Introduction

The co-occurrence and interdependence of changes in physiological reaction across interaction partners is called physiological synchronization [Ellamil et al. (2016); Palumbo et al. (2017), PS;], also referred to as empathic resonance or linkage. This cross-reactivity among group members or dyads has emerged as an important phenomenon to consider when investigating social processes. However, the mechanisms of PS have yet to be determined fully (Shamay-Tsoory et al. 2019; for an interesting theory, see Koban et al. 2019). An important question in this regard is under which circumstances PS happens (Gvirts and Perlmutter 2020). Here, type of relationship (Bizzego et al. 2020; Konvalinka et al. 2011), physical and social presence (Azhari et al. 2020; Järvelä et al. 2016), shared attitudes (Wróbel and Królewiak 2017), social context (Danyluck and Page-Gould 2019), shared movement (Gordon et al. 2020), and autism (McNaughton and Redcay 2020) have all been suggested to contribute to the phenomenon of synchronization across interaction partners.

In the current manuscript, we want to expand on this research and investigate the influence of acute stress on PS. While there have been some findings highlighting the importance of emotional state (Coutinho et al. 2019), e.g., whether acute stress actually increases or decreases PS remains unclear. To investigate this, here, we examine whether PS emerges in situations with strangers who have minimal interaction, and are even prevented from direct social contact with each other. For this purpose, we reanalyze a data set in which participants underwent a laboratory stressor [the Trier Social Stress Test for groups (TSST-G)], or a non-stressful control task together.

The TSST is a standardized paradigm to reliably elicit an endocrine stress response in a laboratory setting (Kirschbaum et al. 1993). The TSST-G (von Dawans et al. 2011) is similarly effective in stimulating cortisol release, but was designed to be more efficient by inducing stress in groups of up to six participants simultaneously. The TSST-G is by now widely employed as a successful laboratory stressor for groups (for a qualitative investigation, see Vors et al. 2018). During the TSST-G, participants are typically prohibited from visual contact. However, as part of the TSST-G participants complete two different oral presentation tasks, during which they listen to their fellow group members’ performances. Previous research has shown that tone of voice might indicate stress to others, and might be one pathway to PS [Dimitroff et al. (2017); Prochazkova and Kret (2017)].

If PS is present in our sample, and influenced by stressor exposure, this would indicate that PS emerges in situations which have previously not been regarded as promoting PS, the extent of which potentially varying based on psychological and physiological states. In addition to providing insights into possible mechanisms behind PS, our reanalysis can thus inform researchers employing the TSST-G on the possibility that PS is affecting individual participants’ stress responses.

In the original study, we exposed participants to the TSST-G or a non-stressful control task and measured cortisol and self-rated stress reactivity seven times to depict the change in physiological and subjective stress levels. For this reanalysis, we hypothesized that cortisol and alpha-amylase responses to the TSST-G within a group are not independent, i.e., that participants’ endocrine stress responses are at least partly explained by their group members’ cortisol trajectories. We also examine the influence of the stressor on this relationship, by comparing it to a non-stressful control condition. Finally, we explore group members’ influence on self-rated stress.

Methods

To investigate cortisol and alpha-amylase synchronization during the TSST-G, we reanalyzed data from a larger project in which the TSST-G was employed and compared to a non-stressful control task. In our analysis, we made use of the information about which participants were tested together in one testing session (hereafter referred to as group). Additional details about the data collection as well as the aim of the larger project have been published previously (Popovic et al. 2020).

Sample

N = 146 young healthy adults were recruited for the study. Of these, eight participants had to be excluded due to data loss or unclear group membership. Thus, the final sample in this analysis consisted of N = 138 participants (65 female, mean age = 23.48, and SD age = 3.99), with 75 participants (54.3%) in the TSST-G and 63 participants in the control condition. Participants were tested in 44 groups (mean group size = 3.14 participants, range 2–4; SD = 0.77). Testing took place between 9 am and 5 pm (median = 2 pm). Time of day was statistically controlled for to account for the circadian rhythm of the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis (Miller et al. 2016). Participants’ age, initial cortisol and alpha-amylase values, gender distribution, hormonal contraception intake, and group size did not differ between groups in the TSST-G condition compared to groups in the control condition (see Popovic et al. 2020, \(p >.05\)).

