Introduction

The molecular revolution has brought about scientific advances that have increased the accessibility and use of genetic testing. Indeed, the need to disentangle questions about the public’s understanding of and attitudes toward genetic testing has become more prominent (Condit 2001; Khoury et al. 2009). As genetic testing for disease risk, and now ancestry, become more popular, accessible, and more widely used, it is even more important to understand the cultural and demographic differences that may influence people’s perceived personal utility of the science.

Since the Human Genome Project, the rate of identification of genes and genetic variants associated with various diseases has increased substantially, and is accompanied by a similar rate of development of genetic tests to identify persons at increased risk for those diseases (Guttmacher and Collins 2005). The incorporation of genetic testing into clinical care provides clinicians and the public with additional insight into familial and individual predisposition to certain diseases, allowing them to take additional steps to reduce those risks (Collins and McKusick 2001; Kaphingst and Mcbride 2010). However, there have also been anxieties regarding the potential misuse of genetic test results, such as marginalization and insurance or employment discrimination (Suther and Kiros 2009; Thompson et al. 2003; Wong et al. 2004). For personal genomics to be effective, education for the public and medical providers will need to keep up with the rapid pace of discovery in the field of genomic medicine (Khoury et al. 2009).

In recent years, direct-to-consumer (DTC) marketing of genetic tests for a host of diseases and various non-disease traits has become commonplace, allowing the public to have ready access to personal genetic and genomic information outside of the clinical setting (Lee and Crawley 2009; McCabe and McCabe 2004). While there are advocates of DTC testing for disease who highlight the potential for consumers to have more control and empowerment through access to their genetic information (Prainsack et al. 2008), some fear that consumers can be misled by commercial test results due to a lack of appropriate information, context, and counseling associated with the testing services (Bandelt et al. 2008; Hudson et al. 2007). Consumers frequently believe they are receiving valuable medical advice or diagnostic information (McGuire et al. 2009) while small print disclaimers on DTC genetic testing companies’ websites typically state that their services do not offer medical advice, diagnosis or treatment (Howard and Borry 2009). Having an educational and informative discussion concerning the actual pros and cons of predictive genetic testing may dissuade initially interested individuals from pursuing genetic testing (Wilde et al. 2010). Thus, public education and ensuring potential consumers understand fully the risks, as well as the benefits of DTC genetic testing is of upmost importance.

Genetic ancestry estimation, another application of DTC testing, has also attracted public interest and demand, particularly among groups such as African Americans, who have had comparably less success with traditional family genealogical records (Bolnick et al. 2007; Elliott and Brodwin 2002). The potential personal or communal benefits of genetic ancestry testing notwithstanding, there are concerns about potential negative psychological, social, ethical, and political consequences, and inadequate communication and understanding about the limitations of testing (Bolnick et al. 2007; Duster 2009; Elliott and Brodwin 2002; Shriver and Kittles 2004; Winston and Kittles 2005). These concerns have led some scholars to call for regulation of genetic ancestry testing (Lee et al. 2009). Wagner (2010), however, notes that there is a need for empirical data on the actual impact of ancestry testing, since much of the existing literature on the topic is speculative.

In a qualitative study of genetic ancestry test-takers, Nelson (2008) found that for some, learning about their genetic ancestry overturned well-established beliefs about their identity, whereas for others, the information offered confirmation about their ancestry. However, there is not enough data to fully understand how consumers view genetic ancestry testing, how they interpret their test results, or the extent to which the results affect their psychological and social well-being. The psychosocial impact of the test results seems to be related to people’s motivations for and preconceived notions of testing (Bolnick et al. 2007; Elliott and Brodwin 2002; Shriver and Kittles 2004; Winston and Kittles 2005). Thus, ascertaining people’s perceptions of and attitudes towards genetic testing may be important in determining and addressing the effects test results may have.

Educational level has been thought to influence awareness and attitudes regarding genetic testing. In general, the public is not well informed about genetics, and although better-educated groups appear to be more knowledgeable (MacNew et al. 2010; Priest 2000), it has been difficult to attribute a person’s perception that there are disadvantages to genetic testing to his or her educational level. Previous studies have shown that both lower (Gaskell et al. 1999; Thompson et al. 2003) and higher levels (Gaskell et al. 2000) of education are associated with negative perspectives on genetic testing, while other studies showed no association of education with attitudes or awareness (Einsiedel 2000; Hughes et al. 1997). Further, other evidence suggests that educational background does influence how much information or perceived benefit an individual might gain from advertisements and educational material on genetic testing (Bowen et al. 2009); which in turn, may influence participation in genetic testing. As some research suggests, those from higher education neighborhoods are more likely to participate in genetic testing for disease (Hensley Alford et al. 2010). Overall, whatever the mechanism, the influence of educational level on attitudes towards genetic testing is unclear, and previous associations are likely due to other sociodemographic factors that are correlated with education level.

