may help during this process as different coping styles are suggested to be important in mastering different stages of invasion (Chapple et al. 2012). At least among partially migratory blue tits, some of the migratory individuals (17 %) do not return to their breeding ground (Nilsson et al. 2008). Their high exploration propensity and ability to flexibly decide whether or not to migrate allow them settling in new areas. Overall, partial migrants seem to be cognitively much better prepared to respond to human-altered habitats than obligate migrants. This seems to be the case for both, resident and migratory individuals, among partially migratory populations which is in stark contrast to species consisting of resident and migratory populations that are either adapted to residency or migratoriness.
Nomads
Nomadic species have evolved cognitive abilities to track superabundant but highly unpredictable resources (Fig. 1). As such, they seem to be predisposed to fare well in human-altered habitats. But is this really the case? The long-term spatial memory may help nomads to decide where to go when local conditions deteriorate. However, like in the migrants they may head to an area which is not suitable any more (Cresswell 2014). Nonetheless, they may be better off than migrants as they respond strongly to environmental cues (even over large distances; for example, Roshier et al. 2006), show large-scale exploratory movements (Bennetts and Kitchens 2000), have similarly sized brains as residents and often move in groups (Dean 1997) which may increase their chances of finding suitable sites. While nomads are often diet and habitat specialists (Dean 1997) and, therefore, are less resilient to environmental change than residents (Runge et al. 2015), their ability to dynamically respond to variable environmental conditions by large-scale movements may counteract this specialisation. Indeed, nomadic Worthen’s sparrows (Spizella wortheni) which occur in semi-arid and arid areas in Mexico maintained high genetic diversity with nearly no differentiation between sites despite considerable habitat fragmentation due to their large-scale movements (Canales-Delgadillo et al. 2012). Simmons et al. (2004) speculate that nomadic species may counteract climate change by moving away from unsuitable habitats.
Nomads also seem to be little afraid of changes in their environment, even less than residents in a direct comparison which is in stark contrast to reactions in migrants. As a consequence, they may be less stressed by human-induced changes and may tolerate human activity (e.g. trumpeter hornbills (Bycanistes bucinator) move into agricultural landscapes during the non-breeding season; Lenz et al. 2015). Their low neophobia may help them to settle relatively easy in captive environments where nomadic species such as the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata), the cockatiel (Nymphicus hollandicus) or the budgerigar (Melopsittacus undulatus) are among the most successful and easy to keep bird species. Nomadic species as a group also seem to cope relatively well with environmental change. Dean (2004) stated that none of the nomadic species are classified as threatened or endangered which is confirmed in the selection in the Online Resource 1, though four species were classed as either near threatened or vulnerable. Moreover, their population development is largely stable or even increasing (Online Resource 1). Overall, the ability of nomads to respond to environmental cues, decide about movement decisions flexibly and collect large-scale information about the environment during extended movements seems to prepare them well for climate change-induced environmental challenges and human-induced changes such as habitat fragmentation.
Conclusion
Different movement patterns have evolved as a response to predictable and unpredictable variation in the environment accompanied by specific cognitive abilities to deal with the adversaries of a mobile life. It seems that it is not movement per se that disadvantage an organism in a rapidly changing environment, but it is the adaptation to specific environmental conditions (e.g. predictable variable environments) resulting in a particular combination of cognitive traits that may leave some groups behind. For instance, the combination of relying on memory in combination with little propensity to explore and high avoidance of changes in the familiar environment leaves migrants with only little room to respond to unpredictable environmental change. In contrast, disadvantages of spatial memory in nomads can be counteracted by flexibility where to go and attention paid to environmental stimuli. Their low neophobia also makes them less susceptible to environmental change. While species from all movements patterns (including residents) suffer from habitat loss, species with cognitive adaptations to unpredictable environmental conditions such as partial migrants and nomads seem to be able to respond more flexibly to climate and human-induced change (e.g. habitat fragmentation) than migrants that have adapted to predictable conditions, thus confirming the initial hypothesis. To buffer the negative impact of environmental change particularly on migrants, conservation efforts require maintaining suitable, non-disturbed (or only slightly disturbed) habitats on the breeding and overwintering ground and when on migration.
