Abstract
Recently, the misinformation problem has been addressed with a crowdsourcing-based approach: to assess the truthfulness of a statement, instead of relying on a few experts, a crowd of non-expert is exploited. We study whether crowdsourcing is an effective and reliable method to assess truthfulness during a pandemic, targeting statements related to COVID-19, thus addressing (mis)information that is both related to a sensitive and personal issue and very recent as compared to when the judgment is done. In our experiments, crowd workers are asked to assess the truthfulness of statements, and to provide evidence for the assessments. Besides showing that the crowd is able to accurately judge the truthfulness of the statements, we report results on workers’ behavior, agreement among workers, effect of aggregation functions, of scales transformations, and of workers background and bias. We perform a longitudinal study by re-launching the task multiple times with both novice and experienced workers, deriving important insights on how the behavior and quality change over time. Our results show that workers are able to detect and objectively categorize online (mis)information related to COVID-19; both crowdsourced and expert judgments can be transformed and aggregated to improve quality; worker background and other signals (e.g., source of information, behavior) impact the quality of the data. The longitudinal study demonstrates that the time-span has a major effect on the quality of the judgments, for both novice and experienced workers. Finally, we provide an extensive failure analysis of the statements misjudged by the crowd-workers.
1 Introduction
“We’re concerned about the levels of rumours and misinformation that are hampering the response. [...] we’re not just fighting an epidemic; we’re fighting an infodemic. Fake news spreads faster and more easily than this virus, and is just as dangerous. That’s why we’re also working with search and media companies like Facebook, Google, Pinterest, Tencent, Twitter, TikTok, YouTube and others to counter the spread of rumours and misinformation. We call on all governments, companies and news organizations to work with us to sound the appropriate level of alarm, without fanning the flames of hysteria.”
These are the alarming words used by Dr. Tedros Adhanom Ghebreyesus, the WHO (World Health Organization) Director General during his speech at the Munich Security Conference on 15 February 2020.Footnote 1 It is telling that the WHO Director General chooses to target explicitly misinformation related problems.
Indeed, during the still ongoing COVID-19 health emergency, all of us have experienced mis- and dis-information. The research community has focused on several COVID-19 related issues [5], ranging from machine learning systems aiming to classify statements and claims on the basis of their truthfulness [66], search engines tailored to the COVID-19 related literature, as in the ongoing TREC-COVID ChallengeFootnote 2 [46], topic-specific workshops like the NLP COVID workshop at ACL’20,Footnote 3 and evaluation initiatives like the TREC Health Misinformation Track.Footnote 4 Besides the academic research community, commercial social media platforms also have looked at this issue.Footnote 5
Among all the approaches, in some very recent work, Roitero et al. [47], La Barbera et al. [27], and Roitero et al. [51] have studied whether crowdsourcing can be used to identify misinformation. As it is well known, crowdsourcing means to outsource a task—which is usually performed by a limited number of experts—to a large mass (the “crowd”) of unknown people (the “crowd workers”), by means of an open call. The recent works mentioned before [27, 47, 51] specifically crowdsource the task of misinformation identification, or rather assessment of the truthfulness of statements made by public figures (e.g., politicians), usually on political, economical, and societal issues.
The idea that the crowd is able to identify misinformation might sound implausible at first—isn’t the crowd the very means by which misinformation is spread? However, on the basis of the previous studies [27, 47, 51], it appears that the crowd can provide high quality results when asked to assess the truthfulness of statements, provided that adequate countermeasures and quality assurance techniques are employed.
In this paperFootnote 6, we address the very same problem, but focusing on statements about COVID-19. This is motivated by several reasons. First, COVID-19 is of course a hot topic but, although there is a great amount of research efforts worldwide devoted to its study, there are no studies yet using crowdsourcing to assess truthfulness of COVID-19-related statements. To the best of our knowledge, we are the first to report on crowd assessment of COVID-19-related misinformation. Second, the health domain is particularly sensitive, so it is interesting to understand if the crowdsourcing approach is adequate also in such a particular domain. Third, in the previous work [27, 47, 51], the statements judged by the crowd were not recent. This means that evidence on statement truthfulness was often available out there (on the Web), and although the experimental design prevented to easily find that evidence, it cannot be excluded that the workers did find it, or perhaps they were familiar with the particular statement because, for instance, it had been discussed in the press. By focusing on COVID-19-related statements we instead naturally target recent statements: in some cases the evidence might be still out there, but this will happen more rarely.
Fourth, an almost ideal tool to address misinformation would be a crowd able to assess truthfulness in real time, immediately after the statement becomes public: although we are not there yet, and there is a long way to go, we find that targeting recent statements is a step forward in the right direction. Fifth, our experimental design differs in some details, and allows us to address novel research questions. Finally, we also perform a longitudinal study by collecting the data multiple times and launching the task at different timestamps, considering both novice workers—i.e., workers who have never done the task before—and experienced workers—i.e., workers who have performed the task in previous batches and were invited to do the task again. This allows us to study the multiple behavioral aspects of the workers when assessing the truthfulness of judgments.
This paper is structured as follows. In Section 2, we summarize related work. In Section 3, we detail the aims of this study and list some specific research questions, addressed by means of the experimental setting described in Section 4. In Section 5, we present and discuss the results, while in Section 6, we present the longitudinal study conducted. Finally, in Section 7, we conclude the paper: we summarize our main findings, list the practical implications, highlight some limitations, and sketch future developments.
2 Background
We survey the background work on theoretical and conceptual aspects of misinformation spreading, the specific case of the COVID-19 infodemic, the relation between truthfulness classification and argumentation, and on the use of crowdsourcing to identify fake news.
2.1 Echo chambers and filter bubbles
The way information spreads through social media and, in general, the Web has been widely studied, leading to the discovery of a number of phenomena that were not so evident in the pre-Web world. Among those, echo chambers and epistemic bubbles seem to be central concepts [38].
Regarding their importance in news consumption, Flaxman et al. [17] examine the browsing history of US-based users who read news articles. They found that both search engines and social networks increase the ideological distance between individuals, and that they increase the exposure of the user to material of opposed political views.
These effects can be exploited to spread misinformation. [62] modelled how echo chambers contribute to the virality of misinformation, by providing an initial environment in which misinformation is propagated up to some level that makes it easier to expand outside the echo chamber. This helps to explain why clusters, usually known to restrain the diffusion of information, become central enablers of spread.
On the other side, acting against misinformation seems not to be an easy task, at least due to the backfire effect, i.e., the effect for which someone’s belief hardens when confronted with evidence opposite to its opinion. Sethi and Rangaraju [53] studied the backfire effect and presented a collaborative framework aimed at fighting it by making the user understand her/his emotions and biases. However, the paper does not discuss the ways techniques for recognizing misinformation can be effectively translated to actions for fighting it in practice.
2.2 Truthfulness and argumentation
Truthfulness classification and the process of fact-checking are strongly related to the scrutiny of factual information extensively studied in argumentation theory [3, 28, 54, 61, 64, 68]. Lawrence and Reed [28] survey the techniques which are the foundations for argument mining, i.e., extracting and processing the inference and structure of arguments expressed using natural language. Sethi [54] leverages argumentation theory and proposes a framework to verify the truthfulness of facts, Visser et al. [64] uses it to increase the critical thinking ability of people who assess media reports, Sethi et al. [55] uses it together with pedagogical agents in order to develop a recommendation system to help fighting misinformation, and Snaith et al. [57] present a platform based on a modular architecture and distributed open source for argumentation and dialogue.
2.3 COVID-19 infodemic
The number of initiatives to apply Information Access—and, in general, Artificial Intelligence—techniques to combat the COVID-19 infodemic has been rapidly increasing (see Bullock et al. [5] for a survey). Tangcharoensathien et al. [58] distilled a subset of 50 actions from a set of 594 ideas crowdsourced during a technical consultation held online by WHO (World Health Organization) to build a framework for managing infodemics in health emergencies.
There is significant effort on analyzing COVID-19 information on social media, and linking to data from external fact-checking organizations to quantify the spread of misinformation [9, 19, 69]. Mejova and Kalimeri [33] analyzed Facebook advertisements related to COVID-19, and found that around 5% of them contain errors or misinformation. Crowdsourcing methodologies have also been used to collect and analyze data from patients with cancer who are affected by the COVID-19 pandemic [11].
Tsai et al. [63] investigate the relationships between news consumption, trust, intergroup contact, and prejudicial attitudes toward Asians and Asian Americans residing in the USA during the COVID-19 pandemic
2.4 Crowdsourcing truthfulness
Recent work has focused on the automatic classification of truthfulness or fact-checking [2, 12, 25, 34, 37, 43, 55].
Zubiaga and Ji [71] investigated, using crowdsourcing, the reliability of tweets in the setting of disaster management. CLEF developed a Fact-Checking Lab [4, 12, 37] to address the issue of ranking sentences according to some fact-checking property.
There is recent work that studies how to collect truthfulness judgments by means of crowdsourcing using fine grained scales [27, 47, 51]. Samples of statements from the PolitiFact dataset—originally published by Wang [65]—have been used to analyze the agreement of workers with labels provided by experts in the original dataset. Workers are asked to provide the truthfulness of the selected statements, by means of different fine grained scales. Roitero et al. [47] compared two fine grained scales: one in the [0,100] range and one in the \((0,+\infty )\) range, on the basis of Magnitude Estimation [36]. They found that both scales allow to collect reliable truthfulness judgments that are in agreement with the ground truth. Furthermore, they show that the scale with one hundred levels leads to slightly higher agreement levels with the expert judgments. On a larger sample of PolitiFact statements, La Barbera et al. [27] asked workers to use the original scale used by the PolitiFact experts and the scale in the [0,100] range. They found that aggregated judgments (computed using the mean function for both scales) have a high level of agreement with expert judgments. Recent work by Roitero et al. [51] found similar results in terms of external agreement and its improvement when aggregating crowdsourced judgments, using statements from two different fact-checkers: PolitiFact and ABC Fact-Check (ABC). Previous work has also looked at internal agreement, i.e., agreement among workers [47, 51]. Roitero et al. [51] found that scales have low levels of agreement when compared with each other: correlation values for aggregated judgments on the different scales are around ρ = 0.55–0.6 for PolitiFact and ρ = 0.35–0.5 for ABC, and τ = 0.4 for PolitiFact and τ = 0.3 for ABC. This indicates that the same statements tend to be evaluated differently in different scales.
There is evidence of differences on the way workers provide judgments, influenced by the sources they examine, as well as the impact of worker bias. In terms of sources, La Barbera et al. [27] found that the vast majority of workers (around 73% for both scales) use indeed the PolitiFact website to provide judgments. Differently from La Barbera et al. [27], Roitero et al. [51] used a custom search engine in order to filter out PolitiFact and ABC from the list of results. Results show that, for all the scales, Wikipedia and news websites are the most popular sources of evidence used by the workers. In terms of worker bias, La Barbera et al. [27] and Roitero et al. [51] found that worker political background has an impact on how workers provide the truthfulness scores. In more detail, they found that workers are more tolerant and moderate when judging statements from their very own political party.
Roitero et al. [52] use a crowdsourcing-based approach to collect truthfulness judgments on a sample of PolitiFact statements concerning COVID-19 to understand whether crowdsourcing is a reliable method to be used to identify and correctly classify (mis)information during a pandemic. They find that workers are able to provide judgments which can be used to objectively identify and categorize (mis)information related to the pandemic and that such judgments show high level of agreement with expert labels when aggregated.