In contrast to the unclear association between education and attitudes towards genetic testing, there have been consistent findings concerning the relationship between race or ethnicity and attitudes and awareness regarding testing. In general, minority ethnic groups report lower knowledge and greater concern regarding genetic testing (Singer et al. 2004; Suther and Kiros 2009) and are less accepting of genetic testing than their White counterparts (Armstrong et al. 2005; Case et al. 2007; Halbert et al. 2006; Hughes et al. 1997; Peters et al. 2004). This is not surprising given the well-documented mistreatment of minorities in medical research in the past (Fairchild and Bayer 1999; Gamble 1993, 1997), and the African American community’s expressed mistrust of medical research (Corbie-Smith et al. 1999) and genetic testing (Suther and Kiros 2009; Thompson et al. 2003). In addition, there are significant racial differences in cultural values (Halbert et al. 2007) and awareness regarding genetic testing (MacNew et al. 2010) that may influence an individual’s perception of medical research including genetics. Given the current attention to racial/ethnic health disparities and mounting findings showing genetic variants associated with heightened disease risk or particular treatment responses that differ significantly in frequency among racial/ethnic groups (Ge et al. 2009; Zeigler-Johnson et al. 2008; Zhang et al. 2009; Zhu et al. 2005), there will likely be increased interest in racial/ethnic differences in attitudes toward genetic testing.

Few studies have looked for regional differences in awareness and attitudes regarding genetic testing that may implicitly suggest influential sociocultural factors within each region, not specifically tied to educational background or race/ethnicity. One study examining regional differences showed that a lower percentage of Canadian participants than US participants indicated at least one negative emotional response to genetic testing for hemochromatosis (Power et al. 2007). Similarities in attitudes towards genetic testing have also been shown across people of different backgrounds within the same region. Westerners and non-Westerner immigrants in the Netherlands were shown to have similar attitudes towards genetic testing for cystic fibrosis (Lakeman et al. 2008, 2009).

Cities in the US can be drastically different from each other, particularly with regard to their culture, structure, climate, and concentration on education and research. Each major US city might have its own unique traditions, pastimes and differing social influences, each tending to have its own idiosyncrasies developed directly from the history of the given place, and its people. Much of the culture, molded by the economy and type of market that is most prevalent, becomes specialized over time, leading to more drastic regional differences over time (Scott 2000).

In summary, previous research has focused mostly on racial/ethnic and socioeconomic differences in attitudes towards genetic testing. It has been suggested that views differ mainly by racial/ethnic or socioeconomic status; however, less research has examined within group and regional differences. There may be more variability among people of different regions of the country than people of different race/ethnicity or socioeconomic strata. The goal of the current study is to evaluate differences in awareness and attitudes regarding genetic testing for disease and ancestry among and within four geographically diverse regions in the US; Norman, OK; Cincinnati, OH; Harlem, NY; and Washington, DC.

Methods

Study sample

The sample consisted of 452 persons, 146 (32.3%) males and 306 (67.7%) females, with a mean age of 48.7 years (range 18–82, SD = 15.9) (Table 1). Approximately 66% percent of participants reported that they were married, widowed, or separated/divorced, and about 32% were never married. Most of the participants (88.94%) had at least some college education or were college graduates, and most (67.04%) were employed. A large proportion of participants reported their ethnicity as African American (41.15%); however, about 28% of participants failed to or chose not to report their ethnicity. All study participants were attendees at community forums on DNA testing for ancestry that were held in OK (n = 94), OH (n = 98), NY (n = 153), and DC (n = 107).

