Introduction

Sleep patterns change rapidly across the earliest years of life and play an important role in children’s daytime functioning. There is a growing interest in determining those factors influencing children’s sleep development (both intrinsic (i.e., child characteristics) and extrinsic (i.e., environmental input)) [1, 2]. Children sleep patterns might rapidly take a turn-over in light of external factors such the current public health pandemic due to COVID-19 outbreak. On March 14, 2020, in response to the first wave outbreak, the Spanish Government imposed one of the most stringent lockdowns in Europe (BOE-A-2020–3692 https://www.boe.es/eli/es/rd/2020/03/14/463) especially for children, as an emergency measure to prevent further spreading of the infection. After 6 weeks of strict home confinement, Spanish minors were permitted to leave the house for one hour within allotted time slots and half-mile radius. This sudden unexpected circumstance meant having to adapt to a new reality for both adults and children. Previous literature has found that during non-school period, children tend to be physically less active, have longer screen time, irregular sleep patterns, and less adequate diets, resulting in weight gain and a loss of cardiorespiratory fitness [3, 4].

In these recent past months, there is growing evidence being published about the impact of this pandemic on children’s health, sleep patterns and psychological development. A meta-analysis on the psychological and behavioural impact of home confinement and quarantine measures during COVID-19 pandemic in 22.996 children/adolescents from diverse European countries, Asian countries, and South America showed that 79.4% of children were affected negatively by the pandemic and quarantine; at least 21.3% of them had a sleep disturbance [5]. A cross-sectional study carried out during the quarantine on 1143 Spanish and Italian parents of children aged three to 18 years old found that 85.7% of the parents perceived changes in their children’s emotional state and behaviours. The most frequent symptoms were difficulty concentrating (76.6%), boredom (52%), irritability (39%), restlessness (38.8%), nervousness (38%), feelings of loneliness (31.3%), uneasiness (30.4%), and worries (30.1%). Spanish parents reported more symptoms than Italians ones [6].

When analyzing preschoolers’ sleep patterns during home confinement, a recent cross-sectional study [7] on 412 infants and toddlers 0–35 months from several mainly European countries described an acute decrease in sleep quality. At follow-up (1 and 2 months later), this effect rebounded to the level reported for the period before the home confinement. Caregiver’s stress level was also identified as a substantial risk factor determining lower sleep quality [7]. Another study based in China [8] evaluated 1619 preschoolers (aged 4–6 years) sleep patterns based on the completed the Children’s Sleep Habit Questionnaire (CSHQ), comparing the findings to a similar sample recruited in 2018. Confined preschoolers showed later bedtimes and wake times, longer nocturnal and shorter nap sleep durations, comparable 24-h sleep duration, and fewer caregiver-reported sleep disturbances [8]. Another study carried out in Japan [9] compared cross-sectional data from online surveys describing the sleep behaviour of infants and caregivers in two time slots (before and during the COVID-19 pandemic) finding no significant differences in wake-up times, bedtimes, or nocturnal sleep times (p = 0.613). However, confined infants showed a decrease in total sleep time and media use increased significantly [9].

The aim of this work is to compare two similar samples of infant and toddlers in Spain three to 36 months of age (before and during COVID-19 confinement) in order to identify possible sleep pattern changes related to the COVID-19 confinement. We hypothesize to find a negative impact on infants and toddlers sleep patterns during the COVID-19 confinement.

Methods

We analyzed a repeated cross-sectional survey of parents who reported having a healthy infant/toddler between 3 and 36 months of age in Spain. One conducted before COVID-19 confinement (2017–2018) [10] and another conducted during the strict home confinement of the first wave of COVID-19 (March–May of 2020). We used an internet self-administrated questionnaire assessing sleep patterns using the Spanish validated Brief Infant Sleep Questionnaire (BISQ) criteria [11, 12] in both surveys. The BISQ comprises 14 items: 4 on the demographic characteristics of the child and 10 on sleep habits. There are 4 multiple-choice single-answer questions and 6 open-ended questions about the timing and duration of sleep. This questionnaire has been described as a robust instrument of proven reliability and validity, easy to use and brief, and thus adequate as a screening tool [13, 14].The sample recruitment, the inclusion, and exclusion criteria as well as the variables measured and analyses carried out mirrored our previous study in order to be able to perform adequate comparisons between both our samples [10].

