Introduction

Inadequate trunk stabilization has been hypothesized to contribute to low back pain (LBP) [1]. Trunk stabilization is often assessed by studying responses to mechanical perturbations. Using this approach, several studies have shown longer response delays after external mechanical perturbations of trunk posture in LBP patients than in controls [2], which has been interpreted as an impairment and a cause of the incidence and persistence of LBP [3,4,5]. In apparent contrast to impaired stabilization, higher trunk stiffness, i.e., a higher mechanical resistance to perturbations, has been reported [6], possibly due to altered trunk muscle recruitment patterns in patients compared to controls [7]. These changes have been interpreted as adaptive responses to enhance control over trunk movement and therewith prevent pain provocation [7]. In fact, increased trunk muscle activity and increased trunk stiffness could explain the longer response delays found in LBP patients. Higher muscle activity before the perturbation would render detection of reflex onsets, i.e., the onset of a change in muscle activity after the perturbation, more difficult causing overestimated delays. In addition, with increased stiffness, the same mechanical disturbance will cause a smaller and slower deviation of trunk posture. Consequently, the perturbation might be sensed later, causing a later change in excitation of trunk muscles. So paradoxically, the finding of increased response delays might not reflect a slower reflex loop, but a methodological issue or a change in the input into the reflex loop resulting from functional, adaptive responses to enhance trunk stabilization.

We have recently developed a method to assess intrinsic and reflexive contributions to trunk stabilization, which aims to circumvent the limitations described above [8,9,10]. The method was shown to have good reliability in healthy subjects and LBP patients [9]. The present study used this method to assess differences in trunk stabilization between LBP patients and healthy controls when maximally resist the perturbation or when sitting relaxed but upright, to investigate their maximal ability, natural behavior and their ability to modulate between both tasks. Based on findings of changed muscle recruitment [7] and intrinsic stiffness [6], we hypothesized that LBP patients would show lower admittance during the relax task (explained by higher intrinsic stiffness and damping) and, related to that, less modulation between the relax task and resist task. Given the methodological issues regarding onset determination in previous studies, we expected no difference in reflex delays between the groups.

Materials and methods

Participants

Forty-nine participants suffering from LBP and a control group of fifty healthy subjects participated in this study. The groups were group matched on sex and height, but the patient group was significantly older and heavier (Table 1). The patients suffered from nonspecific LBP for at least 3 months and were under treatment by physical therapists (9), pain specialists (26) and rehabilitation centers (14). The healthy participants had not experienced LBP in the 12 months prior to the experiments. None of the participants suffered from radicular pain or other (neurological) disorders or used medication that could affect balance control. All participants gave informed consent according to the guidelines of the medical ethical committee of VU Medical Center, Amsterdam (2013/37).

Table 1 Information about the patient and control group

Throughout the measurements, patients reported momentary pain six times with a BS-11 score [11]. In the week prior to the measurement, they filled in a pain diary for seven days, containing BS-11 scores in the morning, afternoon, evening, and scoring the lowest and highest pain level during the day. The LBP patients also filled in the questionnaires on illness beliefs, fear of movement, catastrophizing, depression and anxiety: the Oswestry Disability Index (ODI) [12], Back Beliefs Questionnaire (BBQ) [13], Tampa Scale for Kinesiophobia (TSK) [14], Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) [15] and Pain Catastrophizing Scale (PCS) [16]. Averages of their scores are found in Table 1. In view of high correlations between questionnaire scores, two composite scores were constructed. A pain and disability score was calculated as the average of z-normalized momentary pain scores, pain diary scores and ODI scores, and a pain-related cognition score was calculated as the average of z-normalized scores on the BBQ, TSK, HADS and PCS.