Procedure

In the beginning of the experiment, participants gave written informed consent and subsequently filled out questionnaires. This anticipation phase took about 15 min. Afterward, participants were randomly assigned to a stress condition using the TSST-G or to a control condition. Following the experimental manipulation of stress levels, participants stayed together as a group, but worked independently on subsequent non-stressful tasks of risk perception which are not part of the current research question. The overall duration of the experiment was 85 min (for details, refer to Popovic et al. 2020).

TSST-G and control task

Stress was induced using a slightly modified version of the TSST-G (see Popovic et al. 2020). The modifications made the protocol more feasible for testing groups of up to four participants. Participants were introduced to the task as a group. After a 5-min preparation phase, participants entered a room with two confederates acting in a non-emotional, non-supporting manner throughout the stress task, which included a public speaking task and a mental arithmetic task for a period of 4–6 min each, per participant. During both tasks, visual barriers prevented participants from having visual contact. In the control condition, groups performed similar tasks in writing and in absence of a committee (Popovic et al. 2020). The duration of the task was 27 min and the same in both experimental conditions, independent of the number of participants in the group.

Endocrine measures

During the study, seven saliva samples were collected using Salivettes (Sarstedt AG & Co., Nümbrecht, Germany) in 12- to 15-min intervals, from 15 min before the onset of the TSST-G or control task to 45 min after. Cortisol levels (nmol/l) were determined using a time-resolved fluorescence immunoassay (Dressendörfer et al. 1992). Salivary alpha-amylase (U/ml) levels were determined using the enzyme kinetic method. The extraction of cortisol and alpha-amylase levels was performed by the biochemical laboratory of the University of Trier, Germany. For the analysis of cortisol, six additional participants had to be excluded from the study due to cortisol trajectories, suggesting that they were responding to a stressor happening prior to their arrival at the laboratory (high baseline with subsequently only declining levels) or because of very high cortisol levels throughout the entire experiment exceeding three standard deviations from the mean—see Popovic et al. (2020), for details. For the analysis of alpha-amylase, two participants had to be excluded due to all measures being zero.

Self-rated stress

To assess psychological self-rated stress levels throughout the experiment, visual analogue scales ranging from 0 indicating no stress to 10 indicating maximal stress were used. Participants provided their assessments at the same time when saliva samples were taken. Self-rated stress measures will hereafter be referred to as subjective stress.

Questionnaires

At the beginning of the experiment, participants filled out demographic questionnaires, as well as the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg 1965), and the Perceived Stress Scale (Cohen et al. 1983). The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale is a measure for global self-esteem. The Perceived Stress Scale measures everyday stressful experiences. Both questionnaires were included in the design due to potentially confounding effects of physiological stress markers (see, Popovic et al. 2020). However, we found no such effects in our analysis.

Statistical analysis

Cortisol synchrony

For the analysis of cortisol synchrony, we relied on a stability and influence model, a type of actor-partner interdependence model, following guidelines established by Thorson et al. (2018). In dyads, stability and influence models assess whether a partner’s (sender) previous measurement can explain variance in the other partner’s (receiver) current measurement, aka influence portion, beyond the receiver’s own previous measurement, aka stability portion (assessed through the autocorrelational structure). With each participant acting as sender as well as receiver, all possible combinations of dyads within a group were subsequently tested (for a group with participants A, B, and C, this would result in the combinations A–B, B–C, A–C, B–A, C–B, and C–A). We employed multi-level growth curve models to test for intraindividual stability and physiological synchrony. We included independent variables (fixed and random effects) stepwise into our model to predict cortisol values, using the following model equation (notation adapted from Finch and Bolin (2017)):

