In recent years, there has been a rapid growth in new digital technology, and, despite its advantages, excessive digital media use by children has been associated with physical and psychological adverse health consequences [1]. This line of research has paid particular attention to how the use of digital devices may affect young children’s sleep [2]. Sleep is a basic physiological process characterized by intense brain activity. It promotes physical and mental well-being and has an impact on multiple aspects of development from early childhood [3]. Indeed, sleep problems in early childhood are associated with a number of poor developmental outcomes in neurocognitive [3,4,5], socioemotional [6, 7], physical health [8, 9] and family functioning [10,11,12].

Most sleep disorders in childhood may be attributed to variations in one or more of its basic parameters [6]. According to Buysse [13] and Meltzer et al. [14], six main parameters can be identified: (1) sleep duration; (2) efficiency; (3) time spent sleeping; (4) alertness during the day; (5) subjective quality; and (6) behaviors associated with sleep, including screen use. The relationship between these sleep parameters and different health risks from early childhood onwards has been the subject of a large body of research that concludes there is a bidirectional relationship between sleep disorders and physical and psychological problems [6, 15]. However, much of the research has focused on adult populations or on school-age children and adolescents, with data on preschoolers being scant and inconclusive [16].

Sleep measurement and development in preschoolers

Although studies with preschoolers typically use sleep diaries or parent-report questionnaires, as they are easy to use and inexpensive and have shown to be useful for estimating some aspects of children’s sleep, these types of subjective sleep measures are susceptible to the biases of those who complete them, such as inaccuracies in recall or social desirability bias [17, 18]. However, the use of objective measures, such as actigraphy or polysomnography (PSG), may be useful in overcoming these limitations. In this sense, the most complete test for the study of sleep and its phases is PSG [19], although the costs associated with the use of PSG and the laboratory context in which it is implemented hinder its application in studies using preschool samples [20]. For this reason, several studies have resorted to the use of actigraphy as a reference method for estimating sleep in everyday contexts since the algorithms applied have high precision and sensitivity [21, 22]. Actigraphy is a non-invasive technique that permits objective estimation of sleep parameters by recording the frequency of movement in terms of acceleration. Its results tend to be highly consistent with data provided by parental sleep reports [23].

Sleep duration, quality and architecture change across the lifespan, especially in the first five years of life, given that the process is dependent on the maturation of the central nervous system, the environment and experience [24]. The most important changes occur from birth to age two. These are due, in part, to the processes of brain maturation, neuroplasticity and myelination that take place in this developmental period [25]. From the second year and throughout the preschool stage, changes in sleep are more gradual [26, 27]: the total number of hours of sleep progressively decreases and most of this sleep takes place during the night, with 10–13 h of sleep being recommended; daytime naps decrease; the amount of REM sleep is reduced; the length of REM-NREM cycles extends to 90 min; sleep onset latency (i.e., the time it takes the child to fall asleep after going to bed) increases; and fewer awakenings occur, thus improving sleep efficiency. From the age of 6, sleep structure begins to resemble that of adults [28].

Screen use in childhood and its effect on sleep

Guidelines published by prominent institutions recommend avoiding screen use until the age of two, and reducing such use in preschoolers to between 30 min and an hour a day [29, 30]. However, the burgeoning role of digital devices in the 21st century and their presence in children’s lives from birth complicates complying with these recommendations.

Excessive time using digital devices can influence sleep disturbances in preschoolers in several ways: (1) electromagnetic radiation, perceived as the blue light from screens, can affect melatonin levels and, consequently, cause circadian disruption and sleep problems [31, 32]; (2) the length and time of screen use can be detrimental to sleep time or the performance of other activities that help acquire good sleep habits, thus impairing sleep quantity and quality [33, 34]; and (3) the type of content accessed, given that exposure to violence or adult content elevates a child’s psychophysiological arousal, affecting pre-sleep relaxation and increasing the likelihood of delayed sleep onset and disruptions in sleep continuity [33, 35, 36].