3 Aims and research questions
With respect to our previous work by Roitero et al. [47], La Barbera et al. [27], and Roitero et al. [51], and similarly to Roitero et al. [52], we focus on claims about COVID-19, which are recent and interesting for the research community, and arguably deal with a more relevant/sensitive topic for the workers. We investigate whether the health domain makes a difference in the ability of crowd workers to identify and correctly classify (mis)information, and if the very recent nature of COVID-19-related statements has an impact as well. We focus on a single truthfulness scale, given the evidence that the scale used does not make a significant difference. Another important difference is that we ask the workers to provide a textual justification for their decision: we analyze them to better understand the process followed by workers to verify information, and we investigate if they can be exploited to derive useful information.
In addition to Roitero et al. [52], we perform a longitudinal study that includes 3 additional crowdsourcing experiments over a period of 4 months and thus collecting additional data and evidence that include novel responses from new and old crowd workers (see footnote 6). The setup of each additional crowdsourcing experiment is the same as the one of Roitero et al. [52]. This longitudinal study is the focus of the research questions RQ6–RQ8 below, which are a novel contribution of this paper. Finally, we also exploit and analyze worker behavior. We present this paper as an extension of our previous work [52] in order to be able to compare against it and make the whole paper self-contained and much easier to follow, improving the readability and overall quality of the paper thanks to its novel research contributions.
More in detail, we investigate the following specific Research Questions:
-
RQ1
Are the crowd-workers able to detect and objectively categorize online (mis)information related to the medical domain and more specifically to COVID-19? What are the relationship and agreement between the crowd and the expert labels?
-
RQ2
Can the crowdsourced and/or the expert judgments be transformed or aggregated in a way that it improves the ability of workers to detect and objectively categorize online (mis)information?
-
RQ3
What is the effect of workers’ political bias and cognitive abilities?
-
RQ4
What are the signals provided by the workers while performing the task that can be recorded? To what extent are these signals related to workers’ accuracy? Can these signals be exploited to improve accuracy and, for instance, aggregate the labels in a more effective way?
-
RQ5
Which sources of information does the crowd consider when identifying online misinformation? Are some sources more useful? Do some sources lead to more accurate and reliable assessments by the workers?
-
RQ6
What is the effect of re-launching the experiment and re-collecting all the data at different time-spans? Are the findings from all previous research questions still valid?
-
RQ7
How does considering the judgments from workers which did the task multiple times change the findings of RQ6? Do they show any difference when compared to workers whom did the task only once?
-
RQ8
Which are the statements for which the truthfulness assessment done by the means of crowdsourcing fails? Which are the features and peculiarities of the statements that are misjudged by the crowd-workers?
4 Methods
In this section, we present the dataset used to carry out our experiments (Section 4.1), and the crowdsourcing task design (Section 4.2). Overall, we considered one dataset annotated by experts, one crowdsourced dataset, one judgment scale (the same for the expert and the crowd judgments), and a total of 60 statements.
4.1 Dataset
We considered as primary source of information the PolitiFact dataset [65] that was built as a “benchmark dataset for fake news detection” [65] and contains over 12k statements produced by public appearances of US politicians. The statements of the datasets are labeled by expert judges on a six-level scale of truthfulness (from now on referred to as E6): pants-on-fire, false, mostly-false, half-true, mostly-true, and true. Recently, the PolitiFact website (the source from where the statements of the PolitiFact dataset are taken) created a specific section related to the COVID-19 pandemic.Footnote 7 For this work, we selected 10 statements for each of the six PolitiFact categories, belonging to such COVID-19 section and with dates ranging from February 2020 to early April 2020. Appendix A contains the full list of the statements we used.
4.2 Crowdsourcing experimental setup
To collect our judgments, we used the crowdsourcing platform Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk). Each worker, upon accepting our Human Intelligence Task (HIT), is assigned a unique pair or values (input token, output token). Such pair is used to uniquely identify each worker, which is then redirected to an external server in order to complete the HIT. The worker uses the input token to perform the accepted HIT. If s/he successfully completes the assigned HIT, s/he is shown the output token, which is used to submit the MTurk HIT and receive the payment, which we set to $1.5 for a set of 8 statements.Footnote 8 The task itself is as follows: first, a (mandatory) questionnaire is shown to the worker, to collect his/her background information such as age and political views. The full set of questions and answers to the questionnaire can be found in Appendix B. Then, the worker needs to provide answers to three Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT) questions, which are used to measure the personal tendency to answer with an incorrect “gut” response or engage in further thinking to find the correct answer [18]. The CRT questionnaire and its answers can be found in Appendix C. After the questionnaire and CRT phase, the worker is asked to assess the truthfulness of 8 statements: 6 from the dataset described in Section 4.1 (one for each of the six considered PolitiFact categories) and 2 special statements called Gold Questions (one clearly true and the other clearly false) manually written by the authors of this paper and used as quality checks as detailed below. We used a randomization process when building the HITs to avoid all the possible source of bias, both within each HIT and considering the overall task.
To assess the truthfulness of each statement, the worker is shown: the Statement, the Speaker/Source, and the Year in which the statement was made. We asked the worker to provide the following information: the truthfulness value for the statement using the six-level scale adopted by PolitiFact, from now on referred to as C6 (presented to the worker using a radio button containing the label description for each category as reported in the original PolitiFact website), a URL that s/he used as a source of information for the fact-checking, and a textual motivation for her/his response (which can not include the URL, and should contain at least 15 words). In order to prevent the user from using PolitiFact as primary source of evidence, we implemented our own search engine, which is based on the Bing Web Search APIsFootnote 9 and filters out PolitiFact from the returned search results.
We logged the user behavior using a logger as the one detailed by Han et al. [21, 22], and we implemented in the task the following quality checks: (i) the judgments assigned to the gold questions have to be coherent (i.e., the judgment of the clearly false question should be lower than the one assigned to true question); and (ii) the cumulative time spent to perform each judgment should be of at least 10 s. Note that the CRT (and the questionnaire) answers were not used for quality check, although the workers were not aware of that.
Overall, we used 60 statements in total and each statement has been evaluated by 10 distinct workers. Thus, considering the main experiment, we deployed 100 MTurk HITs and we collected 800 judgments in total (600 judgments plus 200 gold question answers). Considering the main experiment and the longitudinal study all together, we collected over 4300 judgments from 542 workers, over a total of 7 crowdsourcing tasks. All the data used to carry out our experiments can be downloaded at https://github.com/KevinRoitero/crowdsourcingTruthfulness. The choice of making each statement being evaluated by 10 distinct workers deserves a discussion; such a number is aligned with previous studies using crowdsourcing to assess truthfulness [27, 47, 51, 52] and other concepts like relevance [30, 48]. We believe this number is a reasonable trade-off between having fewer statements evaluated by many workers and more statements evaluated by few workers. We think that an in depth discussion about the quantification of such trade-off requires further experiments and therefore is out of scope for this paper; we plan to address this matter in detail in future work.
5 Results and analysis for the main experiment
We first report some descriptive statistics about the population of workers and the data collected in our main experiment (Section 5.1). Then, we address crowd accuracy (i.e., RQ1) in Section 5.2, transformation of truthfulness scales (RQ2) in Section 5.3, worker background and bias (RQ3) in Section 5.4, worker behavior (RQ4) in Section 5.5; finally, we study information sources (RQ5) in Section 5.6. Results related to the longitudinal study (RQ6–8) are described and analyzed in Section 6.
5.1 Descriptive statistics
5.1.1 Worker background, behavior, and bias
Questionnaire
Overall, 334 workers resident in the USA participated in our experiment.Footnote 10 In each HIT, workers were first asked to complete a demographics questionnaire with questions about their gender, age, education, and political views. By analyzing the answers to the questionnaire of the workers which successfully completed the experiment, we derived the following demographic statistics. The majority of workers are in the 26–35 age range (39%), followed by 19–25 (27%), and 36–50 (22%). The majority of the workers are well educated: 48% of them have a four year college degree or a bachelor degree, 26% have a college degree, and 18% have a postgraduate or professional degree. Only about 4% of workers have a high school degree or less. Concerning political views, 33% of workers identified themselves as liberals, 26% as moderate, 17% as very liberal, 15% as conservative, and 9% as very conservative. Moreover, 52% of workers identified themselves as being Democrat, 24% as being Republican, and 23% as being Independent. Finally, 50% of workers disagreed on building a wall on the southern US border, and 37% of them agreed. Overall we can say that our sample is well balanced.
CRT test
Analyzing the CRT scores, we found that: 31% of workers did not provide any correct answer, 34% answered correctly to 1 test question, 18% answered correctly to 2 test questions, and only 17% answered correctly to all 3 test questions. We correlate the results of the CRT test and the worker quality to answer RQ3.
Behaviour (abandonment)
When considering the abandonment ratio (measured according to the definition provided by Han et al. [22]), we found that 100/334 workers (about 30%) successfully completed the task, 188/334 (about 56%) abandoned (i.e., voluntarily terminated the task before completing it), and 46/334 (about 7%) failed (i.e., terminated the task due to failing the quality checks too many times). Furthermore, 115/188 workers (about 61%) abandoned the task before judging the first statement (i.e., before really starting it).
5.2 RQ1: Crowd accuracy
5.2.1 External agreement
To answer RQ1, we start by analyzing the so called external agreement, i.e., the agreement between the crowd collected labels and the experts ground truth. Figure 1 shows the agreement between the PolitiFact experts (x-axis) and the crowd judgments (y-axis). In the first plot, each point is a judgment by a worker on a statement, i.e., there is no aggregation of the workers working on the same statement. In the next plots, all workers redundantly working on the same statement are aggregated using the mean (second plot), median (third plot), and majority vote (right-most plot). If we focus on the first plot (i.e., the one with no aggregation function applied), we can see that, overall, the individual judgments are in agreement with the expert labels, as shown by the median values of the boxplots, which are increasing as the ground truth truthfulness level increases. Concerning the aggregated values, it is the case that for all the aggregation functions the pants-on-fire and false categories are perceived in a very similar way by the workers; this behavior was already shown in [27, 51], and suggests that indeed workers have clear difficulties in distinguishing between the two categories; this is even more evident considering that the interface presented to the workers contained a textual description of the categories’ meaning in every page of the task.
If we look at the plots as a whole, we see that within each plot the median values of the boxplots increase when going from pants-on-fire to true (i.e., going from left to right of the x-axis of each chart). This indicates that the workers are overall in agreement with the PolitiFact ground truth, thus indicating that workers are indeed capable of recognizing and correctly classifying misinformation statements related to the COVID-19 pandemic. This is a very important and not obvious result: in fact, the crowd (i.e., the workers) is the primary source and cause of the spread of disinformation and misinformation statements across social media platforms [8]. By looking at the plots, and in particular focusing on the median values of the boxplots, it appears evident that the mean (second plot) is the aggregation function which leads to higher agreement levels, followed by the median (third plot) and the majority vote (right-most plot). Again, this behavior was already remarked in [27, 49, 51], and all the cited works used the mean as primary aggregation function.
To validate the external agreement, we measured the statistical significance between the aggregated rating for all the six PolitiFact categories; we considered both the Mann-Whitney rank test and the t-test, applying Bonferroni correction to account for multiple comparisons. Results are as follows: when considering adjacent categories (e.g., pants-on-fire and false), the difference between categories are never significant, for both tests and for all the three aggregation functions. When considering categories of distance 2 (e.g., pants-on-fire and mostly-false), the differences are never significant, apart from the median aggregation function, where there is statistical significance to the p < .05 level in 2/4 cases for both Mann-Whitney and t-test. When considering categories of distance 3, the differences are significant—for the Mann-Whitney and the t-test respectively—in the following cases: for the mean, 3/3 and 3/3 cases; for the median, 2/3 and 3/3 cases; for the majority vote, 0/3 and 1/3 cases. When considering categories of distance 4 and 5, the differences are always significant to the p > 0.01 level for all the aggregation functions and for all the tests, apart from the majority vote function and the Mann-Whitney test, where the significance is at the p > .05 level. In the following, we use the mean as it is the most commonly used approach for this type of data [51].