Table 1 Sociodemographic characteristics of the sample

Recruitment and data collection procedures

Participants were recruited through four community-based forums convened between June 2004 and March 2006 in Norman, OK; Cincinnati, OH; Harlem, NY; and Washington, DC. The forums were put together with the help of local host organizations and advertised through mass media, community organizations, churches, and flyers. Attendees came from across the respective states or metropolitan areas and have/had ongoing contact with the host sites. Information was not available on where participants grew up or how long they had been living in their current location. The primary purpose of the forums was to provide information on genetic ancestry testing and respond to questions from community members. There was also some discussion of genetics and disease. Potential attendees were informed beforehand that free genetic testing would be offered and that the present study on attitudes toward genetic testing would be conducted. Virtually all attendees participated in both genetic ancestry testing and this study. This paper describes awareness and attitudes of study participants prior to their uptake of genetic ancestry testing. Study goals and procedures were explained to participants, after which they received two copies of the consent form, one to sign and return and the other to keep for their records. Participants then completed a 22-item survey eliciting information on their background, knowledge about their ancestry, awareness and value of genetic testing for disease and ancestry, perceived benefits and risks of genetic ancestry testing, and motivations for having ancestry testing. The survey was self-administered and comprised both fixed-choice and open-ended items. Knowledge about personal ancestry was determined by quantifying the amount of detail the participant reported in response to an open-ended question asking, “How much do you know about your ancestry in general?” Approval for the study was obtained from the Institutional Review Board at Howard University.

Data analysis

Chi-square tests were performed to analyze site differences on categorical variables. Fisher's exact test p values are reported for comparisons where cell sizes are less than 5. Significant chi-square tests were followed by logistic regressions. Variables were recoded for logistic regression analyses in that the response choice of interest was coded as ‘1’ and all other responses were coded as ‘0’. ANOVA tests were performed to examine site differences on continuous variables. All multivariate tests (i.e. logistic regressions and ANOVAs) controlled for sex, age, and education. Self-reported ethnicity was not used as a covariate due to the significant frequency of missing responses (28%). All data analyses were performed using SAS 9.0.

Results

Site demographics

We looked at demographic factors to determine what characteristics among the four sites might relate to differences in knowledge, awareness and attitudes regarding genetic testing. Descriptive demographic data by site are presented in Table 1. A significant chi-square test showed differences in reported ethnicity among sites in that both the OH and DC sites had greater percentages of participants reporting African American ethnicity, 73.4 and 76.9%, respectively, than the OK site, 42.6%, and the NY site, 45.0% (p < 0.001). Further, the OK site had a significantly larger percentage of self-reported mixed ethnicity participants, 39.7%, than the other three sites, <18.4%, p < 0.05. Over 28% of participants failed to report their ethnicity. There were no site differences in participants’ tendency to not report their ethnicity. For age, the DC site was younger than both the OK and OH sites (p = 0.043), and for education, the DC and OH site had a higher education level than both the OK and NY sites (p = 0.036). The NY site had the largest percentage of unemployed participants, 9.9 compared to 4.0% on average for all other sites (p = 0.014). For religion, of all four sites, the DC group had the largest proportion of participants reporting “no religion” (15.7 vs. ≤5.15%) and the smallest proportion of participants identifying with Christianity (68.5 vs. ≥80.4; p = 0.01). In summary, the OK and NY sites had fewer self-reported African Americans, while the DC participants were younger, had a higher mean education, and were less likely to identify with an organized religion than all other sites. We considered these factors in later analyses as possible alternative explanations for site differences in awareness and attitudes.

Experience and awareness

As shown in Table 2, 15.0% of all participants previously had genetic testing or counseling for disease risk. There were no site differences in past experience with genetic testing or counseling.

Table 2 Experience and attitudes regarding genetic testing

The DC site reported reading or hearing more about genetic testing for African ancestry than all other sites (p < 0.001), and knowing more about genetic testing in general than the OH site (p < 0.05; Table 3). However, with respect to knowledge about their personal ancestry, participants at the OK site reported more detail about their own personal ancestry than participants in either the OH or DC group (p = 0.02).

Table 3 Awareness about genetic testing and knowledge about personal ancestry

Attitudes

Participants from the NY site were more likely to request disease-related genetic testing (76.8%) than participants at all other sites (<65.4%; p = 0.014). In particular, the NY site was 2.5 times more likely to request genetic testing for disease than the OK site and about two times more likely than the OH and DC participants.