Participants

We compared 1634 infants and toddlers recruited in two different stages (1380 before confinement (February 2017 to February 2018) and 254 during confinement (March–May 2020). Both samples included healthy Spanish children between ages 3 to 36 months. Data were collected from a digital online questionnaire available within a free-access website. Parents were invited to participate either during their child’s paediatric follow-up visit at Hospital Universitari General de Catalunya or through online free access forums about parenting topics.

Measures

Parents completed a self-reported 47 item online questionnaire, which included the Spanish validated Brief Infant Sleep Questionnaire (BISQ-E) [12] (based on the BISQ [11]. Other items included in the questionnaire were demographic data and general family habits; however, this complementary data was not used in the current study analysis.

In order to understand developmental patterns and resembling the original BISQ methodology [11], children were grouped in five age ranges as initially done in the BISQ validation. Group 1 was ages 3 to 6 months; group 2, ages 7 to 12 months; group 3, ages 13 to 18 months; group 4, ages 19 to 24 months; and group 5, ages 25 to 36 months.

Based on the BISQ criteria established for clinically classifying poor sleepers [11] (more than three awakenings a night, nocturnal awakenings longer than 1 h and, a total sleep time in the 24-h period of less than 9 h), where any of these indications in a child suggests the need for further follow-up, we used this same criterion for defining problematic sleep behaviour. Parental perception of sleep quality was also included in the analysis as a variable present in the original BISQ [11].

Procedure

The Institutional Ethics Review Board at General University of Catalunya Hospital and Catalonia International University, Barcelona, approved this study. All users provided consent to the inclusion of their data in the present research study and were able to decline participation at all times. The researchers provided users with contact information should they have any questions, concerns, or desire to withdraw consent. Data from second cross-sectional sample were collected from April 2020 to June 2020.

For this study, a self-registered website domain was created called www.epison.es where the questionnaire was uploaded and encrypted. The completion of the questionnaire was voluntary, and parents were not offered any compensation for their participation. Inclusion criteria were parents with infants and toddlers from 3 to 36 months of age residing in Spain. Exclusion criteria were limited to literacy in Spanish language.

Analyses

The variables were described with frequency and percentage for the qualitative variables, the mean and standard deviation for the quantitative normal-distributed variables, and median and interquartile range for the quantitative not normally distributed variables.

To compare two groups (before and during confinement, for example), a prevalence ratio was calculated for qualitative variables, with their confidence interval and p-value. Mann–Whitney test was performed for quantitative not normally distributed variables.

The confidence intervals were calculated with a level of confidence of 95%. The tests were considered significant when the p-value was under 0.05. The normality was tested with the Shapiro–Wilk test (n < 50) and the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. (n ≥ 50). All the analyses were performed with R Statistical Software version 3.5.1 for MAC.

Results

Both samples of infants and toddlers were distributed regarding age in months as following: infants and toddlers age 3–6 months 13.8%, 7–12 months 20.4%, 13–18 months 17.3%, 19–24 months 14.6%, and 25–36 months 33.9%. Further data on demographics are presented in Table 1.

Table 1 Demographics among both samples (before and during confinement sample)

Table 2 compares sleep patterns and ecology before and during confinement. We found that during confinement, infants and toddlers went to sleep later than similar population under no confinement: median bedtime 21:30 (IQR 21–22) before confinement vs. 21:36 bedtime (IQR 21–22:30) during confinement (p = 0.004). This finding was also significant when considering the youngest infant group (3–6 months of age) with median bedtime 21:24 (IQR 20:48.8–22:18) before confinement vs. median bedtime 22:00 (IQR 21:30–23) during confinement (p = 0.017)) and the oldest toddler group (25–36 months of age) (median bedtime 21:36 (IQR 21:18–22:06) before confinement vs. median bedtime 22:00 (IQR 21:30–22:36) during confinement (p < 0.001) (see Supplementary tables: Table A. Bedtime). Regarding total sleep time in a 24-h period, we found a tendency for less total sleep time during confinement (12.0 h (IQR 11.0–13.0) before confinement vs. 12.0 h (IQR 11.0–12.7) during confinement) however non statistically significant (p = 0.078).

Table 2 Comparison on sleep patterns and ecology among both samples (before and during confinement)

In Table 2, prevalence of infants and toddlers presenting with the criteria used in the original BISQ to define poor sleepers are presented [11] (total sleep time < 9 h, > 3 night awakenings, duration of awakenings > 1 h) as well as sleep onset latency longer than 30 min both before and during COVID-19 confinement. Results were adjusted for age distribution of our sample, sex, and parental level of education. We found more infants and toddlers presenting with a sleep latency longer than 30 min during confinement (p < 0.001). However, we found a decrease in infants and toddlers presenting with longer than 1 h nocturnal awakenings: 16.7% (IQR 14.8–18.8) before confinement vs. 9.8% (IQR 6.47–14.18) during confinement (p = 0.011).