5.2.2 Internal agreement
Another standard way to address RQ1 and to analyze the quality of the work by the crowd is to compute the so-called internal agreement (i.e., the agreement among the workers). We measured the agreement with α [26] and Φ [7], two popular measures often used to compute workers’ agreement in crowdsourcing tasks [31, 47, 49, 51]. Analyzing the results, we found that the the overall agreement always falls in the [0.15,0.3] range, and that agreement levels measured with the two scales are very similar for the PolitiFact categories, with the only exception of Φ, which shows higher agreement levels for the mostly-true and true categories. This is confirmed by the fact that the α measure always falls in the Φ confidence interval, and the little oscillations in the agreement value are not always indication of a real change in the agreement level, especially when considering α [7]. Nevertheless, Φ seems to confirm the finding derived from Fig. 1 that workers are most effective in identifying and categorizing statements with a higher truthfulness level. This remark is also supported by [7] which shows that Φ is better in distinguishing agreement levels in crowdsourcing than α, which is more indicated as a measure of data reliability in non-crowdsourced settings.
5.3 RQ2: Transforming truthfulness scales
Given the positive results presented above, it appears that the answer to RQ1 is overall positive, even if with some exceptions. There are many remarks that can be made: first, there is a clear issue that affects the pants-on-fire and false categories, which are very often mis-classified by workers. Moreover, while PolitiFact used a six-level judgment scale, the usage of a two- (e.g., True/False) and a three-level (e.g., False / In between / True) scale is very common when assessing the truthfulness of statements [27, 51]. Finally, categories can be merged together to improve accuracy, as done for example by Tchechmedjiev et al. [59]. All these considerations lead us to RQ2, addressed in the following.
5.3.1 Merging ground truth levels
For all the above reasons, we performed the following experiment: we group together the six PolitiFact categories (i.e., E6) into three (referred to as E3) or two (E2) categories, which we refer respectively with 01, 23, and 45 for the three-level scale, and 012 and 234 for the two-level scale.
Figure 2 shows the result of such a process. As we can see from the plots, the agreement between the crowd and the expert judgments can be seen in a more neat way. As for Fig. 1, the median values for all the boxplots is increasing when going towards higher truthfulness values (i.e., going from left to right within each plot); this holds for all the aggregation functions considered, and it is valid for both transformations of the E6 scale, into three and two levels. Also in this case we computed the statistical significance between categories, applying the Bonferroni correction to account for multiple comparisons. Results are as follows. For the case of three groups, both the categories at distance one and two are always significant to the p < 0.01 level, for both the Mann-Whitney and the t-test, for all three aggregation functions. The same behavior holds for the case of two groups, where the categories of distance 1 are always significant to the p < 0.01 level.
The agreement between the PolitiFact experts and the crowd judgments. From left to right: C6 aggregated with mean; C6 aggregated with median; C6 aggregated with majority vote. First row: E6 to E3; second row: E6 to E2. Compare with Fig. 1
Summarizing, we can now conclude that by merging the ground truth levels we obtained a much stronger signal: the crowd can effectively detect and classify misinformation statements related to the COVID-19 pandemic.
5.3.2 Merging crowd levels
Having reported the results on merging the ground truth categories we now turn to transform the crowd labels (i.e., C6) into three (referred to as C3) and two (C2) categories. For the transformation process we rely on the approach detailed by Han et al. [23]. This approach has many advantages [23]: we can simulate the effect of having the crowd answers in a more coarse-grained scale (rather than C6), and thus we can simulate new data without running the whole experiment on MTurk again. As we did for the ground truth scale, we choose to select as target scales the two- and three- levels scale, driven by the same motivations. Having selected C6 as being the source scale, and having selected the target scales as the three- and two- level ones (C3 and C2), we perform the following experiment. We perform all the possible cutsFootnote 11 from C6 to C3 and from C6 to C2; then, we measure the internal agreement (using α and Φ) both on the source and on the target scale, and we compare those values. In such a way, we are able to identify, among all the possible cuts, the cut which leads to the highest possible internal agreement.
We found that, for the C6 to C3 transformation, both for α and Φ there is a single cut which leads to higher agreement levels with the original C6 scale. On the contrary, for the C6 to C2 transformation, we found that there is a single cut for α which leads to similar agreement levels as in the original C6 scale, and there are no cuts with such a property when using Φ. Having identified the best possible cuts for both transformations and for both agreement metrics, we now measure the external agreement between the crowd and the expert judgments, using the selected cut.
Figure 3 shows such a result when considering the judgments aggregated with the mean function. As we can see from the plots, it is again the case that the median values of the boxplots is always increasing, for all the transformations. Nevertheless, inspecting the plots we can state that the overall external agreement appears to be lower than the one shown in Fig. 1. Moreover, we can state that even using these transformed scales the categories pants-on-fire and false are still not separable. Summarizing, we show that it is feasible to transform the judgments collected on a C6 level scale into two new scales, C3 and C2, obtaining judgments with a similar internal agreement as the original ones, and with a slightly lower external agreement with the expert judgments.
Comparison with E6. C6 to C3 (first two plots) and to C2 (last two plots), then aggregated with the mean function. Best cut selected according to α (fist and third plot) and Φ (second and fourth plot). Compare with Fig. 1
5.3.3 Merging both ground truth and crowd levels
It is now natural to combine the two approaches. Figure 4 shows the comparison between C6 transformed into C3 and C2, and E6 transformed into E3 and E2. As we can see form the plots, also in this case the median values of the boxplots are increasing, especially for the E3 case (first two plots). Furthermore, the external agreement with the ground truth is present, even if for the E2 case (last two plots) the classes appear to be not separable. Summarizing, all these results show that it is feasible to successfully combine the aforementioned approaches, and transform into a three- and two-level scale both the crowd and the expert judgments.
5.4 RQ3: Worker background and bias
To address RQ3, we study if the answers to questionnaire and CRT test have any relation with workers quality. Previous work have shown that political and personal biases as well as cognitive abilities have an impact on the workers quality [27, 51]; recent articles have shown that the same effect might apply also to fake news [60]. For this reason, we think it is reasonable to investigate if workers’ political biases and cognitive abilities influence their quality in the setting of misinformation related to COVID-19.
When looking at the questionnaire answers, we found a relation with the workers quality only when considering the answer to the workers political views (see B for questions and answers). In more detail, using Accuracy (i.e., the fraction of exactly classified statements), we measured the quality of workers in each group. The number and fraction of correctly classified statements are however rather crude measures of worker’s quality, as small misclassification errors (e.g, pants-on-fire in place of false) are as important as more striking ones (e.g., pants-on-fire in place of true). Therefore, to measure the ability of workers to correctly classify the statements, we also compute the Closeness Evaluation Measure (CEMORD), an effectiveness metric recently proposed for the specific case of ordinal classification [1] (see Roitero et al. [51, Sect. 3.3] for a more detailed discussion of these issues). The accuracy and CEMORD values are respectively of 0.13 and 0.46 for “Very conservative”, 0.21 and 0.51 for “Conservative”, 0.20 and 0.50 for “Moderate”, 0.16 and 0.50 for “Liberal”, and 0.21 and 0.51 for “Very liberal”. By looking at both Accuracy and CEMORD, it is clear that “Very conservative” workers provide lower quality labels. The Bonferroni corrected two tailed t-test on CEMORD confirms that “Very conservative” workers perform statistically significantly worse than both “Conservative” and “Very liberal” workers. The workers’ political views affect the CEMORD score, even if in a small way and mainly when considering the extremes of the scale. An initial analysis of the other answers to the questionnaire (not shown) does not seem to provide strong signals; a more detailed analysis is left for future work.
We also investigated the effect of the CRT tests on the worker quality. Although there is a small variation in both Accuracy and CEMORD (not shown), this is never statistically significant; it appears that the number of correct answers to the CRT tests is not correlated with worker quality. We leave for future work a more detailed study of this aspect.
5.5 RQ4: Worker behavior
We now turn to RQ4, and analyze the behavior of the workers.
5.5.1 Time and queries
Table 1 (fist two rows) shows the amount of time spent on average by the workers on the statements and their CEMORD score. As we can see, the time spent on the first statement is considerably higher than on the last statements, and overall the time spent by the workers almost monotonically decreases while the statement position increases. This, combined with the fact that the quality of the assessment provided by the workers (measured with CEMORD) does not decrease for the last statements is an indication of a learning effect: the workers learn how to assess truthfulness in a faster way.
We now turn to queries. Table 1 (third and fourth row) shows query statistics for the 100 workers which finished the task. As we can see, the higher the statement position, the lower the number of queries issued: 3.52% on average for the first statement down 2.30% for the last statement. This can indicate the attitude of workers to issue fewer queries the more time they spend on the task, probably due to fatigue, boredom, or learning effects. Nevertheless, we can see that on average, for all the statement positions each worker issues more than one query: workers often reformulate their initial query. This provides further evidence that they put effort in performing the task and that suggests the overall high quality of the collected judgments. The third row of the table shows the number of times the worker used as query the whole statement. We can see that the percentage is rather low (around 13%) for all the statement positions, indicating again that workers spend effort when providing their judgments.
5.5.2 Exploiting worker signals to improve quality
We have shown that, while performing their task, workers provide many signals that to some extent correlate with the quality of their work. These signals could in principle be exploited to aggregate the individual judgments in a more effective way (i.e., giving more weight to workers that possess features indicating a higher quality). For example, the relationships between worker background/bias and worker quality (Section 5.4) could be exploited to this aim.
We thus performed the following experiment: we aggregated C6 individual scores, using as aggregation function a weighted mean, where the weights are either represented by the political views, or the number of correct answers to CRT, both normalized in [0.5,1]. We found a very similar behavior to the one observed for the second plot of Figure 1; it seems that leveraging quality-related behavioral signals, like questionnaire answers or CRT scores, to aggregate results does not provide a noticeable increase in the external agreement, although it does not harm.
5.6 RQ5: Sources of information
We now turn to RQ5, and analyze the sources of information used by the workers while performing the task.
5.6.1 URL analysis
Figure 5 shows on the left the distribution of the ranks of the URL selected as evidence by the worker when performing each judgment. URLs selected less than 1% times are filtered out from the results. As we can see from the plot, about 40% of workers selected the first result retrieved by our search engine, and selected the remaining positions less frequently, with an almost monotonic decreasing frequency (rank 8 makes the exception). We also found that 14% of workers inspected up to the fourth page of results (i.e., rank= 40). The breakdown on the truthfulness PolitiFact categories does not show any significant difference.
Figure 5 shows on the right part the top 10 of websites from which the workers choose the URL to justify their judgments. Websites with percentage ≤ 3.9% are filtered out. As we can see from the table, there are many fact check websites among the top 10 URLs (e.g., snopes: 11.79%, factcheck 6.79%). Furthermore, medical websites are present (cdc: 4.29%). This indicates that workers use various kind of sources as URLs from which they take information. Thus, it appears that they put effort in finding evidence to provide a reliable truthfulness judgment.
5.6.2 Justifications
As a final result, we analyze the textual justifications provided, their relations with the web pages at the selected URLs, and their links with worker quality. Fifty-four percent of the provided justifications contain text copied from the web page at the URL selected for evidence, while 46% do not. Furthermore, 48% of the justification include some “free text” (i.e., text generated and written by the worker), and 52% do not. Considering all the possible combinations, 6% of the justifications used both free text and text from web page, 42% used free text but no text from the web page, 48% used no free text but only text from web page, and finally 4% used neither free text nor text from web page, and either inserted text from a different (not selected) web page or inserted part of the instructions we provided or text from the user interface.