When asked, “How important is it to you to find out more about your ancestry?” the NY group was almost two times more likely to indicate “very important” than the DC and OK groups, and was three times more likely to indicate “very important” than the OH group (p = 0.012; Table 2). The OK group tended to see less benefit in genetic testing for ancestry than participants from other sites, but this association only approached significance (p = 0.06). It appears that overall the NY group had the most positive attitude toward genetic testing and the OK group had the least positive. Seventy percent of participants in the study valued both genetic testing for disease and ancestry equally. However, although the OK participants were least positive about both ancestry and disease testing than participants from the other three sites, they valued ancestry testing more than disease testing. In fact, OK participants were 2.6 and 4.7 times more likely to value ancestry testing over disease testing than the OH and NY sites, respectively (p < 0.001). We performed a sub-group analysis limiting the sample to individuals reporting any African ethnicity and found that the effect showing a preference for ancestry testing over disease testing among the OK group remained significant (p = 0.004).

Are site differences due to previous experience, ethnicity, or religion?

To determine whether the above site differences in awareness and attitudes were due to other variables, we reran each multivariate model and added separately the following variables as covariates: previous experience with genetic testing/counseling, self-reported ethnicity, and religion. If these variables explained the effect of site differences, when added to the model, site would no longer be significant or the effect of site would decrease. In each model, however, the site differences remained and were independent of all covariates. Thus, site differences in awareness and attitudes are independent of factors such as previous experiences and religion. It is difficult to determine whether self-reported ethnicity may explain these effects due to the significant amount of missing data on this variable. However, when limiting the sample to only those with available ethnicity data (n = 327), there were still significant site differences in attitudes and awareness; and, including ethnicity in the model did not attenuate these effects.

Principal components of attitudes and awareness

To help illustrate the findings of this study, the differences across the US in awareness and attitudes towards genetic testing, we ran a principal component analysis on participant responses to seven items that represent attitudes and awareness concerning genetic testing for disease and ancestry to identify principal components describing the most variation in the data. Table 4 shows items and loadings for the top two principal components which together explain 50% of the variance in participant responses; the first component representing awareness and the second representing attitudes towards testing. One item, “Do you see possible harms or risks from having a DNA test for ancestry?” did not load significantly on either component.

Table 4 Principal components items and loadings

Figure 1 shows means for these top two components by site. The data plot of the two top principal components for each site suggests that examining the combinations of both awareness and attitudes may reveal even more divergence between each site than was evident when looking at individual factors alone; regions are more clearly differentiated from each other in respect to awareness and attitudes. The DC participants expressed the highest awareness of genetic testing for disease and ancestry but were neutral in their attitudes. NY site participants expressed the most positive attitude towards testing while reporting only average awareness. The OH site reported being neutral in attitude and low in awareness, while the OK site reported the least positive attitude towards testing and below average awareness. These stark regional differences in attitudes and awareness could not be better explained by any other measured variable.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Site differences in attitude and awareness factor scores

Discussion

The current study benefited from a sample that was socioeconomically, ethnically, and regionally diverse. Unlike research by Chen and Goodson (2007) that indicates associations between genetic testing attitudes and socioeconomic status, gender, and age, our findings show that differences in awareness and attitudes among four geographically diverse major cities in the US: Norman, OK; Cincinnati, OH; Harlem, NY; and Washington, DC, could not be better accounted for by demographic factors that have typically been associated with attitudes towards genetic testing. Findings from this study suggest that ethnicity alone does not determine an individual’s socioenvironmental experience or beliefs about genetic testing. This paper presents a different perspective from previous studies that treated ethnic groups as monolithic and have suggested awareness, beliefs, and attitudes towards genetic testing for disease are largely driven by ethnicity (see Lannin et al. 1998; Lerman et al. 1999; Palmer et al. 2008; Suther and Kiros 2009). Instead, our findings point to awareness and attitude differences within ethnic groups and similarities across ethnic groups residing in the same region.

Although 28% of the participants chose to not report their ethnicity, the data that we do have indicates that our sample was ethnically diverse. Forty-two percent identified as African American, 9% as White, and 13% as mixed. The remaining participants reported other ethnicities not broadly categorized (i.e. East African, West African, Afro-Caribbean, Central American, Native American, or Central African). The preponderance of missing self-report ethnicity data in this study may reflect public attitudes towards genetic testing and/or race. Other variables that typically have a high frequency of non-response for surveys, such as income and occupation had lower levels of missing data (12 and 15%, respectively) than self-reported ethnicity in the current study. Missing ethnicity data was not associated with education, SES, or geographic region. Participants were also asked to report the ethnicity of their parents and grandparents. Of those participants who did not report their own ethnicity, only 23% failed to report ethnicity for any of their grandparents, suggesting that participants were deliberate and may have had a variety of motivations underlying their pattern of reporting for ethnicity.