Sleep and behavioural problems in preschoolers

Sleep disturbances in childhood tend to be comorbid with behavioral disorders [6, 37], especially in terms of internalizing and externalizing problems. The former have been associated with inwardly directed behaviors and emotional states, such as withdrawal, anxiety, inhibition or mood instability, while externalizing symptoms refer to outwardly directed behaviors and mood states and are characterized by a lack of emotional regulation, manifest in behaviors such as anger and irritability, aggression, hyperactivity or rule breaking [38].

Previous studies have reported an association between sleep problems and a higher level of externalizing and internalizing symptoms. For example, Scharf et al. [39] examined, using maternal reports, how nighttime sleep duration affected externalizing behavior in 4-year-olds, concluding that shorter sleep duration was associated with a greater likelihood of hyperactivity, aggressiveness, impulsivity, and tantrums. Likewise, relationships have been found with internalizing symptoms, such as emotional difficulties, anxiety or somatization [40, 41]. Similar results were obtained with 2-to 4-year-olds years in a longitudinal study, with longer sleep duration and sleep quality being predictors of fewer behavioral problems [42]. Furthermore, few works have used using actigraphy in preschool population are their results are inconsistent: while some suggest significant links between sleep problems and aggressive behaviors and emotional problems (e.g., [43]), others report no significant relationships (e.g., [44]).

Sleep and cognition in preschoolers

The first years of life are a fundamental stage for cognitive development, including aspects such as memory, language, and executive functions [45]. However, despite the growing evidence on the importance of sleep for proper cognitive functioning in preschoolers [15, 46], most studies have focused on school-aged children [47 for a review]. with information on infant and preschool samples continuing to be scant. Additionally, considering that the characteristics and architecture of sleep change rapidly during this life stage, findings from studies on older participants cannot reasonably be generalized to younger children [48].

Cross-sectional studies with preschoolers have yielded contrasting data. The work by Nathanson and Fries [49] found no relationship between parent-reported sleep time and performance in inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility. More recently, the study by Peled and Scher [50], with a sample of preschoolers aged 3 to 4 years, showed an association between working memory scores and sleep quality, but not with duration. Finally, Nieto et al. [5], with a sample of 3- to 6-year- olds, found that parent-reported nighttime sleep duration was related to inhibition and working memory, but not to cognitive flexibility. In addition, longitudinal data on preschoolers seem to indicate that children who present correct sleep development from very early ages tend to show better performance in subsequent stages (e.g., [8, 21]).

Given the importance of sleep in certain behavioral and cognitive processes, and knowing screen use can affect sleep patterns, we can hypothesize that excessive screen use at preschool age negatively affects behavior and cognition in the youngest children. For this review, we consider as excessive use of screens the one which exceeds the recommendations established by the health authorities, that is, more than an hour for preschool-age children [27, 28]. Hence, the main aim of this work was to systematically analyze the empirical evidence on the relationships between excessive screen use and sleep patterns in 3- to-6-olds years, as well as the impacts on psychological (internalizing and externalizing problems) and cognitive functioning.

To achieve this general aim, the following specific objectives were proposed: (a) to determine the impact of excessive screen use on sleep at the preschool stage; (b) to detect how these aspects related to sleep may impact different behavioral and cognitive factors; (c) to ascertain whether excessive screen use may influence the emergence of behavioral and cognitive problems in preschool years; and (d) to evaluate the findings of the contributions and identify future research directions.

Materials and methods

This study used the criteria for conducting systematic reviews described by the PRISMA methodology [25]. To formulate the research question, the following format was used: problem of interest, population, exposure and outcome. The resulting question was thus as follows What is the relationship between excessive screen use, sleep-related aspects, and the appearance of psychological and/or cognitive effects in preschool children (3–6 years)? To answer this question and limit the search to the topic of interest, the descriptors for health sciences (DeCS) were used, with English always being the working language.