Concerning the preferred way to provide justifications, each worker seems to have a clear attitude: 48% of the workers used only text copied from the selected web pages, 46% of the workers used only free text, 4% used both, and 2% of them consistently provided text coming from the user interface or random internet pages.
We now correlate such a behavior with the workers quality. Figure 6 shows the relations between different kinds of justifications and the worker accuracy. The plots show the absolute value of the prediction error on the left, and the prediction error on the right. The figure shows if the text inserted by the worker was copied or not from the web page selected; we performed the same analysis considering if the worker used or not free text, but results where almost identical to the former analysis. As we can see from the plot, statements on which workers make less errors (i.e., where x-axis = 0) tend to use text copied from the web page selected. On the contrary, statements on which workers make more errors (values close to 5 in the left plot, and values close to +/− 5 in the right plot) tend to use text not copied from the selected web page. The differences are small, but it might be an indication that workers of higher quality tend to read the text from selected web page, and report it in the justification box. To confirm this result, we computed the CEMORD scores for the two classes considering the individual judgments: the class “copied” has CEMORD = 0.640, while the class “not copied” has a lower value, CEMORD = 0.600.
We found that such behavior is consistent for what concerns the usage of free text: statements on which workers make less errors tend to use more free text than the ones that make more errors. This is an indication that workers which add free text as a justification, possibly reworking the information present in the selected URL, are of a higher quality. In this case the CEMORD measure confirms that the two classes are very similar: the class free text has CEMORD = 0.624, while the class not free text has CEMORD = 0.621.
By looking at the right part of Fig. 6 we can see that the distribution of the prediction error is not symmetrical, as the frequency of the errors is higher on the positive side of the x-axis ([0,5]). These errors correspond to workers overestimating the truthfulness value of the statement (with 5 being the result of labeling a pants-on-fire statement as true). It is also noticeable that the justifications containing text copied from the selected URL have a lower rate of errors in the negative range, meaning that workers which directly quote the text avoid underestimating the truthfulness of the statement.
6 Variation of the judgments over time
To perform repeated observations of the crowd annotating misinformation (i.e., doing a longitudinal study) with different sets of workers, we re-launched the HITs of the main experiment three subsequent times, each of them one month apart. In this section we detail how the data was collected (Section 6.1) and the findings derived from our analysis to answer RQ6 (Section 6.2), RQ7 (Section 6.3), and RQ8 (Section 6.4).
6.1 Experimental setting
The longitudinal study is based on the same dataset (see Section 4.1) and experimental setting (see Section 4.2) of the main experiment. The crowdsourcing judgments were collected as follows. The data for main experiment (from now denoted with Batch1) has been collected on May 2020. On June 2020 we re-launched the HITs from Batch1 with a novel set of workers (i.e., we prevented the workers of Batch1 to perform the experiment again); we denote such set of data with Batch2. On July 2020, we collected an additional batch: we re-launched the HITs from Batch1 with novice workers (i.e., we prevented the workers of Batch1 and Batch2 to perform the experiment again); we denote such set of data with Batch3. Finally, on August 2020, we re-launched the HITs from Batch1 for the last time, preventing workers of previous batches to perform the experiment, collecting the data for Batch4. Then, we considered an additional set of experiments: for a given batch, we contacted the workers from previous batches sending them a $0.01 bonus and asking them to perform the task again. We obtained the datasets detailed in Table 2 where BatchXfromY denotes the subset of workers that performed BatchX and had previously participated in BatchY. Note that an experienced (returning) worker who does the task for the second time gets generally a new HIT assigned, i.e., a HIT different from the performed originally; we have no control on this matter, since HITs are assigned to workers by the MTurk platform. Finally, we also considered the union of the data from Batch1, Batch2, Batch3, and Batch4; we denote this dataset with Batchall.
6.2 RQ6: Repeating the experiment with novice workers
6.2.1 Worker background, behavior, bias, and abandonment
We first studied the variation in the composition of the worker population across different batches. To this aim, we considered a general linear mixture model (GLMM) [32] together with the analysis of variance (ANOVA) [35] to analyze how worker behavior changes across batches, and measured the impact of such changes. In more detail, we considered the ANOVA effect size ω2, an unbiased index used to provide insights of the population-wide relationship between a set of factors and the studied outcomes [14,15,16, 50, 70]. With such setting, we fitted a linear model with which we measured the effect of the age, school, and all other possible answers to the questions in the questionnaire (B) w.r.t. individual judgment quality, measured as the absolute distance between the worker judgments and the expert one with the mean absolute error (MAE). By inspecting the ω2 index, we found that while all the effects are either small or non-present [40], the largest effects are provided by workers’ answers to the taxes and southern border questions. We also found that the effect of the batch is small but not negligible, and is on the same order of magnitude of the effect of other factors. We also computed the interaction plots (see for example [20]) considering the variation of the factors from the previous analysis on the different batches. Results suggest a small or not significant [13] interaction between the batch and all the other factors. This analysis suggests that, while the difference among different batches is present, the population of workers which performed the task is homogeneous, and thus the different dataset (i.e., batches) are comparable.
Table 3 shows the abandonment data for each batch of the longitudinal study, indicating the amount of workers which completed, abandoned, or failed the task (due to failing the quality checks). Overall, the abandonment ratio is quite well balanced across batches, with the only exception of Batch3, that shows a small increase in the amount of workers which failed the task; nevertheless, such small variation is not significant and might be caused by a slightly lower quality of workers which started Batch3. On average, Table 3 shows that 31% of the workers completed the task, 50% abandoned it, and 19% failed the quality checks; these values are aligned with previous studies (see [51]).
6.2.2 Agreement across batches
We now turn to study the quality of both individual and aggregated judgments across the different batches. Measuring the correlation between individual judgments we found rather low correlation values: the correlation between Batch1 and Batch2 is of ρ = 0.33 and τ = 0.25, the correlation between Batch1 and Batch3 is of ρ = 0.20 and τ = 0.14, between Batch1 and Batch4 is of ρ = 0.10 and τ = 0.074; the correlation between Batch2 and Batch3 is of ρ = 0.21 and τ = 0.15, between Batch2 and Batch4 is of ρ = 0.10 and τ = 0.085; finally, the correlation values between Batch3 and Batch4 is of ρ = 0.08 and τ = 0.06.
Overall, the most recent batch (Batch4) is the batch which achieves the lowest correlation values w.r.t. the other batches, followed by Batch3. The highest correlation is achieved between Batch1 and Batch2. This preliminary result suggest that it might be the case that the time-span in which we collected the judgments of the different batches has an impact on the judgments similarity across batches, and batches which have been launched in time-spans close to each other tend to be more similar than other batches.
We now turn to analyze the aggregated judgments, to study if such relationship is still valid when individual judgments are aggregated. Figure 7 shows the agreement between the aggregated judgments of Batch1, Batch2, Batch3, and Batch4. The plot shows in the diagonal the distribution of the aggregated judgments, in the lower triangle the scatterplot between the aggregated judgments of the different batches, and in the upper triangle the corresponding ρ and τ correlation values. The plots show that the correlation values of the aggregated judgments are greater than the ones measured for individual judgments. This is consistent for all the batches. In more detail, we can see that the agreement between Batch1 and Batch2 (ρ = 0.87, τ = 0.68) is greater than the agreement between any other pair of batches; we also see that the correlation values between Batch1 and Batch3 is similar to the agreement between Batch2 and Batch3. Furthermore, it is again the case the Batch4 achieves lower correlation values with all the other batches.
Overall, these results show that (i) individual judgments are different across batches, but they become more consistent across batches when they are aggregated; (ii) the correlation seems to show a trend of degradation, as early batches are more consistent to each other than more recent batches; and (iii) it also appears that batches which are closer in time are also more similar.
6.2.3 Crowd accuracy: external agreement
We now analyze the external agreement, i.e., the agreement between the crowd collected labels and the expert ground truth. Figure 8 shows the agreement between the PolitiFact experts (x-axis) and the crowd judgments (y-axis) for Batch1, Batch2, Batch3, Batch4, and Batchall; the judgments are aggregated using the mean.
Agreement between the PolitiFact experts and crowd judgments. From left to right, top to bottom: Batch1 (same as second boxplot in Fig. 1), Batch2, Batch3, Batch4, and Batchall
If we focus on the plots we can see that, overall, the individual judgments are in agreement with the expert labels, as shown by the median values of the boxplots, which are increasing as the ground truth truthfulness level increases. Nevertheless, we see that Batch1 and Batch2 show clearly higher agreement level with the expert labels than Batch3 and Batch4. Furthermore, as already noted in Fig. 1, it is again the case that for all the aggregation functions the pants-on-fire and false categories are perceived in a very similar way by the workers; this again suggests that workers have clear difficulties in distinguishing between the two categories. If we look at the plots we see that within each plot the median values of the boxplots are increasing when going from pants-on-fire to true (i.e., going from left to right of the x-axis of each chart), with the exception of Batch3 and in a more evident way Batch4. This indicates that, overall, the workers are in agreement with the PolitiFact ground truth and that this is true when repeating the experiment at different time-spans. Nevertheless, there is an unexpected behavior: the data for the batches is collected across different time-spans; thus, it seems intuitive that the more time passes, the more the workers should be able to recognize the true category of each statements (for example by seeing it online or reported on the news). Figure 8 however tells a different story: it appears that the more the time passes, the less agreement we found between the crowd collected labels and the experts ground truth. This behavior can be caused by many factors, which are discussed in the next sections. Finally, by looking the last plot of Fig. 8, we see that Batchall show a behavior which is similar to Batch1 and Batch2, indicating that, apart from the pants-on-fire and false categories, the median values of the boxplots are increasing going from left to right of the x-axis of each chart, thus indicating that also in this case the workers are in agreement with the PolitiFact ground truth.
From previous analysis we observed differences in how the statements are evaluated across different batches; to investigate if the same statements are ordered in a consistent way over the different batches, we computed the ρ, τ, and rank-biased overlap (RBO) [67] correlation coefficient between the scores aggregated using using the mean as aggregation function, among batches, for the PolitiFact categories. We set the RBO parameter such as the top-5 results get about 85% of weight of the evaluation [67]. Table 4 shows such correlation values. The upper part of Table 4 shows the ρ and τ correlation scores, while the bottom part of the table shows the bottom- and top-heavy RBO correlation scores. Given that statements are sorted by their aggregated score in a decreasing order, the top-heavy version of RBO emphasize the agreement on the statements which are mis-judged for the pants-on-fire and false categories; on the contrary, the bottom-heavy version of RBO emphasize the agreement on the statements which are mis-judged for the true category.
As we can observe by inspecting Tables 4 and 5, there is a rather low agreement between how the same statements are judged across different batches, both when considering the absolute values (i.e., when considering ρ), and their relative ranking (i.e., when considering both τ and RBO). If we focus on the RBO metric, we see that in general the statements which are mis-judged are different across batches, with the exceptions of the ones in the false category for Batch1 and Batch2 (RBO top-heavy = 0.85), and the ones in the true category, again for the same two batches (RBO bottom-heavy = 0.92). This behavior holds also for statements which are correctly judged by workers: in fact we observe a RBO bottom-heavy correlation value of 0.81 for false and a RBO top-heavy correlation value of 0.5 for true. This is another indication of the similarities between Batch1 and Batch2.