Participants may have chosen to not report their own ethnicity for several reasons including an uncertainty about their own ethnicity and fear of being wrong, perceived lack of privacy, and the potential for misuse and misinterpretation of this information. The use of race and ethnicity in the research and clinical settings has historically been problematic (Caulfield et al. 2009; Duster 2006). Given the current social context of racial profiling, growing inequities, and media hype, the potential harms associated with racial and ethnic categorization cannot be ignored (Varcoe et al. 2009). These issues might be associated with the reluctance of some participants to report their own ethnicity.

The sites used for the sample were geographically dispersed, with an average of 530 miles between each site and over 1,500 miles separating Norman, OK, from Harlem, NY. Thus the current study was well designed to identify possible regional differences in the lay public’s awareness and attitudes regarding genetic testing for disease and ancestry, while also ruling out demographic factors that could logically account for these differences.

The Harlem, NY, group had the largest percentage of unemployed and the largest percentage of White participants. The NY group reported being more likely to request genetic testing for disease and placed the most importance on finding out more about their ancestry. Overall, the NY group had the most positive attitude towards genetic testing while reporting average awareness.

Harlem, NY, is traditionally a predominantly Black area that has experienced significant ethnic shifts due to economic boom and bust cycles (Taylor 1998). Being at the heart of NY, an international city, Harlem may be fully exposed to a culture of curiosity and openness to new ideas, thus promoting positive attitudes towards science and medicine.

The Washington, DC group was the youngest, had the largest percentage of African Americans, and had the largest proportion of participants with no religious affiliation. The DC site reported reading and hearing more about genetic testing for African ancestry and generally had more awareness of genetic testing than all other sites.

Washington, DC differs from all US cities because it was specifically established to serve as the nation’s capital by the Constitution of the United States (United States National Park Service and Parks and History Association 1987). It has been embroiled in issues of politics, policy, race, and national identity from the very beginning (Taylor 1998). DC residents potentially have more exposure to genetic testing information due to the presence of a number of academic and policy institutions in the area and proximity to the National Institutes of Health, as well as media communications in DC surrounding the genetic testing discourse. Washington, DC is also home to a prominent genetic ancestry testing company (i.e., African Ancestry Inc.), which might partially account for the increased awareness among DC participants about genetic testing for African ancestry. Prior research suggesting that DTC advertising of genetic testing is positively associated with awareness of genetic testing (Bowen et al. 2009) supports this hypothesis.

The Cincinnati, OH group had a large percentage of African Americans and the smallest number of participants identifying as mixed. The OH group was least likely to indicate that finding out more about their ancestry was very important and generally reported the lowest level of awareness about genetic testing.

Cincinnati, OH, is sometimes thought of as the first purely American city, lacking the heavy European influence that was present on the east coast (Taylor 1998). Race relations in Cincinnati have historically been tense as it lies at the intersection of states that allowed slavery before the Civil War, namely Kentucky, and one that did not, namely Ohio, as well as being an important stop on the Underground Railroad (Taylor 1998). Despite the highly racialized history of Cincinnati, however, ethnic identity did not seem to influence the pattern of responding among this group.

The Norman, OK group was the most ethnically diverse with almost 30% of the participants reporting mixed ethnicity. This may have been due to a high number of participants within the OK group that believed they had considerable Indigenous American ancestry. Accordingly, they reported more details about their personal ancestries than other sites.

Norman, OK, is approximately 20 miles south of downtown Oklahoma City. Among study sites Oklahoma is unique due to its Native American heritage (May 1996). It has the second largest population of Native Americans (United States Bureau of the Census Geography Division 2002) and more than 25 Native American languages are spoken in the state, the most of any state (King 2008).

The general reluctance of some Native Americans or Native American tribes to participate in genetic research and other genetics-related activities has been well discussed (Bolnick et al. 2007; International HapMap Consortium 2004; TallBear 2007) and supports most attitudinal findings from the OK participants in this study, many of whom claim some Native American ancestry. The OK site had below average awareness about genetic testing and expressed the least positive attitudes towards genetic testing, but, interestingly, valued ancestry testing more than disease testing. Their preference for ancestry testing might be due to the potential or expected material gains associated with having evidence of one’s Native American ancestry.