$$\begin{aligned} \begin{aligned}&y_{ti} ={} \beta _{00} + \beta _{10} * \mathrm {time}_{ti} + \beta _{20} * y_{(t-k)i} \\&\quad + \beta _{30} * \mathrm {condition}_i + \beta _{40} * S_{ti} \\&\quad +\beta _{50} * S_{(t-k)i} + U_{0i} + U_{1i} * \mathrm {time}_{ti} + e_{ti}, \end{aligned} \end{aligned}$$
(1)

where the dependent variable \(y_{ti}\) represents the receiver’s cortisol level, for individual i at measurement point t, with \(i = 1, ..., N\) and \(t = 0, ..., T\). \(\beta\)s represent regression coefficients. Level-1 independent variables were linear, quadratic, and cubic effects of time (time\(_t\) was the measurement point in minutes with index t), the preceding, or lagged, dependent variable \(y_{(t-k)i}\) with lag size \(k = 1\:(k = 1, 2, ..., M-1\)), with M being the total number of measurements, and the sender’s concurrent and lagged cortisol values, \(S_{ti}\) and \(S_{(t-k)i}\). In contrast to the procedure described by Thorson et al. (2018), we also included the sender’s concurrent values as predictors as we expected to see simultaneous changes in sender and receiver. The level-2 independent variable was the group’s experimental condition (TSST-G versus control task). Random effects for each participant are represented by \(U_{0i}\) (random intercept), and \(U_{1i}\) (random slopes), where \(U = {\mathcal {N}}(0, \sigma ^2_{U0})\). Random error is given by \(e_{ti} = {\mathcal {N}}(0, \sigma ^2_e)\). For simplicity of presentation, polynomial time effects, covariates, as well as interaction effects were omitted in this notation, but were included in the analyses. Level-1 independent variables were centered, except for time effects (see Enders and Tofighi 2007). We did not use orthogonal coding of polynomial effects of the time variable based on suggestions by Biesanz et al. (2004).

In each step of our analysis, a more complex model was selected when its fit was significantly increased compared to the simpler model, as determined by analyses of variance and the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC). For the final model, regression coefficients were obtained (beta-coefficients and F values), and significance was determined. Furthermore, we calculated the coefficient of determination and tested model assumptions, including homogeneity of variance and normal distribution of residuals.

To test the influence of lag size on PS, as well as for illustrative purposes, we further calculated cross-correlation functions for all pairs of participants. Cross-correlation functions indicate the relationship between two time series dependent on lag size k. If \(k \ne 0\), this means that values of one partner correlate with previous (\(k < 0\)) or future (\(k > 0\)) values of the other partner, i.e., during the interaction, one partner is lagging behind. While we only examined \(k = 0\) and \(k = 1\) in our model, significant relationships for all lag sizes \(k = 0, 1, ..., M-1\) can be visualized.

Cross-correlation coefficients (CC coefficients) can be interpreted analogous to correlation coefficients. The significance of CC coefficients can be tested by comparing them to the conventional limit for significance (CCSL; see Dean and Dunsmuir 2016).

For illustrative purposes, we calculated the area under the curve with respect to the increase (AUC\(_I\)) for each participant’s cortisol time series. The AUC\(_I\) indicates the overall increase and decrease of cortisol over time, resulting in one value per participant. Positive values indicate an overall increase from the starting point (i.e., the first measurement), while negative values indicate an overall decrease in cortisol. AUC\(_I\) was calculated according to a formula provided by Pruessner et al. (2003).

Further analyses

While we mainly focused on cortisol synchrony, we also calculated PS of alpha-amylase, and synchrony of subjective stress. For the analysis of alpha-amylase and subjective stress, respectively, the same model as for cortisol was employed. In each model, covariates were included when they were significantly correlated with the respective dependent variable and, thus, differed for models of cortisol, alpha-amylase, and subjective stress.