6.2.4 Crowd accuracy: internal agreement
We now turn to analyze the quality of the work of the crowd by computing the internal agreement (i.e., the agreement among workers) for the different batches. Table 6 shows the agreement between the the agreement measured with α [26] and Φ [7] for the different batches. The lower triangular part of the table shows the correlation measured using ρ, the upper triangular part shows the correlation obtained with τ. To compute the correlation values we considered the α and Φ values on all PolitiFact categories; for the sake of computing the correlation values on Φ we considered only the mean value and not the upper 97% and lower 3% confidence intervals. As we can see from the Table 6, the highest correlation values are obtained between Batch1 and Batch3 when considering α, and between Batch1 and Batch2 when considering Φ. Furthermore, we see that Φ leads to obtain in general lower correlation values, especially for Batch4, which shows a correlation value of almost zero with the others batches. This is an indication that Batch1 and Batch2 are the two most similar batches (at least according to Φ), and that the other two batches (i.e., Batch3) and especially Batch4, are composed of judgments made by workers with different internal agreement levels.
6.2.5 Worker behavior: time and queries
Analyzing the amount of time spent by the workers for each position of the statement in the task, we found a confirmation of what already found in Section 5.5; the amount of time spent on average by the workers on the first statements is considerably higher than the time spent on the last statements, for all the batches. This is a confirmation of a learning effect: the workers learn how to assess truthfulness in a faster way as they spend time performing the task. We also found that as the number of batch increases, the average time spent on all documents decreases substantially: for the four batches the average time spent on each document is respectively of 222, 168, 182, and 140 s. Moreover, we performed a statistical test between each pair of batches and we found that each comparison is significant, with the only exception of Batch2 when compared against Batch3; such decreasing time might indeed be a cause for the degradation in quality observed while the number of batch increases: if workers spend on average less time on each document, it is plausible to assume they spend less time in thinking before assessing the truthfulness judgment for each document, or they spend less time on searching for an appropriate and relevant source of evidence before assessing the truthfulness of the statement.
In order to investigate deeper the cause for such quality decrease in recent batches, we inspect now the queries done by the workers for the different batches. By inspecting the number of queries issued we found that the trend to use a decreasing number of queries as the statement position increases is still present, although less evident (but not in a significant way) for Batch2 and Batch3. Thus, we can still say that the attitude of workers to issue fewer queries the more time they spend on the task holds, probably due to fatigue, boredom, or learning effects.
Furthermore, it is again the case that on average, for all the statement positions, each worker issues more than one query: workers often reformulate their initial query. This provides further evidence that they put effort in performing the task and suggests an overall high quality of the collected judgments.
We also found that only a small fraction of queries (i.e., less than 2% for all batches) correspond to the statement itself. This suggests that the vast majority of workers put significant effort into the task of writing queries, which we might assume is an indication of their willingness to perform a high quality work.
6.2.6 Sources of information: URL analysis
Figure 9 shows the rank distributions of the URLs selected as evidence by the workers when performing each judgment. As for Fig. 5, URLs selected less than 1% of the times are filtered out from the results. As we can see from the plots, the trend is similar for Batch1 and Batch2, while Batch3 and Batch4 display a different behavior. For Batch1 and Batch2 about 40% of workers select the first result retrieved by the search engine, and select the results down the rank less frequently: about 30% of workers from Batch2 and less than 20% of workers from Batch3 select the first result retrieved by the search engine. We also note that the behavior of workers from Batch3 and Batch4 is more towards a model where the user clicks randomly on the retrieved list of results; moreover, the spike which occurs in correspondence of the ranks 8, 9, and 10 for Batch4 can be caused by the fact that workers from such batch scroll directly down the user interface with the aim of finishing the task as fast as possible, without putting any effort in providing meaningful sources of evidence.
To provide further insights on the observed change in the worker behavior associated with the usage of the custom search engine, we now investigate the sources of information provided by the workers as justification for their judgments. Investigating the top 10 websites from which the workers choose the URL to justify their judgments we found that, similarly to Fig. 5, it is again the case that there are many fact check websites among the top 10 URLs: snopes is always the top ranked website, and factcheck is always present within the ranking. The only exception is Batch4, in which each fact-checking website appears in lower rank positions. Furthermore, we found that medical websites such as cdc are present only in two batches out of four (i.e., Batch1 and Batch2) and that the Raleigh area news website wral is present in the top positions in all batches apart from Batch3: this is probably caused by the location of workers which is different among batches and they use different sources of information. Overall, such analysis confirms that workers tend to use various kind of sources as URLs from which they take information, confirming that it appears that they put effort in finding evidence to provide reliable truthfulness judgments.
As further analysis we investigated the amount of change in the URLs as retrieved by our custom search engine, in particular focusing on the inter- and intra-batch similarity. To do so, we performed the following. We selected the subset of judgments for which the statement is used as a query; we can not consider the rest of the judgments because the difference in the URLs retrieved is caused by the different query issued. To be sure that we selected a representative and unbiased subset of workers, we measured the MAE of the two population of workers (i.e., the ones which used the statements as query and the ones who do not); in both cases the MAE is almost the same: 1.41 for the former case and 1.46 for the latter. Then, for each statement, we considered all possible pair of workers which used the statement as a query. For each pair we measured, considering the top 10 URLs retrieved, the overlap among the lists of results; to do so, we considered three different metrics: the rank-based fraction of documents which are the same on the two lists, the number of elements in common between the two lists, and RBO. We obtained a number in the [0,1] range, indicating the percentage of overlapping URLs between the two workers. Note that since the query issued is the same for both workers, the change in the ranked list returned is only caused by some internal policy of the search engine (e.g., to consider the IP of the worker which issued the query, or load balancing policies). When measuring the similarities between the lists, we considered both the complete URL, or the domain only; we focus on the latter option: in this way if an article moved for example from the landing page of a website to another section of the same website we are able to capture such behavior. The findings are consistent also when considering the full URL. Then, in order to normalize for the fact that the same queries can be issued by a diffident number of workers, we computed the average of the similarity scores for each statement among all the workers. Note that this normalization process is optional and findings do not change. After that, we computed the average similarity score for the three metrics; we found that the similarity of lists of the same batch is greater than the similarity of the lists from different batches; in the former case we have similarity scores of respectively 0.45, 0.64, and 0.72, while in the latter 0.14, 0.42, and 0.49.
6.2.7 Justifications
We now turn to the effect of using different kind of justifications on the worker accuracy, as done in the main analysis. We analyze the textual justifications provided, their relations with the web pages at the selected URLs, and their links with worker quality.
Figure 10 shows the relations between different kinds of justifications and the worker accuracy, as done for Fig. 6. The plots show the prediction error for each batch, calculated at each point of difference between expert and crowd judgments. The plots show if the text inserted by the worker was copied or not from the selected web page. As we can see from the plots, while Batch1 and Batch2 are very similar, Batch3 and Batch4 present important differences. As we can see from the plots, statements on which workers make less errors (i.e., where x −axis = 0) tend to use text copied from the web page selected. On the contrary, statements on which workers make more errors (i.e., values close to +/− 5) tend to use text not copied from the selected web page. We can see that overall workers of Batch3 and Batch4 tend to make more errors than workers from Batch1 and Batch2. As it was for Fig. 6 the differences between the two group of workers are small, but it might be an indication that workers of higher quality tend to read the text from selected web page, and report it in the justification box. By looking at the plots we can see that the distribution of the prediction error is not symmetrical, as the frequency of the errors is higher on the positive side of the x-axis ([0,5]) for Batch1, Batch2, and Batch3; Batch4 shows a different behavior. These errors correspond to workers overestimating the truthfulness value of the statements. We can see that the right part of the plot is way higher for Batch3 with respect to Batch1 and Batch2, confirming that workers of Batch3 are of a lower quality.
6.3 RQ7: Analysis or returning workers
In this section, we study the effect of returning workers on the dataset, and in particular we investigate if workers which performed the task more than one time are of higher quality than the workers which performed the task only once.
To investigate the quality of returning workers, we performed the following. We considered each possible pair of datasets where the former contains returning workers and the latter contains workers which performed the task only once. For each pair, we considered only the subset of HITs performed by returning workers. For such set of HITs, we compared the MAE e CEM scores of the two sets of workers.
Figure 11 shows on the x-axis the four batches, on the y-axis the batch containing returning workers (“2f1” denotes Batch2from1, and so on), each value representing the difference in MAE (first plot) and CEM (second plot); the cell is colored green if the returning workers have a higher quality than the workers which performed the task once, red otherwise. As we can see from the plot, the behavior is consistent across the two metrics considered. Apart from few cases involving Batch4 (and with a small difference), it is always the case that returning workers have similar or higher quality than the other workers; this is more evident when the reference batch is Batch3 or Batch4 and the returning workers are either from Batch1 or Batch2, indicating the high quality of the data collected for the first two batches. This is somehow an expected result and reflects the fact that people gain experience by doing the same task over time; in other words, they learn from experience. At the same time, we believe that such a behavior is not to be taken for granted, especially in a crowdsourcing setting. Another possible thing that could have happened is that returning workers focused on passing the quality checks in order to get the reward without caring about performing the task well; our findings show that this is not the case and that our quality checks are well designed.
We also investigated the average time spent on each statement position for all the batches. We found that the average time spent for Batch2from1 is 190 s (was 169 s for Batch2), 199 s for Batch3from1or2 (was 182 s for Batch3), and 213 s for Batch4from1or2or3 (was 140 s for Batch4). Overall, the returning workers spent more time on each document with respect to the novice workers of the corresponding batch. We also performed a statistical test between of each pair of batches of new and returning workers and we found statistical significance (p < 0.05) in 12 tests out of 24.
6.4 RQ8: Qualitative analysis of misjudged statements
To investigate if the statements which are mis-judged by the workers are the same across all batches, we performed the following analyses. We sorted, for each PolitiFact category, the statements according to their MAE (i.e., the absolute difference between the expert and the worker label), and we investigated if such ordering is consistent across batches; in other words, we investigated if the most mis-judged statement is the same across different batches. Figure 12 shows, for each PolitiFact category, the relative ordering of its statements sorted according to decreasing MAE (the document with rank 1 is the one with highest MAE). From the plots we can manually identify some statements which are consistently mis-judged for all the PolitiFact categories. In more detail, those statements are the following (sorted according to MAE): for pants-on-fire: S2, S8, S7, S5, S1; for false: S18, S14, S11, S12, S17; for mostly-false: S21, S22, S25; for half-true: S31, S37, S33; for mostly-true: S41, S44, S42, S46; for true: S60, S53, S59, S58.
We manually inspected the selected statements to investigate the cause of failure. We manually checked the justifications for the 24 selected statements.
For all statements analyzed, most of the errors in Batch3 and Batch4 are given by workers who answered randomly, generating noise. Answers were categorized as noise when the following two criteria were met: (1) the chosen URL is unrelated to the statement (e.g. a Wikipedia page defining the word “Truthfulness” or a website to create flashcards online); (2) the justification text provides no explanation for the truthfulness value chosen (neither personal nor copied from a URL which is different from the selected one). We found that noisy answers become more frequent with every new batch and account for almost all the errors in Batch4. In fact, the number of judgments with a noisy answer for the four batches are respectively 27, 42, 102, and 166; conversely, the number of non-noisy answers for the four batches are respectively 159, 166, 97, and 54. The non-noise errors in Batch1, Batch2 and Batch3 seem to depend on the statement. By manually inspecting the justifications provided by the workers we identified the following main reasons of failure in identifying the correct label.
-
In four cases (S53, S41, S25, S14), the statements were objectively difficult to evaluate. This was because they either required extreme attention to the detail in the medical terms used (S14), they were an highly debated points (S25), or required knowledge of legislation (S53).