Measurements

In the experimental setup, subjects were seated in a kneeling-seated posture with their pelvis restrained (Fig. 1). Force perturbations were applied in ventral direction on the T10 spinous process by a magnetically driven linear actuator (Servotube STB2510S Forcer and Thrustrod TRB25-1380, Copley Controls, MA, USA), with a thermoplastic patch (4 × 4 cm) placed between the subject and the actuator to improve force transfer and comfort. During the trials, subjects were instructed to keep their eyes closed, arms crossed in front of the chest, and to minimize lateral excursions. Task instructions were to ‘maximally resist the perturbation’ by minimizing flexion/extension excursions (resist task), or to ‘relax but remain sitting upright’ (relax task).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Experimental setup

Each trial had a duration of 50 s and consisted of a linearly increasing force ramp of 3 s from 0 N to 60 N followed by a crested multi-sine signal [17] with a 35 N amplitude superimposed on a 60 N baseline preload.

The actuator displacement and contact force between the actuator and subject were measured, as well as surface electromyography (EMG) of the lumbar part of the bilateral M. Longissimus at level L3 and L4 recorded at 2048 samples/s (REFA, TMSi, the Netherlands). The M. Longissimus was chosen because of the high coherence between this muscle’s activity and thorax displacement [8].

System identification

Closed loop identification techniques [17] were used to describe the translational low back admittance and reflexes as frequency response functions (FRFs). The admittance FRF describes the actuator displacement as a function of the contact force, representing the resistance against the perturbation as a function of frequency. The reflex FRF describes the EMG amplitude of the lumbar part of the M. Longissimus as a function of the actuator displacement.

Coherences of admittance and reflex FRFs were assessed for the frequencies containing power in the perturbation signal. Coherence ranges from zero to one, where one reflects a perfect, noise-free relation between input and output. A coherence greater than 0.24 was considered significant with α ≤ 0.05 [18].

Because task-related modulation of the FRFs mainly occurs below the natural frequency around 1.1 Hz, and because trunk mass dominates responses at higher frequencies [8], low-frequency gains (LF gains) of the admittance and reflexive FRFs were defined by averaging over the five frequency pairs within the 0.2–1.1 Hz range.

Parametric identification

A linear neuromuscular control model was constructed to translate the FRFs into physiological elements representing intrinsic and reflexive contributions [8, 10]. Model parameters were estimated by optimizing the fit of the model transfer functions to the transfer functions estimated as described above, using simulated annealing. The effective trunk mass (m) was fitted. The stabilizing properties of passive tissues and muscle co-contraction and the destabilizing effect of trunk mass were lumped into two parameters describing the overall intrinsic stiffness and damping (k, b) of the low back. The reflexive contribution was described by position, velocity and acceleration feedback gains (kp,kv,ka) with a single reflex delay (τref). Muscle activation dynamics were implemented as a second-order system [19] with a cutoff frequency and a dimensionless damping, set to 0.75 Hz and 1.05, respectively, as the average activation dynamics in van Drunen et al. [8]. Contact dynamics between the subjects’ trunk and the actuator were estimated as a spring and damper (kc, bc).

The validity of the optimized model and its parameters was assessed in the time domain using the variance accounted for (VAF). A VAF of 100% reflects a perfect description of the measured signal by the model at the input frequencies.

For more detailed information on the perturbation characteristics, data recording, data processing, system identification and parametric identification, see van Drunen et al. [8] and van Dieën et al. [10].

Statistics

The FRF gains and LF gains were log-transformed to satisfy the assumption of normality. To test our hypotheses, nonparametric LF gains and parametric parameters were compared between tasks and groups with generalized estimating equations (GEE) with LBP, task, task*LBP, age and LBP*age as predictors. In this analysis, age was dichotomized based on the median age of all participants. Preliminary checks were conducted to ensure validity of the assumption of normality, linearity, homogeneity of variances and homogeneity of regression slopes. Body mass was not used as a covariate, since effective mass is accounted for in the parametric estimation.

Linear correlation analyses were performed to test the relation between trunk stabilization parameters, pain and disability and pain-related cognitions. Pearson’s R ranges from 0–1 where < 0.3 indicates a ‘poor’ correlation, 0.3–0.5 represents a ‘moderate’ correlation and > 0.5 represents a ‘strong’ correlation [20].