Software

All statistical analyses were conducted in R version 3.6.2 (R Core Team 2019) with RStudio version 1.1.463 (RStudio Team 2020). Multi-level models were calculated using the nlme package (Pinheiro et al. 2019). The final model’ coefficient of determination was calculated using the performance package (Lüdecke et al. 2020).

Results

Figure 1 displays trajectories of cortisol concentration, alpha-amylase concentration, and subjective stress levels within each experimental condition. Despite the pronounced influence of the experimental condition, there was also considerable variance within each condition. For cortisol, the variation in AUC\(_I\) values is shown in Fig. 2.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Cortisol (left), subjective stress (middle), and alpha-amylase (right) trajectories, mean, and standard errors for both experimental conditions. The gray rectangle indicates the time span of the intervention. Time = minutes since the first stress assessment.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Boxplot of AUC\(_I\) for cortisol in each experimental condition. While conditions differ significantly in overall cortisol responses, there is also pronounced variation within each condition

Cortisol synchrony

Our hypothesis regarding PS during the TSST-G was tested using a multi-level stability and influence model. For cortisol, model fit was improved, compared to a basic model, when adding a random intercept for each participant, indicating inter-individual differences in baseline cortisol (intraclass coefficient ICC = 0.72). Model fit was further improved by adding a random linear, quadratic, and cubic time effect, indicating differences in individual cortisol trajectories. By adding all fixed effects that further improved overall model fit, we arrived at our final model, for which regression coefficients and their significance were evaluated.

Here, cortisol values changed significantly over time (linear effect: \(F(1,1564) = 45.51, \:p < 0.001\); quadratic effect: \(F(1,1564) = 111.81, \:p < 0.001\); and cubic effect: \(F(1,1564) = 18.51, \:p < 0.001\)). Overall, cortisol levels were higher in the TSST-G compared to the control condition (\(F(1,129) = 8.89, \:p = 0.003\)), with cortisol trajectories differing between experimental conditions (linear effect: \(F(1,1564) = 129.84, \:p < 0.001\); quadratic effect: \(F(1,1564) = 76.43, \:p < 0.001\); and cubic effect: \(F(1,1564) = 9.55, \:p = 0.002\)). Within-person stability explained further variance in cortisol levels (\(F(1,1564) = 144.23, \:p < 0.001\)), and changed over time (linear: \(F(1,1564) = 508.87, \:p < 0.001\); quadratic: \(F(1,1564) = 126.81, \:p < 0.001\); cubic: \(F(1,1564) = 76.02, \:p < 0.001\)). Within-person stability was slightly higher in the TSST-G condition overall (\(F(1,1564) = 12.28, \:p < 0.001\)); however, within-person stability decreased during the measurement after the experimental manipulation in the TSST-G condition (linear: \(F(1,1564) = 0.68, \:p = 0.411\); quadratic: \(F(1,1564) = 11.86, \:p < 0.001\); cubic: \(F(1,1564) = 20.96, \:p < 0.001\)). When examining PS, i.e., mutual influence between group members, we found a main effect of senders’ concurrent cortisol (\(F(1,1564) = 8.30, \:p = 0.004\)), but not lagged cortisol (i.e., influence of previous values; \(F(1,1564) = 0.71, \:p = 0.400\)). Senders’ concurrent influence did not change over time (all \(p > 0.05\)), but depended on experimental condition (\(F(1,1564) = 7.28, \:p = 0.007\)), with stronger PS in the control condition. Senders’ lagged influence was also stronger in the control condition (\(F(1,1564) = 16.87, \:p < 0.001\)), and changed over time (linear effects: \(F(1,1564) = 0.55, \:p = 0.49\); quadratic effects: \(F(1,1564) = 0, \:p = 0.99\); cubic effects: \(F(1,1564) = 15.74, \:p < 0.001\)), especially in the TSST-G condition (linear effects: \(F(1,1564) = 1.4, \:p = 0.237\); quadratic effects:\(F(1,1564) = 14.94, \:p < 0.001\); cubic effects: \(F(1,1564) = 2.39, \:p = 0.122\)).