Search strategy

An exhaustive electronic search of the Medline (PubMed), PsycINFO (American Psychological Association), Scopus and Web of Science databases was conducted from February 7 to April 30, 2022. This search was applied to the fields Title + Summary + Keywords. The terms included in the search were “preschool” OR “early childhood” OR child*; -AND “screen time” OR “screen media” OR “media exposure” OR “television” OR “electronic device” OR “tablet” OR “mobile” OR “technology” -AND “sleep duration” OR “sleep disturbance” OR “sleep disorder” OR “sleep quality” -AND “behavior” OR externali* OR internali* OR cognit*. An analysis of the bibliographic references of the included studies completed the electronic search.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

In accordance with the aims of the present study, we selected publications meeting the following inclusion criteria: (a) the study sample had to consist of preschoolers aged between 3 and 6; (b) the study included measures of sleep duration, reported by primary caregivers or actigraphy; other primary caregiver self-report measures referring to sleep patterns could also be included; (c) screen exposure measures necessarily included time of use; (d) externalizing and/or internalizing behaviors were measured using validated questionnaires for participants or primary caregivers; and (e) cognitive variables focused on language, executive functions and/or theory of mind, and were measured by questionnaires administered by an expert. The following were excluded: (a) studies published in a language other than English; (b) studies with samples of preschoolers with psychopathologies or learning disabilities; (c) studies without measurements of any of the variables of interest (sleep, screen exposure and behavior/cognition), or where such measures could not be estimated with the data reported, or those that did not establish relationships between these variables; (d) intervention studies; or (e) theoretical or systematic reviews, meta-analyses, theses or book chapters.

Data extraction and analysis

Following the initial bibliographic search, duplicates were excluded from the selection of articles identified. Subsequently, we reviewed the titles and abstracts of the remaining articles to check they met the inclusion and exclusion criteria, and those eligible for inclusion were selected. Articles that raised doubts were evaluated by an independent reviewer until a decision was reached on their inclusion or exclusion (see Fig. 1 for a flow chart adapted from the PRISMA methodology).

Fig. 1
figure 1

PRISMA flow diagram

Risk of bias

The quality of the included articles was assessed using a list of methodological criteria related to the topic of interest (See Table 1). The criteria used to carry out the quality analysis were established based on the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) for studies that are not clinical trials [51]. Twelve criteria were used: (1) sociodemographic data of the sample were described (age, race, parents’ educational level, etc.); (2) inclusion/exclusion criteria were included; (3) the data collection process was described (e.g., interview, parental report); (4) the study sample was randomly selected; (5) the sample size was greater than 500; (6) comparisons were made between groups based on exposure variables (sleep and screen use); (7) the “screen use” variable was objectively evaluated; (8) the “sleep” variable was objectively assessed by actigraphy; (9) standardized, validated questionnaires were used for behavioral and cognitive variables; (10) behavioral and cognitive variables were assessed by an individual blinded to exposure status; (11) statistical adjustments were made to account for the main confounding factors; and (12) the results were followed up. Each criterion was scored as met/not met. The total study quality score was obtained from the sum of the criteria met, with scores ranging from 0 to 12. Study quality was rated as low (0–4), medium (5–8), or high (9–12).

Table 1 List of criteria for assessing the methodological quality of studies

Results

A total of 510 studies were identified. Of these, 72 were dropped due to their being duplicates. Of the remaining 438, titles and abstracts were reviewed, and 332 were excluded (232 focused on a topic other than that of interest; 51 included samples of non-preschoolers; and 49 were review studies, meta-analyses or book chapters). This left 106 texts to be read in full. Of these 106 studies, 49 included populations of the wrong age range and 44 did not measure one or more of the variables of interest considered in the present review. Finally, 13 articles were finally selected for this systematic review (Table 2).

Characteristics of the studies included

The scores for the quality of the studies included ranged from 5 to 9 points The mean quality was 7.2 points, meaning the overall quality of the results obtained is medium. The methodological deficiencies are mainly related to the lack of standardized questionnaires to evaluate screen use in preschool population, the absence of follow-up of the results obtained, and the use of subjective sleep reported through questionnaires completed by the children’s parents or caregivers.

All the studies used a cross-sectional design, and only the work by McNeill et al. [56] included a 12-month longitudinal follow-up. The studies were published between 2007 and 2022.