-
In four cases (S42, S46, S59, S60), the workers were not able to find relevant information, so they decided to guess. The difficulty in finding information was justified: the statements were either too vague to find useful information (S59), others had few official data on the matter (S46) or the issue had already been solved and other news on the same topic had taken its place, making the web search more difficult (S60, S59, S42) (e.g. truck drivers had trouble getting food in fast food restaurants, but the issue was solved and news outlets started covering the new problem “lack of truck drivers to restock supermarkets and fast food chains”).
-
In four cases (S33, S37, S59, S60), the workers retrieved information which covered only part of the statement. Sometimes this happened by accident (S60, information on Mardi Gras 2021 instead of Mardi Gras 2020) or because the workers recovered information from generic sites, which allowed them to prove only part of the claim (S33, S37).
-
In four cases (S2, S8, S7, S1), pants-on-fire statements were labeled as true (probably) because they had been actually stated by the person. In this cases the workers used a fact-checking site as the selected URL, sometimes even explicitly writing that the statement was false in the justification, but selected true as label.
-
In thirteen cases (S7, S8, S2, S18, S22, S21, S33, S37, S31, S42, S44, S58, S60), the statements were deemed as more true (or more false) than they actually were by focusing on part of the statement or reasoning on how plausible they sounded. In most of the cases the workers found a fact-checking website which reported the ground truth label, but they decided to modify their answer based on their personal opinion. True statements from politics were doubted (S60, about nobody suggesting to cancel Mardi Gras) and false statements were excused as exaggerations used to frame the gravity of the moment (S18, about church services not resuming until everyone is vaccinated).
-
In five cases (S1, S5, S17, S12, S11), the statements were difficult to prove/disprove (lack of trusted articles or test data) and they reported concerning information (mainly on how the coronavirus can be transmitted and how long it can survive). Most of the workers retrieved fact-checking articles which labeled the statements as false or pants-on-fire, but they chose an intermediate rating. In these cases, the written justifications contained personal opinions or excerpts from the selected URL which instilled some doubts (e.g., tests being not definitive enough, lack of knowledge on the behavior of the virus) or suggested it is safe to act under the assumption we are in the worst-case-scenario (e.g., avoid buying products from China, leave packages in the sunlight to try and kill the virus).
Following the results from the failure analysis, we removed the worst individual judgments (i.e., the ones with noise) according to the failure analysis; we found that the effect on aggregated judgments is minimal, and the resulting boxplots are very similar to the ones obtained in Fig. 1 without removing the judgments.
We investigated how the correctness of the judgments was correlated to the attributes of the statement (namely position, claimant and context) and the passing of time. We computed the absolute distance from the correct truthfulness value for each judgment in a batch and then aggregated the values by statement, obtaining the mean absolute error (MAE) and standard deviation (STD) for each statement. For each batch we sorted the statements in descending order according to MAE and STD, we selected the top-10 statements and analyzed their attributes. When considering the position (of the statement in the task), the wrong statements are spread across all positions, for all the batches; thus, this attribute does not have any particular effect. When considering the claimant and the context we found that most of the wrong statements have Facebook User as claimant, which is also the most frequent source of statements in our dataset. To investigate the effect of time we plotted the MAE of each statement against the time passed from the day the statement was made to the day it was evaluated by the workers. This was done for all the batches of novice workers (Batch1 to Batch4) and returning workers (Batch2from1, Batch3from1or2, Batch4from1or2or3). As Fig. 13 shows, the trend of MAE for each batch (dotted lines) is similar for all batches: statements made in April (leftmost ones for each batch) have more errors than the ones made at the beginning of March and in February (rightmost ones for each batch), regardless of how much time has passed since the statement was made. Looking at the top part of Fig. 13, we can also see that the MAE tends to grow with each new batch of workers (black dashed trend line). The previous analyses suggest that this is probably not an effect of time, but of the decreasing quality of the workers. This is also suggested by the lower part of the figure, which shows that MAE tends to remain stable in time for returning workers (which were shown to be of higher quality). We can also see that the trend of every batch remains the same for returning workers: statements made in April and at end of March keep being the most difficult to assess. Overall, the time elapsed since the statement was made seems to have no impact on the quality of the workers’ judgments.
MAE (aggregated by statement) against the number of days elapsed (from when the statement was made to when it was evaluated), for novice workers (top) and returning workers (bottom). Each point is the MAE of a single statement in a Batch. Dotted lines are the trend of MAE in time for the Batch, straight lines are the mean MAE for the Batch. The black dashed line is the global trend of MAE across all Batches
7 Discussion and conclusions
7.1 Summary
This work presents a comprehensive investigation of the ability and behavior of crowd workers when asked to identify and assess the veracity of recent health statements related to the COVID-19 pandemic. The workers performed a task consisting of judging the truthfulness of 8 statements using our customized search engine, which allows us to control worker behavior. We analyze workers background and bias, as well as workers cognitive abilities, and we correlate such information to the worker quality. We repeat the experiment in four different batches, each of them a month apart, with both novice/new and experienced/returning workers. We publicly release the collected data to the research community.
The answers to our research questions can be summarized as follows. We found evidence that the workers are able to detect and objectively categorize online (mis)information related to the COVID-19 pandemic (RQ1). We found that while the agreement among workers does not provide a strong signal, aggregated workers judgments show high levels of agreement with the expert labels, with the only exception of the two truthfulness categories at the lower end of the scale (pants-on-fire and false). We found that both crowdsourced and expert judgments can be transformed and aggregated to improve label quality (RQ2). We found that, although the effectiveness of workers is slightly correlated with their answers to the questionnaire, this is never statistically significant (RQ3).
We exploited the relationship between the workers background / bias and the workers quality in order to improve the effectiveness of the aggregation methods used on individual judgments, but we found that it does not provide a noticeable increase in the external agreement (RQ4). However, we believe that such signals may effectively inform new ways of aggregating crowd judgments and we plan to further address such topic in future work by using more complex methods. We found that workers use multiple sources of information, and they consider both fact-checking and health-related websites. We also found interesting relations between the justifications provided by the workers and the judgment quality (RQ5).
Considering the longitudinal study, we found that re-collecting all the data at different time-spans has a major effect on the quality of the judgments, both when considering novice (RQ6) and experienced (RQ7) workers. When considering RQ6, we found that early batches produced by novice workers are more consistent to each other than more recent batches. Also, batches which are closer in time to each other are more similar in terms of workers’ quality. Novice workers also put effort into the task to look for evidence using different sources of information and to write queries, since they often reformulate it. When considering RQ7, we found that experienced/returning workers spend more time on each statement w.r.t. novice workers in the corresponding batch. Also, experienced workers have similar or higher quality w.r.t. to other workers. We also found that as the number of batch increases, the average time spent on all documents decreases substantially. Finally, we provided a extensive analysis of features and peculiarities of the statements that are misjudged by the crowd-workers, across all datasets (RQ8). We found that the time elapsed since the statement was made seems to have to impact on the quality of the workers’ judgments.
We also remark that within our work we aim to study two different phenomenons: (i) how novice workers address truthfulness of COVID-19-related news over time and (ii) how returning workers address the truthfulness of the same set of news after some time. Our hypothesis is that with the passage of time workers became more aware of the truthfulness of COVID-19-related news. We found that this does not hold when considering a set of novice workers. This result is in line with other works [44]. Nevertheless, a batch launched considering only returning workers leads to an increase in agreement, showing how workers tend to learn by experience. We also found that returning workers did not focus only on passing the quality checks, thus confirming the high quality of collected data. Therefore, we expect to see an increase in quality over time by running an additional batch with returning workers.
7.2 Practical implications
From our analysis we can derive the following remarks which can be helpful in practice.
-
Crowd workers are able to detect and objectively categorize online (mis)information related to the COVID- 19 pandemic; thus researchers can make use of crowdsourcing to detect online (mis)information related to the COVID-19.
-
Researchers should not rely on the agreement among workers, which we found does not provide a strong signal.
-
Researchers should use the arithmetic mean as aggregation function, as it provides with a high level of agreement with the expert labels, and be aware that the two truthfulness categories at the lower end of the scale (i.e., pants-on-fire and false) are being evaluated very similarly from crowd workers.
-
Researches can transform and aggregate the labels to improve label quality if they aim to maximize the agreement with expert labels.
-
Researchers should not rely on questionnaire answers, which we found are not a proxy for worker quality; in particular, we found that workers background / bias is not helpful to increase the quality of the aggregated judgements.
-
The quality checks implemented in the task are helpful to obtain high quality data. The usage of a custom search engine stimulates workers to use multiple sources of information and report what they think are good sources to explain their label.
-
There is a major effect on the quality of the judgments if they are collected for the same documents at multiple time-spans; batches which are closer in time to each other are more similar in terms of workers’ quality, and experienced/returning workers have generally a higher quality than novice workers. Thus, if the aim of the researcher is to maximize the agreement with expert labels, s/he should rely on experienced workers.
-
Researchers should expect the labelling quality to be very depending on statement features and peculiarities: there are statements which are objectively difficult to evaluate; statements for which there is a little or no information will be of a lower quality; and workers might focus only on part of the statement/source of information to give a particular truthfulness label, so asking for a specific textual justification might help in increasing to quality of the labels.
7.3 Limitations
There are a few potential limitations in this study that could be addressed in future research. One issue is the relatively low amount of returning workers, due to the very nature of crowdsourcing platforms. Furthermore, having a single statement evaluated by 10 distinct workers only does not guarantee a strong statistical power, and thus an experiment with a larger worker sample might be required to draw definitive conclusions and reduce the standard deviation of our findings.
Another limitation of this study is that we only consider the final label assigned by PolitiFact experts to the statement. Instead, according to publicly available information about the PolitiFact assessment process [29, 41, 42] each statement is rated by three editors and a reporter, who come up with a consensus for the final judgment. Although such information is not publicly released, we are currently working to have access granted to it, for PolitiFact and other fact-checking datasets: this would allow, for example, a more detailed comparison of the disagreement between workers and the ground truth.
In this paper we employ only statements sampled from the PolitiFact dataset. To generalize our findings, a comparison with multiple datasets is needed. We plan to address that in future work by reproducing our longitudinal study using statements verified by other fact-checking organizations, e.g., statements indexed by Google Fact Check Explorer.Footnote 12
7.4 Future work
Although this study is a first step in the direction of targeting misinformation in real time, we are not there yet. Probably a more complex approach, combining automatic machine learning classifiers, the crowd, and a limited number of experts can lead to a solution. Indeed, in future work we plan to investigate how to combine our crowdsourcing methodology with machine learning to assist fact-checking experts in a human-in-the-loop process [10], by extending information access tools such as FactCatch [39] or Watch ‘n’ Check [6].
Furthermore, it is interesting in future to use the findings from this work to implement a rating or flagging mechanism to be used in social media, in such a way that it allows users to evaluate the truthfulness of statements. This is a complex task which will require a discussion about ethical aspects such as possible abuses from opposing groups of people as well as dealing with under-represented minorities and non genuine behaviors derived from outnumbering.
Another interesting future work consists in taking advantage of the geographical data of the crowd workers. As we stated in Section 4.2, we restricted the access to the task to US workers. In the work detailed in this paper we did not implement any policy to track the specific geographical location of the workers (for example, by doing a reverse IP lookup), nor we asked for further sensible personal information such that the workers ethnicity. While these data could be leveraged to correlate the workers quality with the geo-political situation of their state, such decision to track or ask for such data poses additional issues to be addressed as this policy may go against the workers will to not be tracked.
Concerning practical usage of the crowd labels collected an interesting future direction comprises the use of such labels to automate truthfulness assessment via machine learning techniques. Some work propose various approaches to study news attributes to determine whether such news are fake or not [24, 45] including a very recent work which focuses on using news titles and body [56] to this end. Another approach is using both artificial intelligence and human work to combat fake news by means of a hybrid human-AI framework [10]. Query terms and justification texts provided by workers of high quality can potentially be leveraged to train a machine learning model and build a set of fact-checking query terms.