References
Aborn DA, Moore FR (1997) Pattern of movement by summer tanagers (Piringa rubra) during migratory stopover: a telemetry study. Behaviour 134:1077–1100
Areta JI, Bodrati A, Thom G, Rupp AE, Velazquez M, Holzmann I, Carrano E, Zimmermann CE (2013) Natural history, distribution, and conservation of two nomadic Sporophila seedeaters specializing on bamboo in the Atlantic forest. Condor 115:237–252
Bennetts RE, Kitchens WM (2000) Factors influencing movement probabilities of a nomadic food specialist: proximate foraging benefits or ultimate gains from exploration? Oikos 91:459–467
Berthold P (2003) Genetic basis and evolutionary aspects of bird migration. In: Slater PJB (ed) Advances in the study of behavior. Elsevier, London, pp 175–229
Blums P, Nichols JD, Hines JE, Mednis A (2002) Sources of variation in survival and breeding site fidelity in three species of European ducks. J Anim Ecol 71:438–450
Canales-Delgadillo JC, Scott-Morales L, Korb J (2012) The influence of habitat fragmentation on genetic diversity of a rare bird species that commonly faces environmental fluctuations. J Avian Biol 43:168–176
Chan K (2001) Partial migration in Australian landbirds: a review. Emu 101:281–292
Chapman BB, Hulthen K, Blomqvist DR, Hansson L-A, Nilsson J-A, Brodersen J, Nilsson PA, Skov C, Broenmark C (2011) To boldly go: individual differences in boldness influence migratory tendency. Ecol Lett 14:871–876
Chapple DG, Simmons SM, Wong BBM (2012) Can behavioral and personality traits influence the success of unintentional species introductions? TREE 27:57–64
Clausen KK, Madsen J (2016) Philopatry in a changing world: response of pink-footed geese Anser brachyrhynchus to the loss of a key autumn staging area due to restoration of Filso Lake, Denmark. J Orn 157:229–237
Cormont A, Vos CC, van Turnhout CAM, Foppen RPB, ter Braak CJF (2011) Using life-history traits to explain bird population responses to changing weather variability. Clim Res 49:59–71
Cresswell W (2014) Migratory connectivity of Palaearctic-African migratory birds and their responses to environmental change: the serial residency hypothesis. Ibis 156:493–510
Cristol DA, Reynolds EB, Leclerc JE, Donner AH, Farabaugh CS, Ziegenfus CWS (2003) Migratory dark-eyed juncos, Junco hyemalis, have better spatial memory and denser hippocampal neurons than nonmigratory conspecifics. Anim Behav 66:317–328
Dean WRJ (1997) The distribution and biology of nomadic birds in the Karoo, South Africa. J Biogeogr 24:769–779
Dean WRJ (2004) Nomadic desert birds. Springer, Berlin
Del Hoyo J, Elliott A, Sagatal J (1992–2002) Handbook of the birds of the world, vol 1–7. Lynx Edicions, Barcelona
Del Hoyo J, Elliott A, Christie D (2003–2011) Handbook of the birds of the world, vol 8–16. Lynx Edicions, Barcelona
Diniz CG, Magalhaes NGM, Sousa AA, Filho CS, Diniz DG, Lima CM, Oliviera MA, Paulo DC, Pereira PDC, Sherry DF, Picanco-Diniz CW (2016) Microglia and neurons in the hippocampus of migratory sandpipers. Braz J Med Biol Res 49:e5005. doi:10.1590/1414-431X20155005
Fuchs R, Bingman VP, Ross JD, Bernroider G (2015) Brain contrasts between migratory and nonmigratory North American lark sparrows (Chondestes grammacus). NeuroRep 26:1011–1016
Gwinner E (1986) Circannual rhythms in the control of avian migrations. Adv Study Behav 16:191–228
Healy SD, Clayton NS, Krebs JR (1994) Development of hippocampal specialisation in two species of tit (Parus spp.). Behav Brain Res 61:23–28
Healy SD, Gwinner E, Krebs JR (1996) Hippocampal volume in migratory and nonmigratory warblers: effects of age and experience. Behav Brain Res 81:61–68
Holmes RT, Sherry TW (2001) Thirty-year bird population trends in an unfragmented temperate deciduous forest: importance of habitat change. Auk 118:589–609
IUCN (2015) The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species 2015-4. Online: http://www.iucnredlist.org/. Last Accessed 14 Feb 2016
Iwaniuk A, Dean KM, Nelson JE (2005) Interspecific allometry of the brain and brain regions in parrots (Psittaciformes): comparisons with other birds and primates. Brain Behav Evol 65:40–59
Jahn AE, Bravo SP, Cueto VR, Levey DJ, Morales MV (2012) Patterns of partial avian migration in northern and southern temperate latitudes of the New World. Emu 112:17–22
Jalili A, Jamzad Z, Thompson K, Araghi MK, Ashrafi S, Hasaninejad M, Panahi P, Hooshang N, Azadi R, Tavakol MS et al (2010) Climate change, unpredictable cold waves and possible brakes on plant migration. Global Ecol Biogeogr 19:642–648