Overall, whatever the factors underlying these regional differences, clear distinctions among the sites can be made in terms of awareness and attitudes regarding genetic testing that are surprisingly not better explained by demographic factors. Our interpretations of the outcomes are largely speculative; however, our explanations concerning findings for Washington, DC and Oklahoma are based on years of experience working with the participating communities and host organizations at those sites, as well as our unpublished data and observations of what ensued at the forums. Our study is an incremental step towards filling the gap in knowledge regarding differences in awareness and attitudes toward genetic testing in general and ancestry testing in particular. Further research will be needed to validate our findings and determine specific underlying factors.

The results from this study must be interpreted in the context of the following limitations. First, the unavailability of self-reported ethnicity for many participants compromised our ability to adequately explore the relationship of that variable to awareness and attitudes. Second, all study participants attended a forum on ancestry testing and volunteered for free genetic testing for ancestry, thus the sample is subject to selection bias. The sample is unlikely to be representative of the population. Most participants generally had positive views towards genetic testing, which is expected given that all participants voluntarily attended a forum on genetic testing for ancestry. Third, the Southern and Western regions of the country were not represented in the study. Their inclusion may have added to the present findings and resulted in larger and more informative regional differences. Despite these limitations, we argue that the additional ethnicity information as well as the inclusion of individuals who would not have attended a forum on genetic ancestry testing, individuals who did not receive genetic testing, and individuals from other geographic regions (e.g., Western and Southern states) might have only further strengthened our results by providing more variability in responses, and potentially further differentiating the sites. Our results provide insight from four cities and can (1) inform the development or refinement of a concept model of factors that are linked to genetic testing and (2) guide future studies seeking to explain why there are differences in awareness and attitudes regarding genetic testing.

In conclusion, we make a couple of recommendations for future research. First, research identifying cultural and social factors that may underlie these regional differences is needed. Several factors were not measured or accounted for in the current study that may influence or help explain differences in awareness and attitudes towards genetic testing. Cultural beliefs, attitudes, and behaviors, including spiritual faith and religious practices, have been associated with openness and response to genetic testing for disease (Hughes et al. 2003; Lannin et al. 1998; Schwartz et al. 2000). Other belief systems such as temporal orientation and communalism have also been associated with likelihood to seek genetic testing for disease (Halbert et al. 2005; Hughes et al. 2003; Lukwago et al. 2003). In addition, experiences are shaped differently in different locations with different histories of race and class. For example, what it is to be black in Norman, OK—especially as that might relate to entangled histories with Native American tribes—may be quite different from what it is to be black in Harlem, NY or Washington, DC. The case of Cincinnati further demonstrates the complexity of the relationship between history/experience and attitudes. Future studies will benefit from increased sociological rigor and attention to the individual and collective beliefs, values, and experiences that potentially mediate the observed regional differences shown in the current study. Understanding the covariances and interactions among these socioenvironmental factors and the genetics that underlie health outcomes will facilitate the genetic counseling process associated with individual genetic testing as well as our ability to understand and improve individual and group health (Gravlee et al. 2009). As demonstrated by our study, further exploration of the attitudes toward health-related genetic testing versus ancestry testing is warranted.

Second, there is a need for additional general population studies of awareness and attitudes regarding genetic testing, particularly ancestry testing. Studies utilizing convenient samples, such as reported here, can be informative, but limit the generalizability of findings. The most useful study designs are likely to be those that enable us to also gather perspectives from persons with little or no interest in the testing under investigation.

Third, our results suggest that research findings from single sites should be interpreted with caution. Few studies have included samples from multiple regions across the US, a factor that may have contributed to the inconsistencies in previous findings. More regionally diverse samples are needed to fully explore the differences that geography makes in test takers’ awareness and attitudes including differences in ethnic and socioeconomic identity formation across space and time. Such studies could prove invaluable in informing decisions about the development of state-wide and perhaps national policies regarding genetic testing. Further, the current study is differentiated from other similar studies in that our survey included several open-ended items, which potentially gives the results added depth and breadth. Standardization of surveying methods for general social awareness and attitudes related to science and medicine will help increase our ability to collect and compare data nationally and internationally.

Attitudes towards genetic testing result from a complex contribution of beliefs and perspectives that are constantly in flux and ever changing (Condit 2001). These changing tides may be less due to polar ethnic influences than once thought. Rather, factors local to the individual such as regional history and culture may be more influential than social identity.