Time of day during the experiment acted as a significant covariate (\(F(1,129) = 39.05, \:p < 0.001\)), with higher cortisol values occurring earlier in the day. Gender (in combination with intake of oral contraceptives; levels: male, female with hormonal contraceptives, female without hormonal contraceptives), time since awakening on the day of the experiment, and group size were also tested as potential covariates, but did not improve model fit. The variance explained by the entire model was \(R^2 \approx .94\). In comparison to a model without including senders’ cortisol levels, about 4% more variance was explained when including PS in the model. Figure 3 provides a visualization of PS as operationalized in our model. Figure 4 shows the model fit. Table 1 shows \(\beta\)-coefficients for predictors of the cortisol model. Note that due to a different kind of testing, significance changes for some predictors.

Table 1 \(\beta\)-coefficients with corresponding t values for cortisol influence and stability model
Fig. 3
figure 3

Scatter plot of receivers’ and senders’ concurrent cortisol values (lag size \(k = 0\); A), and senders’ lagged cortisol values (lag size \(k = 1\); B) not differentiated by time point or condition. The entire model explained about 94% of the variance in the dependent variable (other independent variables not shown)

Fig. 4
figure 4

Receivers’ cortisol as predicted by our model versus measured receivers’ cortisol

Influences of lag length on synchrony between group members’ cortisol stress responses were assessed making use of cross-correlation functions. Cross-correlation coefficients are depicted in Fig. 5. While a majority of the CC coefficients indicated a non-significant relationship between participants, there were relatively many pairs of participants who showed a significant CC coefficient at lag size \(k = 0\). Some pairs of participants also showed significant negative CC coefficients.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Cross-correlation coefficients (CC coefficients) for pairwise cortisol time series by lag size k. The color indicates whether a given CC coefficient exceeded the conventional limit for significance (CCSL). CC coefficients for negative lag sizes are symmetrical to those for positive lag sizes

Alpha-amylase synchrony

The analysis of alpha-amylase synchrony was conducted analogous to the analysis of cortisol synchrony. Here, a random intercept for each participant, as well as a random linear, quadratic, and cubic slope for each participant improved model fit compared to a basic model. Like for cortisol, we arrived at a final model by adding predictors when they further improved model fit. In this final model, significant quadratic and cubic time effects indicate change in alpha-amylase levels over time (linear effects: \(F(1,1664) = 0.45, \:p = .500\); quadratic effects: \(F(1,1664) = 48.63, \:p < 0.0001\); cubic effects: \(F(1,1664) = 199.13, \:p < 0.001\)). While there was no main effect of condition (\(F(1,131) = 3.25, \:p = 0.073\)), alpha-amylase changed over time depending on condition (linear effects: \(F(1,1664) = 1.36, \:p = 0.244\); quadratic effects: \(F(1,1664) = 28.29, \:p < 0.001\); cubic effects: \(F(1,1664) = 82.50, \:p < 0.001\)). Within-person stability was significant (\(F(1,1664) = 270.48, \:p < 0.001\)), and changed over time (linear effects: \(F(1,1664) = 363.63, \:p < 0.001\); quadratic effects: \(F(1,1664) = 99.60, \:p < 0.001)\); cubic effects: \(F(1,1664) = 34.18, \:p < 0.001\)). Stability showed stronger changes over time in the TSST-G condition (linear effects: \(F(1,1664) = 32.60, \:p < 0.001\); for quadratic and cubic effects both \(p > 0.05\)). As for synchrony, there was a main effect of both concurrent (\(F(1,1664) = 9.36, \:p = 0.002\)) and lagged PS (\(F(1,1664) = 13.90, \:p < 0.001\)). Interestingly, concurrent PS interacted significantly with receivers’ lagged alpha-amylase (\(F(1,1664) = 5.31, \:p = 0.021\)), such that PS was higher in receivers who had experienced high physiological stress before (see Fig. 7). Body mass index (BMI) acted as a significant covariate in the final model (\(F(1,131) = 35.68, \:p <0.001\)), with higher BMI values associated with decreased alpha-amylase values. CC coefficients for alpha-amylase and their significance can be obtained in Fig. 6.