Sleep time was assessed in various ways: 46.2% of the studies included only direct parental reports (n = 6), 30.8% used parent-completed questionnaires (n = 4), and 15.4% used only actigraphy (n = 2); the remaining study [55] used actigraphy together with parental reports. The participants’ mean nighttime sleep duration ranged from 9.15 h/d [61] to 10.6 h/d [58]. Other measures of sleep patterns were sleep onset latency, night wakings, efficiency, bedtime resistance, sleep anxiety, parasomnias and daytime sleepiness. All the studies measured screen time by means of direct parent reports consisting of open-ended questions (e.g., “On average, how many hours per day does your child sit and watch TV?“), given the absence of standardized measures. Regarding the types of digital devices, two focused exclusively on TV viewing [54, 57], while the rest explored multiple devices, with the most common being TV (n = 11), computers (n = 9), and tablets (n = 7). The studies primarily recorded mean active TV viewing time, which ranged from 2.8 h/d [61] to 3.8 h/d [58]. Other aspects of screen use taken into account by the researchers were passive exposure, presence of screens in the child’s room or proximity of use to sleep periods.

Finally, as regards behavioral and cognitive aspects, most of the studies focus on the analysis of internalizing and externalizing behaviors (n = 11). Those incorporating cognitive measures mainly implemented measures of performance for Theory of Mind (n = 5) and Executive Functions (n = 3).

Table 2 Overview of studies included in the review

Relationships between sleep and screen media

The studies made use of three primary mechanisms to explain the impact of screen use on sleep: direct exposure, passive exposure, and the presence of digital media in the preschoolers’ bedrooms. Direct exposure is consistently associated with shorter total sleep duration, both as measured by actigraphy [54] and by parent reports [49, 55, 59]. Positive associations are also found between prolonged exposure and increased daytime sleepiness [58, 59, 62], increased onset latency [55], increased bedtime resistance [58], and parasomnias [62]. Environmental or passive exposure is also associated with lower total sleep duration [49] and increased daytime sleepiness [59]. Finally, there is no consensus as to whether the presence of a TV in a child’s bedroom contributes to sleep disturbances.

Relationships between Screen Media and behavioral and cognitive outcomes

In most of the studies, prolonged screen use is associated with a greater likelihood of internalizing and externalizing behaviors [52, 55, 59, 61, 63], with such associations being more consistent for externalizing behaviors. Additionally, Helm and Spencer’s [54] study reveals a main effect of the presence of TV in a child’s bedroom on negative affect; that is, children with a TV in their bedrooms scored higher on negative affect than those without. Furthermore, Nathanson and Beyens [58] report that the use of handheld video game consoles is associated with better scores on a self-control scale (negative affect, impulsivity, frustration…).

As regards cognitive performance, three studies show that excessive screen use is associated with lower scores on executive functioning [49, 58], Theory of Mind (ToM) tasks [49], and attention [57]. However, two other studies find no such associations [53, 56]. Thus, there is no clear evidence supporting relationships between screen time and poorer performance on cognitive tasks. Furthermore, passive TV exposure and the presence of a TV in a child’s bedroom correlates negatively with scores on Theory of Mind and Executive Functions [49].

Relationships between Sleep and behavioral and cognitive outcomes

Shorter sleep duration than recommended is significantly related to internalizing and externalizing problems in various studies [60,61,62,63], although these associations are less clear in the case of internalizing behaviors. Wu et al. [61], for example, find that children whose sleep duration was lower than the group mean presented a greater risk of behavioral problems and hyperactivity, but report no significant increase in the risk of emotional symptoms. Meanwhile, Kahn et al. [55] found significant relationships between parent-reported sleep quality and higher internalizing and externalizing scores, but no such associations were found for sleep duration, measured both by actigraph and by parental report. Finally, Séguin and Klimek [59] only observed a positive relationship between the number and duration of nighttime awakenings and symptoms of anxiety or fear.

The relationship between sleep and cognitive variables has been the subject of less research, with inconclusive results. While Nathanson and Fries [49] report that longer total sleep duration significantly correlates with better performance on Theory of Mind, Cliff et al. [53] find no such association. In no case are differences in executive function scores found depending on sleep duration and sleep quality [49, 56]. Finally, in the study by Tso et al. [60], the group classified as “sleep-deprived” (children who slept ≤ 7 h per day during the assessment week) scored significantly lower in the cognitive and language domain compared to the reference group that slept the recommended number of hours.

Sleep as Mediator or Moderator

Some of the studies examined whether sleep acts as a moderating or mediating factor between screen use and behavioral and/or cognitive problems.