Jonzen N, Knudsen E, Holt RD, Saether B-E (2011) Uncertainty and predictability: the niches of migrants and nomads. In: Milner-Gulland EJ, Fryxell JM, Sinclair ARE (eds) Animal migrations: a synthesis. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 91–109
Koolhaas JM, Korte SM, De Boer SF, Van Der Vegt BJ, van Reenen CG, Hopster H, De Jong IC, Ruis MAW, Blokhuis HJ (1999) Coping styles in animals: current status in behavior and stress-physiology. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 23:925–935
LaDage LD, Roth TC II, Pravosudiv VV (2011) Hippocampal neurogenesis is associated with migratory behaviour in adult but not juvenile sparrows (Zonotrichia leucophrys ssp.). Proc R Soc B 278:138–143
Laughlin SB, de Ruyter van Steveninck RR, Anderson JC (1998) The metabolic cost of neural information. Nat Neurosci 1:36–41
Leisler B, Winkler H (2003) Morphological consequences of migration in passerines. In: Berthold P, Gwinner E, Sonnenschein E (eds) Avian migration. Springer, Berlin, pp 175–186
Lenz J, Boehning-Gaese K, Fiedler W, Mueller T (2015) Nomadism and seasonal range expansion in a large frugivorous bird. Ecograph 38:54–62
Lloyd-Evans TL, Atwood JL (2004) 32 years of changes in passerine numbers during spring and fall migrations in coastal Massachusetts. Wilson Bull 116:1–118
Lundberg P (1987) Partial bird migration and evolutionarily stable strategies. J Theor Biol 125:351–360
Lundberg P (1988) The evolution of partial migration in birds. TREE 3:172–175
Lundberg P (2013) On the evolutionary stability of partial migration. J Theor Biol 321:36–39
Marchetti C, Zehtindjiev P (2009) Migratory orientation of sedge warblers (Acrocephalus schoenobaenus) in relation to eating and exploratory behaviour. Behav Proc 82:293–300
Mettke-Hofmann C (2014) Cognitive ecology: ecological factors, life-styles and cognition. WIREs Cogn Sci 5:345–360. doi:10.1002/wcs.1289
Mettke-Hofmann C, Gwinner E (2003) Long-term memory for a life on the move. PNAS 100:5863–5866
Mettke-Hofmann C, Gwinner E (2004) Differential assessment of environmental information in a migratory and a non-migratory passerine. Anim Behav 68:1079–1086
Mettke-Hofmann C, Winkler H, Leisler B (2002) The significance of ecological factors for exploration and neophobia in parrots. Ethology 108:249–272
Mettke-Hofmann C, Ebert C, Schmidt T, Steiger S, Stieb S (2005a) Personality traits in resident and migratory warbler species. Behaviour 142:1357–1375
Mettke-Hofmann C, Wink M, Winkler H, Leisler B (2005b) Exploration of environmental changes relates to lifestyle. Behav Ecol 16:247–254
Mettke-Hofmann C, Lorentzen S, Schlicht E, Schneider J, Werner F (2009) Spatial neophilia and neophobia in resident and migratory warblers (Sylvia). Ethology 115:482–492
Mettke-Hofmann C, Wink M, Braun M, Winkler H (2012) Residency and a broad feeding spectrum are related to extensive spatial exploration in parrots. Behav Ecol 23:1365–1371
Mettke-Hofmann C, Winkler H, Hamel PB, Greenberg R (2013) Migratory New World blackbirds (Icterids) are more neophobic than closely related resident Icterids. PLoS ONE 8(2):e57565. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0057565
Moenkkoenen M, Forsman JT, Helle P (1996) Mixed-species foraging aggregations and heterospecific attraction in boreal bird communities. Oikos 77:127–136
Moenkkoenen M, Helle P, Niemi GJ, Montgomery K (1997) Heterospecific attraction affects community structure and migrant abundances in northern breeding bird communities. Can J Zool 75:2077–2083
Moenkkoenen M, Haerdling R, Forsman JT, Tuomi J (1999) Evolution of heterospecific attraction: using other species as cues in habitat selection. Evol Ecol 13:91–104
Nemeth Z, Moore FR (2007) Unfamiliar stopover sites and the value of social information during migration. J Orn 148(suppl 2):S369–S376
Nilsson ALK, Alerstam T, Nilsson J-A (2006) Do partial and regular migrants differ in their responses to weather? Auk 123:537–547
Nilsson ALK, Alerstam T, Nilsson J-A (2008) Diffuse, short and slow migration among Blue Tits. J Orn 149:365–373
Nilsson ALK, Nilsson J-A, Alerstam T, Baeckman J (2010) Migratory and resident blue tits Cyanistes caeruleus differ in their reaction to a novel object. Naturwissenschaften 97:981–985
Nilsson ALK, Nilsson J-A, Mettke-Hofmann C (2016) Energy reserves, information need and a pinch of personality determine decision-making on route in partially migratory blue tits. PLOS ONE (accepted)