For all tests, results with p-values smaller than 0.05 (two-sided) were considered significant. Processing, system identification, parametric identification and statistical analyses of the data were performed using MATLAB (The Mathworks, Inc., Natick, MA, USA).

Results

Group-level comparison between LBP patients and controls

Mean admittance coherence was high (average and standard deviation: 0.93 ± 0.05), indicating good input–output correlations, while the mean EMG coherence (0.73 ± 0.10) was good considering the noisy character of EMG signals.

A significant main effect of LBP was found on LF admittance, with as hypothesized, lower admittance in LBP patients than in healthy subjects (Figs. 2, 3 and Table 2). In contrast to our hypotheses, damping and stiffness did not differ between groups (Fig. 4). Position, velocity and acceleration reflex gains, however, were significantly higher in LBP patients. The hypothesized larger modulation in healthy controls compared to LBP patients (task*LBP interaction) was only seen in velocity reflex gains, and a trend was seen in admittance. As expected, there was no significant difference in reflex delays between LBP patients and healthy controls.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Group-averaged frequency response functions (FRFs) of the admittance (upper two rows) and EMG (lower two rows). Results for controls (blue lines) and patients (red lines) during the relax task (left) and resist task (right) were averaged over subjects (with shadows as standard deviation). The gain (amplitude difference) and phase (time shift) illustrate the transformation of the input signal into the output signal

Fig. 3
figure 3

Box plots for admittance (top window) and reflexes (bottom window) showing lower admittance and lower reflexes in patients (red bars) compared to healthy controls (blue bars). Error bars represent one standard deviation. * denotes significance at the p < 0.05 level

Table 2 B-values of the effects of LBP, age, task, the interaction effect of task and LBP and the interaction effect of age and LBP
Fig. 4
figure 4

Box plots for the parametric parameters for the relax task (left window) and the resist task (right window) showing differences between patients (red bars) and healthy controls (blue bars). Error bars represent one standard deviation. *denotes significance at the p < 0.05 level

Task and age effects were found in LF admittance gains (higher in the relax condition and younger subjects). Also, task effects were found for LF reflex gains, acceleration and velocity reflex gains (lower in the relax condition). Interaction effects between LBP and age were found on LF admittance gains, damping and position reflex gains, showing smaller differences between LBP patients and healthy controls in the older group. Effective mass was larger in participants with LBP, in line with their larger body mass.

Histograms of the LF admittance gains (Fig. 5) illustrate the difference in admittance between groups, but also show large overlap between LBP patients and healthy controls in both conditions and do not suggest subgroups among LBP patients.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Histograms of LF admittance gains of LBP patients and healthy controls in the relax task (top window) and resist task (bottom window) showing lower values for patients on average, but with substantial overlap between groups. The blue bars represent the controls, the red bars represent the LBP patients, and the purple color represents the overlap between the two groups

Relation of trunk stabilization with pain and disability, and cognition

LF admittance gain in the relax condition was moderately correlated with pain and disability, but also with pain-related cognitions (Table 3). Moderate correlations of intrinsic damping and velocity reflex gains in both conditions and of position feedback gains in the relax condition were found with pain-related cognitions only.

Table 3 Correlations between trunk stability parameters and pain and disability and cognitions

Discussion

We used a novel system identification technique to assess differences in mechanisms of low back stabilization between LBP patients and healthy controls. We hypothesized that LBP patients would show lower admittance during the relax task, because of higher intrinsic stiffness and damping, and less modulation between the relax and resist tasks. In the relax task, we found the expected lower admittance in patients with LBP, and this effect was also found in the resist task. The group differences were larger during the relax task than during the resist task, but modulation between tasks was not significantly different between groups. As expected, parametric identification showed no effects on reflex delays and additionally showed that differences in trunk stabilization between LBP patients and healthy subjects were mostly due to increased position, velocity and acceleration feedback gains in LBP patients. So, in contrast to our hypothesis, differences in trunk stabilization between LBP patients and controls were not dependent on intrinsic stiffness and damping and hence not on feed forward motor control changes, but on enhanced feedback control.