The work by Kahn et al. [55] uses sleep duration measured with an actigraph as a mediating variable between screen use and the appearance of internalizing and externalizing behaviors. Screen use explained 17% of the variance in behavioral scores when sleep was included as a mediator. Their regression slope analysis showed that greater exposure to screens was associated with behavioral problems in children with low sleep duration (this group of participants showed an average of 9.24 h of sleep per day). However, screen use time was not associated with higher levels of behavioral problems in children whose sleep duration was moderate or high (with sleep averages of 9.85 h/day and 10.46 h/day respectively).

Zhao et al. [63] conducted a similar analysis. In their study, the direct and indirect effects of screen use on total internalizing and externalizing scores were separately 0.294 (95% CI: 0.261, 0. 328; p < 0.001) and 0.123 (95% CI 0.112, 0.135; p < 0.001), respectively, which accounted for 0.417 as the total effect. These effects were mediated by sleep duration (0.5%).

Furthermore, the findings of Nathanson and Beyens [58] suggest that afternoon/evening tablet use is significantly associated with worse scores on attention and impulse control when bedtime (95% CI = -0.0015, -0.0002)]), bedtime resistance (95% CI = -0.011, -0.00001) and nighttime sleep duration (95% CI = -0.0014, 0–0002) are used as mediators. Additionally, the relationship between tablet use and scores on attention and inhibition was only significant when the child slept less (Coef. = 0.0007, 95% CI = 0.0001, 0.0013). The cut-off point for sleep duration was less than 10.61hs/day. In contrast, a positive association was found between the use of video game consoles and better attention and inhibition scores, but this was only significant for children who slept longer (Coef. = 0.0009, 95% CI = 0.0001, 0.0017). Sleep duration was shown to be a significant moderator when children slept more than 10.42 h/day.

Finally, Nathanson and Fries [49] report two main findings: (a) an indirect effect of background TV on Theory of Mind scores through lower sleep duration; (b) sleep duration mediates the relationship between afternoon/evening TV viewing and ToM performance, with the results of the analysis being significant.

Discussion

The aim of this systematic review was to provide an overview of the state of the art on how excessive screen use affects sleep-related aspects in preschoolers, which, in turn, influences behavioral and cognitive aspects.

The results of the review suggest that less than half of the 3- to 6-year olds meet health authorities’ recommendations for exposure to screens (less than an hour a day), which coincides with the findings of a recent meta-analysis [64]. This prolonged use may affect sleep parameters through the following: both active and passive exposure, especially at night; increased presence of digital devices in a child’s environment; and displacement of other activities that may favor sleep onset [49, 54, 55, 58, 62, 65]. These patterns of digital device use are consistently associated with (a) a reduction in the total amount of sleep, which is in line with previous research (e.g., [61, 62, 66]); and (b) worse sleep patterns, observable in aspects such as increased bedtime resistance, more nighttime awakenings and daytime sleepiness, results which also align with those of other studies (e.g., [63, 64, 67, 68]). Therefore, our findings point towards a multifaceted impact of screen use on preschoolers’ sleep, such that those presenting greater exposure to screens are the most likely to have shorter duration and poorer quality of sleep.

Furthermore, small to moderate effects of screen use on behavioral and cognitive problems were found [52, 55, 62]. Exposure to more than one hour of screen time in preschoolers appears to be associated with a greater risk of an onset of emotional difficulties, increased negative affect and anxiety, as well as aggressive, hostile, or hyperactive behaviors. Additionally, the data show more robust associations between screen time and externalizing behaviors compared to internalizing behaviors, coinciding with previous research [69]. As regards the relationship between screen use and cognitive variables, drawing conclusions is complicated by the lack of studies. However, small to moderate negative associations were found between increased screen exposure and performance in attention, executive functioning, and theory of mind [49, 57, 58]. Additionally, Nathanson and Beyens [58] found that moderate use of video game consoles may improve preschoolers’ attention. These relationships might be influenced by factors such as the multimedia content children engage with or the time of day when they are most exposed [69]. The results of the present review are consistent with the current literature, which reports inconclusive findings on the impact of digital devices on younger children’s psychological and cognitive well-being [70, 71].