Another interesting future work consists in repeating the longitudinal study on other crowdsourcing platforms, since [44] show that experiments replicated across different platforms result in significantly different data quality levels.
Finally, we believe that further (cross-disciplinary) work is needed to better understand how theories studied in social, psycholinguistic, and cognitive science may explain our empirical findings.
Notes
This paper is an extended version of the work by Roitero et al. [52]. In an attempt of providing a uniform, comprehensive, and more understandable account of our research, we also report in the first part of the paper the main results already published in [52]. Conversely, all the results on the longitudinal study in the second half of the paper are novel.
Before deploying the task on MTurk, we investigated the average time spent to complete the task, and we related it to the minimum US hourly wage.
Workers provide proof that they are based in USA and have the eligibility to work.
C6 can be transformed into C3 in 10 different ways, and C6 can be transformed into C2 in 5 different ways.
References
Amigó E, Gonzalo J, Mizzaro S, Carrillo-de Albornoz J (2020) An effectiveness metric for ordinal classification: formal properties and experimental results. In: Proceedings of the 58th Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics, Association for Computational Linguistics, Online, ACL ’20, pp 3938–3949. https://doi.org/10.18653/v1/2020.acl-main.363
Atanasova P, Nakov P, Màrquez L, Barrón-Cedeño A, Karadzhov G, Mihaylova T, Mohtarami M, Glass J (2019) Automatic fact-checking using context and discourse information. Journal of Data and Information Quality 11(3). https://doi.org/10.1145/3297722
Atkinson K, Baroni P, Giacomin M, Hunter A, Prakken H, Reed C, Simari G, Thimm M, Villata S (2017) Towards artificial argumentation. AI Mag 38(3):25–36. https://doi.org/10.1609/aimag.v38i3.2704
Barrón-Cedeno A, Elsayed T, Nakov P, Da San Martino G, Hasanain M, Suwaileh R, Haouari F, Babulkov N, Hamdan B, Nikolov A et al (2020) Overview of checkthat! 2020: automatic identification and verification of claims in social media. In: International Conference of the Cross-Language Evaluation Forum for European Languages, Springer, CLEF ’20, pp 215–236
Bullock J, Luccioni A, Pham KH, Lam CSN, Luengo-Oroz MA (2020) Mapping the landscape of artificial intelligence applications against COVID-19. 2003.11336
Cerone A, Naghizade E, Scholer F, Mallal D, Skelton R, Spina D (2020) Watch ‘n’ Check: towards a social media monitoring tool to assist fact-checking experts. In: 2020 IEEE International Conference on Data Science and Advanced Analytics (DSAA), pp 607–613. https://doi.org/10.1109/DSAA49011.2020.00085
Checco A, Roitero K, Maddalena E, Mizzaro S, Demartini G (2017) Let’s agree to disagree: fixing agreement measures for Crowdsourcing. In: Dow S, Kalai A T (eds) Proceedings of the Fifth AAAI Conference on Human Computation and Crowdsourcing, HCOMP 2017, 23-26 October 2017. AAAI Press, Québec City, Québec, Canada, pp 11–20
Chen X, Sin SCJ, Theng YL, Lee CS (2015) Why students share misinformation on social media: motivation, gender, and study-level differences. The Journal of Academic Librarianship 41(5):583–592. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acalib.2015.07.003
Cinelli M, Quattrociocchi W, Galeazzi A, Valensise CM, Brugnoli E, Schmidt AL, Zola P, Zollo F, Scala A (2020) The COVID-19 Social Media Infodemic. 2003.05004
Demartini G, Mizzaro S, Spina D (2020) Human-in-the-loop artificial intelligence for fighting online misinformation: challenges and opportunities. Bulletin of the IEEE Computer Society Technical Committee on Data Engineering 43(3):65–74. http://sites.computer.org/debull/A20sept/p65.pdf
Desai A, Warner J, Kuderer N, Thompson M, Painter C, Lyman G, Lopes G (2020) Crowdsourcing a crisis response for COVID-19 in oncology. Nature Cancer 1(5):473–476. https://doi.org/10.1038/s43018-020-0065-z
Elsayed T, Nakov P, Barrón-Cedeño A, Hasanain M, Suwaileh R, Da San Martino G, Atanasova P (2019) Overview of the CLEF-2019 CheckThat! Lab: automatic identification and verification of claims. In: International Conference of the Cross-Language Evaluation Forum for European Languages, Springer, CLEF ’19, pp 301–321
Embretson SE (1996) Item response theory models and spurious interaction effects in factorial ANOVA designs. Appl Psychol Meas 20(3):201–212. https://doi.org/10.1177/014662169602000302
Ferro N, Silvello G (2016) A general linear mixed models approach to study system component effects. In: Proceedings of the 39th International ACM SIGIR Conference on Research and Development in Information Retrieval, Association for Computing Machinery, New York, NY, USA, SIGIR ’16, pp 25–34. https://doi.org/10.1145/2911451.2911530
Ferro N, Silvello G (2018) Toward an anatomy of IR system component performances. Journal of the Association for Information Science and Technology 69(2):187–200. https://doi.org/10.1002/asi.23910
Ferro N, Kim Y, Sanderson M (2019) Using collection shards to study retrieval performance effect sizes. ACM Transactions on Information Systems 37(3). https://doi.org/10.1145/3310364
Flaxman S, Goel S, Rao JM (2016) Filter bubbles, echo chambers, and online news consumption. Public Opinion Quarterly 80 (S1):298–320. https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfw006, https://academic.oup.com/poq/article-pdf/80/S1/298/17120810/nfw006.pdf
Frederick S (2005) Cognitive reflection and decision making. J Econ Perspect 19(4):25–42. https://doi.org/10.1257/089533005775196732
Gallotti R, Valle F, Castaldo N, Sacco P, Domenico MD (2020) Assessing the risks of “infodemics” in response to covid-19 epidemics. 2004.03997
de Haan JR, Wehrens R, Bauerschmidt S, Piek E, Schaik RCv, Buydens LMC (2006) Interpretation of ANOVA models for microarray data using PCA. Bioinformatics 23(2):184–190. https://doi.org/10.1093/bioinformatics/btl572
Han L, Roitero K, Gadiraju U, Sarasua C, Checco A, Maddalena E, Demartini G (2019) All those wasted hours: on task abandonment in crowdsourcing. In: Proceedings of the Twelfth ACM International Conference on Web Search and Data Mining, Association for Computing Machinery, New York, NY, USA, WSDM ’19, pp 321–329. https://doi.org/10.1145/3289600.3291035
Han L, Roitero K, Gadiraju U, Sarasua C, Checco A, Maddalena E, Demartini G (2019) The impact of task abandonment in crowdsourcing. IEEE Trans Knowl Data Eng 1(01):1–1. https://doi.org/10.1109/TKDE.2019.2948168
Han L, Roitero K, Maddalena E, Mizzaro S, Demartini G (2019) On transforming relevance scales. In: Proceedings of the 28th ACM International Conference on Information and Knowledge Management, Association for Computing Machinery, New York, NY, USA, CIKM ’19, pp 39–48. https://doi.org/10.1145/3357384.3357988
Horne B, Adali S (2017) This just in: fake news packs a lot in title, uses simpler, repetitive content in text body, more similar to satire than real news. Proceedings of the International AAAI Conference on Web and Social Media 11(1). https://ojs.aaai.org/index.php/ICWSM/article/view/14976
Kim J, Kim D, Oh A (2019) Homogeneity-based transmissive process to model true and false news in social networks. In: Proceedings of the Twelfth ACM International Conference on Web Search and Data Mining, Association for Computing Machinery, New York, NY, USA, WSDM ’19, pp 348–356. https://doi.org/10.1145/3289600.3291009
Krippendorff K (2011) Computing Krippendorff’s alpha- reliability. http://repository.upenn.edu/asc_papers/43
La Barbera D, Roitero K, Demartini G, Mizzaro S, Spina D (2020) Crowdsourcing truthfulness: the impact of judgment scale and assessor bias. In: Jose JM, Yilmaz E, Magalhães J, Castells P, Ferro N, Silva MJ, Martins F (eds) Advances in information retrieval: proceedings of the European Conference on Information Retrieval, Springer International Publishing, Cham, ECIR ’20, pp 207–214
Lawrence J, Reed C (2020) Argument mining: a survey. Computational Linguistics 45 (4):765–818. https://doi.org/10.1162/coli_a_00364, https://direct.mit.edu/coli/article-pdf/45/4/765/1847520/coli_a_00364.pdf
Lim C (2018) Checking how fact-checkers check. Research & Politics 5(3):2053168018786848. https://doi.org/10.1177/2053168018786848
Maddalena E, Mizzaro S, Scholer F, Turpin A (2017) On crowdsourcing relevance magnitudes for information retrieval evaluation. ACM Trans Inf Syst 35(3). https://doi.org/10.1145/3002172
Maddalena E, Roitero K, Demartini G, Mizzaro S (2017) Considering assessor agreement in IR evaluation. In: Proceedings of the ACM SIGIR International Conference on Theory of Information Retrieval, Association for Computing Machinery, New York, NY, USA, ICTIR ’17, pp 75–82. https://doi.org/10.1145/3121050.3121060
McCullagh P (2018) Generalized Linear Models. Routledge
Mejova Y, Kalimeri K (2020) Advertisers Jump on Coronavirus Bandwagon: Politics, News, and Business. 2003.00923
Mihaylova T, Karadzhov G, Atanasova P, Baly R, Mohtarami M, Nakov P (2019) SemEval-2019 Task 8: fact checking in community question answering forums. In: Proceedings of the 13th International Workshop on Semantic Evaluation, Association for Computational Linguistics, Minneapolis, Minnesota, USA, SemEval-2019, pp 860–869. https://doi.org/10.18653/v1/S19-2149
Morrison DF (2005) Multivariate analysis of variance. Encyclopedia of Biostatistics 5
Moskowitz H (1977) Magnitude estimation: notes on what, how, when, and why to use it. J Food Qual 1(3):195–227
Nakov P, Barrón-Cedeño A, Elsayed T, Suwaileh R, Màrquez L, Zaghouani W, Atanasova P, Kyuchukov S, Da San Martino G (2018) Overview of the CLEF-2018 CheckThat! Lab on automatic identification and verification of political claims. In: Bellot P, Trabelsi C, Mothe J, Murtagh F, Nie JY, Soulier L, SanJuan E, Cappellato L, Ferro N (eds) Experimental IR Meets Multilinguality, Multimodality, and Interaction. Springer International Publishing, Cham, pp 372–387
Nguyen CT (2020) Echo chambers and epistemic bubbles. Episteme 17 (2):141–161. https://doi.org/10.1017/epi.2018.32
Nguyen TT, Weidlich M, Yin H, Zheng B, Nguyen QH, Nguyen QVH (2020) FactCatch: incremental pay-as-you-go fact checking with minimal user effort. In: Proceedings of the 43rd International ACM SIGIR Conference on Research and Development in Information Retrieval, Association for Computing Machinery, New York, NY, USA, SIGIR ’20, pp 2165–2168. https://doi.org/10.1145/3397271.3401408
Olejnik S, Algina J (2003) Generalized eta and omega squared statistics: measures of effect size for some common research designs. Psychological Methods 8(4):434
Politifact (2021) Politifact IFCN principles. https://ifcncodeofprinciples.poynter.org/profile/politifact, (accessed: 20.04.2021)
Politifact (2021) The principles of the Truth-O-Meter: PolitiFact’s methodology for independent fact-checking. https://www.politifact.com/article/2018/feb/12/principles-truth-o-meter-politifacts-methodology-i/#Truth-O-Meter%20ratings, (accessed: 20.04.2021)