Olalla-Kerstrupp A, Ruiz-Ayma G, Gonzales-Rojas JI, Guzman-Velasco A (2015) High fidelity to wintering, stop-over and breeding sites shown by a Long-billed Curlew Numenius americanus tracked with satellite telemetry on migratory flights across North America. Bird Study 62:556–560. doi:10.1080/00063657.2015.1073678
Paruk JD, Chickering MD, Long D IV, Uher-Koch H, East A, Poleschook D, Gumm V, Hanson W, Adams EM, Kovach KA, Evers DC (2015) Winter site fidelity and winter movements in Common Loons (Gavia immer) across North America. Condor 117:485–493
Pavey CR, Nano CEM (2009) Bird assemblages of arid Australia: vegetation patterns have a greater effect than disturbance and resource pulses. J Arid Environ 73:634–642
Paxton KL, van Riper IIIC, O’Brien C (2008) Movement patterns and stopover ecology of Wilson’s warblers during spring migration on the Lower Colorado river in Southeastern Arizona. Condor 110:672–681
Pravosudov VV, Kitaysky AS, Omanska A (2006) The relationship between migratory behaviour, memory and the hippocampus: an intraspecific comparison. Proc R Soc B 273:2641–2649
Rappole JH, Jones P (2003) Evolution of Old and New world migration systems. Ardea 90:525–537
Roshier DA, Klomp NI, Asmus M (2006) Movements of a nomadic waterfowl, Grey Teal Anas gracilis, across inland Australia: results from satellite telemetry spanning 15 months. Ardea 94:461–475
Runge CA, Tulloch A, Hammill E, Possingham HP, Fuller RA (2015) Geographic range size and extinction risk assessment in nomadic species. Cons Biol 29:865–876
Sanderson FJ, Donald PF, Pain DJ, Burfield IJ, van Bommel FPJ (2006) Long-term population declines in Afro-Palearctic migrant birds. Biol Cons 131:93–105
Sergio F, Tanferna A, De Stephanis R, Lopez Jimenez L, Blas J, Tavecchia G, Preatoni D, Hiraldo F (2014) Individual improvements and selective mortality shape lifelong migratory performance. Nature 515:410–413
Simmons RE, Barnard P, Jamieson IG (1998) What precipitates influxes of wetland birds to ephemeral pans in arid landscapes? Observations from Namibia. Ostrich 70:145–148
Simmons RE, Barnard P, Dean WRJ, Midgley GF, Thuiller W, Hughes G (2004) Climate change and birds: perspectives and prospects from southern Africa. Ostrich 75:295–308
Smith JE (2015) Effects of environmental variation on the composition and dynamics of an arid-adapted Australian bird community. Pacif Cons Biol 21:74–86
Sol D (2003) Behavioural flexibility: a neglected issue in the ecological and evolutionary literature? In: Reader SM, Laland KN (eds) Animal innovation. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 63–82
Sol D, Lefebvre L (2000) Behavioural flexibility predicts invasion success in birds introduced to New Zealand. Oikos 90:599–605
Sol D, Lefebvre L, Rodriguez-Teijeiro JD (2005) Brain size, innovative propensity and migratory behaviour in temperate Palaearctic birds. Proc R Soc B 272:1433–1441
Sol D, Garcia N, Iwaniuk A, Davis K, Meade A, Boyle WA, Szekely T (2010) Evolutionary divergence in brain size between migratory and resident birds. PLoS ONE 5(3):e9617. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0009617
Somveille M, Rodrigues ASL, Manica A (2015) Why do birds migrate? A macroecological perspective. Global Ecol Biogeogr 24:664–674
Telleria JL, Ramirez A, Galarza A, Carbonell R, Perez-Tris J, Santos T (2008) Do migratory pathways affect the regional abundance of wintering birds? A test in northern Spain. J Biogeogr 36:220–229
van Turnhout CAM, Foppen RPB, Leuven RSEW, van Strien A, Siepel H (2010) Life-history and ecological correlates of population change in Dutch breeding birds. Biol Cons 143:173–181
Vardanis Y, Nilsson J-A, Klaassen RHG, Strandberg R, Alerstam T (2016) Consistency in long-distance bird migration: contrasting patterns in time and space for two raptors. Anim Behav 113:177–187
Velez-Espino LA, McLaughlin RL, Robillard M (2013) Ecological advantages of partial migration as a conditional strategy. Theor Pop Biol 85:1–11
Winkler H, Leisler B, Bernroider G (2004) Ecological constraints on the evolution of avian brains. J Orn 145:238–244
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
This article is part of the Special Issue Animal cognition in a human-dominated world.
Electronic supplementary material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
About this article
Cite this article
Mettke-Hofmann, C. Avian movements in a modern world: cognitive challenges. Anim Cogn 20, 77–86 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-016-1006-1
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-016-1006-1