Regarding the relationship between sleep and behavioral problems, broadly speaking, the results suggest that lower sleep duration and quality leads to a greater likelihood of externalizing and internalizing problems [55, 59,60,61,62,63]. This relationship appears to be more consistent for externalizing problems, arguably because these types of behavioral problems tend to be more common in the preschool years and decrease with age, while internalizing behaviors typically follow a contrasting developmental trajectory [72]. Moreover, although there seems to be a relationship between sleep disorders and certain cognitive functioning variables such as Theory of Mind [49], there is a lack of agreement between the studies included in the review.

Finally, this review was intended to determine whether sleep duration and sleep quality interact with screen time to predict greater behavioral or cognitive problems. Increased screen exposure is associated with higher scores on behavioral problems (primarily externalizing) and lower scores on cognitive performance (attention, EF, and ToM) when sleep duration is short [49, 58, 63]. Furthermore, the results appear to show a greater influence of factors such as nighttime screen use, or passive and prolonged exposure, rather than total active use time. Although a substantial number of studies report associations between screen use and psychopathology in children, the size of these relationships varies from moderate to small [73], suggesting that other factors may enhance or bolster the consequences of screen use. In this respect, some researchers have suggested that sleep is a bioregulatory system that plays a role in children’s ability to self-regulate emotions, behavior, and thought; in other words, sleep may both protect from, and contribute to, the occurrence of childhood psychopathology depending on the context, with prolonged exposure to screens being a risk factor for sleep impairment [74].

Study limitations and future directions

Firstly, this systematic review was affected by the heterogeneous nature of the studies included. This heterogeneity is evident in both the great variability of the sample sizes and in the diverse definitions and forms of measurement of the variables under study. In this sense, it is worth highlighting the absence of standardized questionnaires for assessing screen use in preschool population, which might have impacted the validity of the results. Similarly, much of the research focuses on the use of tests and sleep diaries to be completed by parents, while the use of objective measures, such as actigraphy, is less common. Additionally, the authors of the articles included use seven different questionnaires to measure behavioral and cognitive variables, with these mostly being completed by the children’s parents, who are familiar with both their children’s sleep status and their screen use, which may enhance the risk of bias.

Secondly, the current state of the literature makes it difficult to analyze specific aspects of screen use (e.g., time of use, passive exposure…), sleep (e.g., duration, bedtime resistance, onset latency…), and behavior and cognition, mainly due to the great variation in the designs and statistical analyses implemented. The quality and comparability of findings could be improved by focusing research on specific aspects of both exposure and outcome variables. Additionally, different aspects of screen use or sleep cannot always be assumed to have the same effect on behavioral and cognitive factors.

Thirdly, it is worth noting that the vast majority of studies are cross-sectional, and are thus unable to establish a causal relationship between screen exposure or sleep and behavioral and/or cognitive aspects. The literature suggests that high levels of screen use affect both sleep duration and quality, and that this, in turn, has an impact on behavior and cognition in preschoolers. However, it is plausible that other factors may explain some of these relationships, such as, for example, parent-child communication. Further longitudinal studies are required to determine the direction of the impact of these variables on each other.

Finally, few of the works include mediation or moderation analysis among the statistical methods used. This makes it difficult to make comparisons between studies and to draw clear conclusions on the mediating or moderating role of sleep in the relationship between screen use and behavioral and cognitive disorders.

As future research, we propose delving into the associations between specific aspects of screen use, sleep, and behavioral and cognitive measures, using longitudinal studies with sufficient statistical power. In addition, the use of objective measures for sleep measurement would be of interest, as would be the assessment of behavioral or cognitive outcomes by a researcher uninformed of the sample’s sleep status or screen use. It would be useful to create a strictly defined protocol that would allow criteria to be unified when selecting and measuring the different variables, thus facilitating the comparison and interpretation of the results obtained in studies.

Conclusions

The current literature appears to suggest that screen use and sleep are related to behavioral and cognitive development in preschool years, but further research is needed to determine the scope and potential of these relationships. Studies in this area could assist in the creation of policies and general recommendations for parents, educators, physicians and other professionals. Additionally, deeper knowledge of how digital devices are used from early childhood and how they affect sleep and intra-individual aspects would allow for the development of effective early interventions on such overuse.