Based on the mentioned BISQ criteria for classifying inadequate sleep patterns, we categorised both samples of infants and toddlers in three groups; (i) none, (ii) one of the poor sleeper BISQ criteria, and (iii) two or more poor sleeper BISQ criteria. We observed a statistically significant increase in infants and toddlers showing criteria for classifying poor sleepers during confinement (p < 0.001). Therefore, during COVID-19 home confinement, infants and toddlers showed longer sleep latency, later bedtime, and more criteria for classifying poor sleepers as defined in the original BISQ (more than three awakenings a night, nocturnal awakenings longer than 1 h and, a total sleep time of less than 9 h [11].

In Table 2, we also compare parental perception of the child’s sleep problem before confinement and during confinement. A trend of increasing parental perception of a child’s sleep as problematic was observed (39.4% of parents referring their child had a sleep problem before confinement vs. 44.1% of parents during confinement) however not statistically significant (adjusted p = 0.363).

In the supplementary tables, the main variables regarding sleep patterns (sleep time, bedtime, number and duration of nocturnal wakefulness, short sleep time, sleep latency, and parental perception) were presented adjusted for sex and age of child, level of education, age of the respondent, and parental perception of sleep difficulty.

Regarding sleep onset latency > 30 min, we found statistically significant differences in both samples globally (12.3% of infants and toddlers presenting a sleep onset latency > 30 min before confinement vs. 33.8% during confinement (p < 0.001). This difference was also found when comparing both sexes, all age groups and among parents with a university degree (10.6% before confinement vs. 38.7% during confinement (p < 0.001) (see supplementary tables: Table E: Sleep onset latency > 30 min).

We found no statistically significant differences in daytime napping (p-value = 0.090). In both samples before confinement and during confinement, daytime napping lasted a median of 2 h, the same first quartile (1 h 30 min) and just 15 min of difference in the third quartile (3 h before home confinement and 2 h 45 min during confinement).

Discussion

This comparative study on infant and toddler sleep patterns analyses two different but comparable samples recruited before and during the COVID-19 pandemic in Spain.

The relevance of studying sleep patterns in early childhood relies on the fact that adequate sleep patterns are linked to proper and long-term social-emotional development. A previous study found that later bedtimes and less total sleep across the 24-h period predicted higher internalizing problem scores (including indices of depression/withdrawal, general anxiety, separation distress, and inhibition) [15]. Regarding sleep patterns in paediatrics specifically during the COVID-19 pandemic, interestingly various and relevant literature has found that changes in sleep patterns are diverse and no unified change for the worse can actually be expected. Zreik [16] analyzed in Israel the possible negative implications for sleep during COVID-19 pandemic for both mothers and infants and toddlers 6–72 months of age, finding that about 30% of mothers reported a negative change in child's sleep quality and a decrease in sleep duration. However, there were also mothers who reported a positive change in sleep quality during COVID-19 pandemic [16]. In our study, we globally found a statistically significantly increase of infants and toddlers presenting inadequate sleep patterns as defined in the BISQ [11] during COVID confinement (37% of infants and toddlers vs. 25% before COVID-19 confinement) (p < 0.001).

Regarding adequacy of sleep duration in our sample, and based on the WHO guidelines for children younger than 5 years as reference [17] (infants younger than 1 year of age should sleep a minimum of 12 h of total sleep time and from 1 to 2 years of age a minimum of 11 h), remarkably, in our research, a quarter of them did not reach this total sleep time of 12 h (IQR 11–12.7). In a recent study analysing sleep patterns in Spanish children of relatively comparable age (0–2 years of age) during home confinement, total sleep time reported was mean of 10.98 h; (SD = 1.8) [18], Another research based in Spain on sleep patterns during COVID-19 pandemic analyzing 280 children from 0 to 4 years of age described a mean sleep time in infants younger than 1 year of 11.69 h (SD = 1.87) and among 1–2 years 11.35 h (SD = 1.74) [19]. Lecuelle [20] found in French preeschoolers that during home confinement, the length of nocturnal sleep increased from 10.3 to 10.9 h, however with no impact on the total duration of sleep over 24 h.