Fig. 6
figure 6

Cross-correlation coefficients (CC coefficients) for pairwise alpha-amylase time series by lag size k. The color indicates whether a given CC coefficient exceeded the conventional limit for significance (CCSL). CC coefficients for negative lag sizes are symmetrical to those for positive lag sizes

Exploratory analysis of emotional synchrony

When conducting a stability and influence analysis with subjective stress as the dependent variable, we found different trajectories between experimental conditions, and significant within-person stability. However, participants within the same group did not influence each other significantly, indicating no self-rated emotional synchrony. The final model, when including the covariate gender/hormonal contraception, explained \(R^2 \approx .88\) of subjective stress.

Discussion

In a reanalysis of data from a previous study (Popovic et al. 2020), our aim was to determine whether we could find signs of endocrine synchronization processes dependent on stressor exposure during a paradigm with minimal social interaction. We tested for physiological synchrony between group members exposed to the TSST-G using a stability and influence model approach by Thorson and West (2018). We found that group peers’ cortisol as well as alpha-amylase concentrations predicted an individual’s cortisol or alpha-amylase levels beyond intraindividual stability, indicating PS of both the hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis and the sympathetic nervous system. Even though the statistical models tested for the influence of the group members, this finding can be interpreted as PS. Physiological influence from one person on another has been found in different contexts before, including TSST settings (Buchanan et al. 2012). Previous studies investigating stress resonance demonstrated that the stress induced in the target of the TSST could create a resonance response in the experimenter (Buchanan et al. 2012), or an observer (Engert et al. 2014). Importantly, in the current study, the results suggest that there is bidirectionality—the model we employed defines each subject in the TSST-G as sender and receiver; thus, individual stress levels affect, and are affected by, other subjects in the vicinity. Bidirectional PS was, therefore, present despite the minimal interaction usually occurring in a TSST-G setting. Specifically, in our experimental setup, we had employed poster boards to visually separate the participants and thus minimize interference. That we were still able to identify signs of PS suggests that this is a robust phenomenon which might occur more frequently than previously thought. Possible mechanisms might be visual cues after the experimental manipulation affecting the endocrine release trajectory, or auditory stimulation (e.g., tone of voice in the TSST-G condition), or olfactory cues (Narciso et al. 2019, e.g., a smell of stress). Which of these factors exactly is responsible for synchronization cannot be assessed by our experimental design. In the future, it would be interesting to measure synchrony when sensory modalities are restricted selectively.

Interestingly, stressor exposure decreased PS of the HPA axis (cortisol PS) compared to a non-stressful control condition, even though participants in the control condition were not exposed to verbal cues from their fellow group members. PS decreased after the completion of the TSST-G (but not after the non-stressful control task). This suggests that PS in cortisol levels is even more present in non-stressful settings despite an effect on both stressful and non-stressful conditions. That would suggest that our ability to respond to, and in part synchronize with, the endocrine activity of those in our vicinity is augmented under non-stressful conditions, i.e., when our HPA axis is operating under baseline conditions. This presents an interesting path for future research as the endocrine synchronization in a non-stressful group setting is, as far as we know, rarely investigated (as is the HPA axis during baseline). It will be interesting to determine if this synchronization depends on additional factors of the group or the individual, and whether it has an effect on the performance of the individual in the group, or the group as a whole. In contrast to cortisol PS, PS of the sympathetic nervous system (alpha-amylase PS) was not influenced by stressor exposure. However, higher previous alpha-amylase in the receiver predicted stronger PS (Fig. 7). Therefore, stress might not dampen but even enhance PS in the sympathetic nervous system. However, alpha-amylase synchrony did not change over time, and might therefore be less influenced by stress in general. Divergences between responses of different stress systems have been reported in the past (Andrews et al. 2013).