Johansson and Sojka [21] introduced a pathophysiological model that might explain the increase in feedback gains found in our study. The model describes how nociception activates gamma motor neurons and consequently enhances muscle spindle feedback. However, in the present study we found no correlation between pain intensity as rated with the pain and disability questionnaires and reflex gains. Alternatively, the increased reflex gains in LBP patients could be a pain-induced behavioral change toward a more protective or more conservative behavior [22]. This interpretation is consistent with the associations found between outcomes reflecting trunk stabilization and pain-related cognitions, whereas correlations with pain and disability were less strong. Fear of movement, illness beliefs, catastrophizing and depression results coincided with a tighter control over trunk posture in LBP patients. This is in line previously reported associations of trunk stiffness with fear of movement [23] and of the level of trunk muscle activity during walking and catastrophizing [24] in chronic LBP patients. Osumi et al. [25] also have recently published a study on the effect of kinesiophobia on lumbar movements. They divided their LBP group in a ‘high-fear’ and ‘low-fear’ group based on their TSK scores and found that in voluntary lumbar bending movements, the high-fear group took longer on movement initiation and during the switch in movement direction. No differences were found between the low-fear group and healthy controls, also pointing toward a fear-based protective tighter control other than one that results from pain. We found no evidence to suggest subgroups among LBP patients with ‘tight’ and ‘loose’ control as proposed by van Dieen et al. [26]. One might expect ‘loose’ control to be most prominent during the relax task, but instead, we observed a lower admittance in the LBP group in both conditions. It should be taken in account that our method involves a 60 N baseline preload, which might make the relax task too demanding to allow ‘loose’ control.

The age effect and age*LBP interaction effect on admittance found in this study emphasize the need to take age in account when analyzing trunk stabilization. We decided to dichotomize age in our analysis because the effect of age is not necessarily linear, and in this way the interaction is easier to interpret. Because dichotomization holds the chance of misclassification, we also performed an analysis without dichotomization. This did not result in different conclusions.

Some limitations of this study should be considered when interpreting the results. We only measured EMG from the M. Longissimus as most representative for the muscle responses to the imposed perturbations, where other muscles including deep muscles, which would require intramuscular electromyography recordings, could hold independent information regarding trunk stabilization. Also, we only collected data from subjects experiencing small sagittal plane perturbations of an upright position and therefore will not have captured all relevant aspects of trunk motor control [27, 28]. Finally, most patients will have had physiotherapy, which often includes some form of motor control exercise (often stabilization exercise), and this may have contributed to the lower admittance. However, stabilization exercise does not specifically address reflex control and this was found to be the main factor underlying differences in impedance. Also treatment effects would not offer a full explanation for decreased admittance during the ‘relax task.’

As mentioned above, we interpret the difference between healthy subjects and LBP patients as an adaptation to protect the lower back. However, it is questionable whether this adaptive behavior is of benefit in the long term. Van Dieën et al. [22] suggested that in the long term more rigid control of posture and movement can be harmful, because the related muscle activity causes high compressive loading on the spine, which may contribute to injury and degeneration, and it reduces variability of the afferent signals from the lumbar area to the central nervous system, which could cause neuroplastic changes that impair motor control. Combined with the motor control differences found in the present study, this would suggest that motor control exercises, at least in patients with low admittance, should aim for increasing admittance, inhibition of lumbar muscle activity and reduction in adverse pain-related cognitions.

In conclusion, LBP patients showed lower admittance during trunk stabilization tasks based on higher position, velocity and acceleration feedback gains. Furthermore, our results indicate a relation between low admittance and adverse cognitions regarding pain in LBP patients. Overall, these results support interpretation of changes in trunk motor control in LBP as protective behavior.