Difficulties initiating and maintaining sleep increased in our study, finding that infants and toddlers during confinement went to bed slightly later. This fact was especially substantial in both the youngest infants and the oldest toddlers. Although we do not have evidence for this finding, a possible explanation for these differences might be that younger and older infants are more aligned with their parents sleeping routines. For example, a study carried in the USA comparing the sleep of infants whose mothers were in home confinement to those whose mothers were working as usual throughout the COVID-19 pandemic found that those Infants with confined mothers had later sleep offset times and longer nighttime sleep durations, compared to infants of mothers who were working as usual [21].

We also found longer sleep onset latencies (> 30 min) during COVID-19 confinement, with infants and toddlers showing more difficulty to fall asleep. Interestingly, however, prolonged nocturnal awakening of more than 1 h decreased. We have analysed the possible impact of the passage to the daylight saving time (March 29, 2020 in Spain) in the sleep latency duration in our sample, not finding significant statistical differences among both samples (PR 0.99 (IC 95% 0.73–1.33, p-value = 0.932). Previous literature has already described similar findings regarding infants and toddlers with delayed bedtime being significantly associated with bedtime resistance even when adjusting for total sleep time [18, 22].

A study examining 3157 preschoolers in Chile found that during early stages of the pandemic, sleep duration increased, and sleep quality declined. Toddlers and preschoolers with space to play at home and living in rural areas experienced an attenuated impact of the pandemic restrictions on their physical activity levels, screen time, and sleep quality [23]. Therefore, home confinement might play a role in regulating sleep patterns intertwined with other variables previously described such as sleep light, activity level, social contact, and psychological well-being [6, 19, 24] as well as the potential influence of longer screen time exposure in explaining later bedtimes [8, 19, 24].

Previous literature has also pointed out that low optimism and parental stress correlates negatively with sleep duration and parental perception of sleep quality [25]. In our study, we found that 44% of parents indicated their child had a sleep problem during COVID-19 confinement, whereas 39% of parents stated sleep difficulties before COVID-19 confinement.

There were some limitations to the present study. Home confinement in Spain took place between March–May 2020; however, we recruited our sample from April to June 2020 after receiving approval from our Ethics Committee; therefore, a memory bias in this short time span might be taken into account. Both samples (before confinement and during confinement) showed slight differences in some characteristics such as a slightly uneven proportion of toddlers among 19–24 months of age in both samples (21% before confinement vs. 14% during confinement (p = 0.026)), a higher proportion of parents with a university degree (64% before confinement vs. 77% during confinement (p < 0.001)), and a higher percentage of parents younger than 25 years of age (1.5% before confinement vs. 10% during confinement (p < 0.001)). This might be probably due to the relatively smaller sample recruited during confinement. However, methodologically in both groups, we followed same procedural guidance.

Our sample may have a limited generalizability as participants were recruited by convenience sampling and were mostly living in the region of Catalonia in Spain. Regarding demographical characteristics, sample size limitations have to be noted. Despite this limitation, the results in this current research showed statistical significance. Our sample included an unbalanced proportion of families with higher education, as can be expected from a primarily internet-based study. Nevertheless, our findings were consistent with previous cross-cultural web-based studies [11, 26, 27], showing similar epidemiological characteristics so we believe we have obtained similar segments of the population within our country for purposes of cross-cultural comparison [11, 27]. For the present research, we based the definition of sleep difficulties solely on the criteria presented in the BISQ [11], thus not taking into account other aspects of this complex construct. We used a limited number of sleep variables to define sleep ecology without taking into account further variables that have shown to have a predictor value for sleep quality such as the use of routines, parental stress during COVID-19 pandemic, and sociodemographic relevant data such as size of living unit, income, housing location and space, and remote-working of one or both parents.

The reliance on parental reports in assessing infant sleep has inherent limitations; however, previous research [13, 28] has shown a high correlation between parent-reported sleep duration and actigraphy-recorded sleep duration in young healthy children. Further, parents with concern about their child’s sleep may have been more likely to participate, skewing this aspect of our results. Information bias may be an inherent limitation to web-based surveys. However, the fact that there was not an interviewer carrying out the questionnaire may have decreased an unacceptability bias (29).

Conclusions

These results suggest that confinement due to COVID-19 pandemic had a significant impact on infants’ and toddler’s sleep during the initial wave in Spain. In light of these findings, sleep habits need constant consideration along a child’s development as a changing environment and context such as the COVID-10 pandemic has shown to have a considerate impact among a short time span. Follow-up studies focusing on infant’s and toddler’s sleep patterns might help assess the potential long-term effects of the COVID-19 pandemic.