Fig. 7
figure 7

The relationship between senders’ and receivers’ alpha-amylase (physiological influence or synchrony) is moderated by receivers’ own previous stress values

This finding is novel and has important repercussions for the use of the TSST-G in stress research. Depending on the experimental question, the researcher should be aware that his or her choice of experimental design will introduce additional variance in the stress responses independent of the individual’s ability to react to a psychosocial stressor. The effect of the group might be negligible if the task is used to induce stress regardless of the magnitude of the individual cortisol stress response—for example, to test a general effect of stress on a subsequent behavioral or cognitive task. If, however, the individual cortisol trajectory in response to stress is the target of the investigation, it might be advisable to switch to the form of single subject TSST, as the TSST-G might create effects that are at least in part due to group composition, and not the individual’s stress perception and processing.

As can be seen in Fig. 5, the CC coefficients for both cortisol and alpha-amylase were negative for some pairs of participants, suggesting that a higher response in the sender led to a lower response in the receiver. Anti-phase synchrony has previously been reported in parent–child dyads (Creavy et al. 2020), and in romantic partners (Reed et al. 2013). For cortisol, significant negative CC coefficients were present in ten pairs, while significant positive CC coefficients were found in 114 pairs, suggesting that it is the exception rather than the norm. All participants who showed anti-phase synchrony were tested in groups of four participants in total, i.e., the biggest group size in our sample. At this point, this is merely an interesting observation however, as statistically, using a model including all pairs of participants, controlling for group size did not improve model fit. Further research will be necessary to determine whether larger groups promote anti-phase synchrony, and if so, which mechanisms are responsible for this connection.

In contrast to physiological synchrony, we could not find emotional synchrony (also referred to as emotional contagion) in our sample. According to the Neurocognitive Model of Emotional Contagion (Prochazkova and Kret 2017), PS precedes, but does not necessarily lead to emotional synchrony. When physiological changes caused by synchronization processes are perceived in the receiver, these changes can be interpreted as emotion, leading to adapting the receiver’s emotional state to that of the sender. However, this interpretation as emotion does not necessarily take place. In our sample, subjective stress as an emotion was independent among group members.

This study has several limitations: First, the original study was not designed with the present research question in mind. Second, while our results demonstrate an interdependence between participants, there is no information about the mechanisms behind this interdependence. Rather than due to PS, interdependence could also originate from situational characteristics that group members had in common at the time of testing; however, all variables known to us (e.g., use of oral contraceptives, time of day) were controlled for in the statistical model and the influence of the group remained significant; at the same time, it is certainly possible that there were additional factors not recorded at the time of testing that influenced the results. Specifically different starting times of experimental sessions can and should be avoided in the future. Moreover, we did not assess known influences on physiological synchrony, like personal characteristics.

These limitations are contrasted by a number of definite strengths of the study: Sample size is rather large and thus allowed for testing of the more complex ‘stability and influence’ model. Both endocrine and subjective stress markers were assessed, and we took special care to test for the influence of potential confounding variables. While there is no unified method to determine PS, stability and influence models as well as related models are accepted methods which have been utilized to assess PS in the previous studies (e.g., Coutinho et al. 2019; Thorson and West 2018).

Taken together, our results show that endocrine synchronization of stress responses before, during, and after the TSST-G might affect individual measurements. We emphasize that these results should be treated as preliminary, as the current study was not designed for that purpose explicitly, and we encourage researchers to test PS under stress more rigorously in future studies. PS is an emerging field of research in the context of collective behavior and its influence on group interactions is not yet fully understood. In future studies employing the TSST-G, depending on the specific experimental design, it should be considered that the use of a group setting will create an additional effect on individuals. In such situations, researchers are encouraged to check for PS in the data, for example by the methods provided here.

In conclusion, PS can be found even in groups without direct personal interaction and might drive physiological activity more than previously expected. However, if participants are exposed to stress, the driving force of PS might lose power. Future research should investigate PS in multiple levels of interaction and under various states of stress to uncover possible mechanisms behind PS as well as influences on and effects of PS.