Popat KK (2019) Credibility analysis of textual claims with explainable evidence. PhD thesis, Saarland University. https://doi.org/10.22028/D291-30005
Qarout R, Checco A, Demartini G, Bontcheva K (2019) Platform-related factors in repeatability and reproducibility of crowdsourcing tasks. Proceedings of the AAAI Conference on Human Computation and Crowdsourcing 7(1):135–143. https://ojs.aaai.org/index.php/HCOMP/article/view/5264
Rashkin H, Choi E, Jang JY, Volkova S, Choi Y (2017) Truth of varying shades: analyzing language in fake news and political fact-checking. In: Proceedings of the 2017 Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing, Association for Computational Linguistics. Denmark, Copenhagen, pp 2931–2937, DOI https://doi.org/10.18653/v1/D17-1317, (to appear in print)
Roberts K, Alam T, Bedrick S, Demner-Fushman D, Lo K, Soboroff I, Voorhees E, Wang LL, Hersh WR (2020) TREC-COVID: rationale and structure of an information retrieval shared task for COVID-19. Journal of the American Medical Informatics Association https://doi.org/10.1093/jamia/ocaa091
Roitero K, Demartini G, Mizzaro S, Spina D (2018) How many truth levels? six? one hundred? even more? validating truthfulness of statements via Crowdsourcing. In: Proceedings of the CIKM Workshop on Rumours and Deception in Social Media (RDSM’18)
Roitero K, Maddalena E, Demartini G, Mizzaro S (2018) On fine-grained relevance scales. In: The 41st International ACM SIGIR Conference on Research & Development in Information Retrieval, Association for Computing Machinery, New York, NY, USA, SIGIR ’18, pp 675–684. https://doi.org/10.1145/3209978.3210052
Roitero K, Maddalena E, Demartini G, Mizzaro S (2018) On fine-grained relevance scales. In: The 41st International ACM SIGIR Conference on Research & Development in Information Retrieval, Association for Computing Machinery, New York, NY, USA, SIGIR ’18, pp 675–684. https://doi.org/10.1145/3209978.3210052
Roitero K, Carterrete B, Mehrotra R, Lalmas M (2020) Leveraging behavioral heterogeneity across markets for cross-market training of recommender systems. In: Companion Proceedings of the Web Conference 2020, Association for Computing Machinery, New York, NY, USA, WWW ’20, pp 694–702. https://doi.org/10.1145/3366424.3384362
Roitero K, Soprano M, Fan S, Spina D, Mizzaro S, Demartini G (2020) Can the crowd identify misinformation objectively? the effects of judgment scale and assessor’s background. In: Proceedings of the 43rd International ACM SIGIR Conference on Research and Development in Information Retrieval, Association for Computing Machinery, New York, NY, USA, SIGIR ’20, pp 439–448. https://doi.org/10.1145/3397271.3401112
Roitero K, Soprano M, Portelli B, Spina D, Della Mea V, Serra G, Mizzaro S, Demartini G (2020) The COVID-19 infodemic: can the crowd judge recent misinformation objectively? In: Proceedings of the 29th ACM International Conference on Information and Knowledge Management, Association for Computing Machinery, New York, NY, USA, CIKM ’20, pp 1305–1314. https://doi.org/10.1145/3340531.3412048
Sethi R, Rangaraju R (2018) Extinguishing the backfire effect: using emotions in online social collaborative argumentation for fact checking. In: 2018 IEEE International conference on web services (ICWS), vol 1, pp 363–366. https://doi.org/10.1109/ICWS.2018.00062
Sethi RJ (2017) Spotting fake news: a social argumentation framework for scrutinizing alternative facts. In: 2017 IEEE International Conference on Web Services (ICWS), pp 866–869. https://doi.org/10.1109/ICWS.2017.108
Sethi RJ, Rangaraju R, Shurts B (2019) Fact checking misinformation using recommendations from emotional pedagogical agents. In: International Conference on Intelligent Tutoring Systems, Springer, pp 99–104
Shrestha A, Spezzano F (2021) Textual characteristics of news title and body to detect fake news: a reproducibility study. In: Hiemstra D, Moens M, Mothe J, Perego R, Potthast M, Sebastiani F (eds) Advances in Information Retrieval - 43rd European Conference on IR Research, ECIR 2021, Virtual Event, March 28 - April 1, 2021, Proceedings, Part II, Springer, Lecture Notes in Computer Science, vol 12657, pp 120–133. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-72240-1_9
Snaith M, Lawrence J, Pease A, Reed C (2020) A modular platform for argument and dialogue. In: 8Th international conference on computational models of argument, COMMA 2020, pp 473–474
Tangcharoensathien V, Calleja N, Nguyen T, Purnat T, D’Agostino M, Garcia-Saiso S, Landry M, Rashidian A, Hamilton C, AbdAllah A, Ghiga I, Hill A, Hougendobler D, van Andel J, Nunn M, Brooks I, Sacco PL, De Domenico M, Mai P, Gruzd A, Alaphilippe A, Briand S (2020) Framework for managing the covid-19 infodemic: Methods and results of an online, crowdsourced who technical consultation. J Med Internet Res 22(6):e19659. https://doi.org/10.2196/19659
Tchechmedjiev A, Fafalios P, Boland K, Gasquet M, Zloch M, Zapilko B, Dietze S, Todorov K (2019) ClaimsKG: a knowledge graph of fact-checked claims. In: Ghidini C, Hartig O, Maleshkova M, Svátek V, Cruz I, Hogan A, Song J, Lefranćois M, Gandon F (eds) The semantic web – ISWC, vol 2019. Springer International Publishing, Cham, pp 309–324
Times TNY (2021) The real story about fake news is partisanship. https://www.nytimes.com/2017/01/11/upshot/the-real-story-about-fake-news-is-partisanship.html, (accessed: 20.04.2021)
Toulmin SE (1958) The uses of argument. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge
Törnberg P (2018) Echo chambers and viral misinformation: Modeling fake news as complex contagion. PLoS One 13(9):e0203958
Tsai JY, Phua J, Pan S, Cc Yang (2020) Intergroup contact, covid-19 news consumption, and the moderating role of digital media trust on prejudice toward asians in the united states: Cross-sectional study. J Med Internet Res 22(9):e22767. https://doi.org/10.2196/22767
Visser J, Lawrence J, Reed C (2020) Reason-checking fake news. Commun ACM 63 (11):38–40. https://doi.org/10.1145/3397189
Wang WY (2017) “Liar, Liar Pants on Fire”: a new benchmark dataset for fake news detection. In: Proceedings of the 55th Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics (Volume 2: Short Papers), Association for Computational Linguistics, Vancouver, Canada, ACL ’17, pp 422–426. https://doi.org/10.18653/v1/P17-2067
Wani A, Joshi I, Khandve S, Wagh V, Joshi R (2021) Evaluating deep learning approaches for covid19 fake news detection. In: Chakraborty T, Shu K, Bernard HR, Liu H, Akhtar MS (eds) Combating online hostile posts in regional languages during emergency situation. Springer International Publishing, Cham, pp 153–163
Webber W, Moffat A, Zobel J (2010) A similarity measure for indefinite rankings. ACM Trans Inf Syst 28(4). https://doi.org/10.1145/1852102.1852106
Will FL (1960) The uses of argument. The Philosophical Review 69(3):399–403. http://www.jstor.org/stable/2183556
Yang KC, Torres-Lugo C, Menczer F (2020) Prevalence of Low-Credibility Information on Twitter During the COVID-19 Outbreak. 2004.14484
Zampieri F, Roitero K, Culpepper JS, Kurland O, Mizzaro S (2019) On topic difficulty in ir evaluation: the effect of systems, corpora, and system components. In: Proceedings of the 42nd International ACM SIGIR Conference on Research and Development in Information Retrieval, Association for Computing Machinery, New York, NY, USA, SIGIR’19, pp 909–912. https://doi.org/10.1145/3331184.3331279
Zubiaga A, Ji H (2014) Tweet, but verify: epistemic study of information verification on twitter. Social Network Analysis and Mining 4(1):1–12
Funding
Open access funding provided by Università degli Studi di Udine within the CRUI-CARE Agreement. This work is partially supported by a Facebook Research award, by the Australian Research Council (DP190102141 and DE200100064), by a MISTI – MIT International Science and Technology Initiatives – Seed Fund (MIT-FVG Seed Fund Project) and by the project HEaD – Higher Education and Development - 1619942002 / 1420AFPLO1 (Region Friuli – Venezia Giulia). We thank the reviewers which provided insightful and detailed remarks that helped to improve the quality of the paper.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Appendices
Appendix
The appendices consist of the statements used in our crowdsourcing experiments (A), the questionnaire provided to workers (B), and the Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT) (C).
Appendix A Statements
Appendix B Questionnaire
-
Q1:
What is your age range?
-
A1:
0–18
-
A2:
19–25
-
A3:
26–35
-
A4:
36–50
-
A5:
50–80
-
A6:
80+
-
A1:
-
Q2:
What is the highest level of school you have completed or the highest degree you have received?
-
A1:
High school incomplete or less,
-
A2:
High school graduate or GED (includes technical/vocational training that doesn’t towards college credit)
-
A3:
Some college (some community college, associate’s degree)
-
A4:
Four year college degree/bachelor’s degree
-
A5:
Some postgraduate or professional schooling, no postgraduate degree
-
A6:
Postgraduate or professional degree, including master’s, doctorate, medical or law degree
-
A1:
-
Q3:
Last year what was your total family income from all sources, before taxes?
-
A1:
Less than 10,000
-
A2:
10,000 to less than 20,000
-
A3:
20,000 to less than 30,000
-
A4:
30,000 to less than 40,000
-
A5:
40,000 to less than 50,000
-
A6:
50,000 to less than 75,000
-
A7:
75,000 to less than 100,000
-
A8:
100,000 to less than 150,000
-
A9:
150,000 or more
-
A1:
-
Q4:
In general, would you describe your political views as
-
A1:
Very conservative
-
A2:
Conservative
-
A3:
Moderate
-
A4:
Liberal
-
A5:
Very liberal
-
A1:
-
Q5:
In politics today, do you consider yourself a
-
A1:
Republican
-
A2:
Democrat
-
A3:
Independent
-
A4:
Something else
-
A1:
-
Q6:
Should the U.S. build a wall along the southern border?
-
A1:
Agree
-
A2:
Disagree
-
A3:
No opinion either way
-
A1:
-
Q7:
Should the government increase environmental regulations to prevent climate change?
-
A1:
Agree
-
A2:
Disagree
-
A3:
No opinion either way
-
A1:
Appendix C Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT)
-
CRT1:
If three farmers can plant three trees in three hours, how long would it take nine farmers to plant nine trees?
-
Correct Answer: 3 hours
-
Intuitive Answer: 9 hours
-
-
CRT2:
Sean received both the 5th highest and the 5th lowest mark in the class. How many students are there in the class?
-
Correct Answer: 9 students
-
Intuitive Answer: 10 students
-
-
CRT3:
In an athletics team, females are four times more likely to win a medal than males. This year the team has won 20 medals so far. How many of these have been won by males?
-
Correct Answer: 4 medals
-
Intuitive Answer: 5 medals
-
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Roitero, K., Soprano, M., Portelli, B. et al. Can the crowd judge truthfulness? A longitudinal study on recent misinformation about COVID-19. Pers Ubiquit Comput 27, 59–89 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00779-021-01604-6
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00779-021-01604-6
Keywords
- Information behavior
- Crowdsourcing
- Misinformation
- COVID-19