Abstract
Recent developments in network neuroscience suggest reconsidering what we thought we knew about the default mode network (DMN). Although this network has always been seen as unitary and associated with the resting state, a new deconstructive line of research is pointing out that the DMN could be divided into multiple subsystems supporting different functions. By now, it is well known that the DMN is not only deactivated by tasks, but also involved in affective, mnestic, and social paradigms, among others. Nonetheless, it is starting to become clear that the array of activities in which it is involved, might also be extended to more extrinsic functions. The present meta-analytic study is meant to push this boundary a bit further. The BrainMap database was searched for all experimental paradigms activating the DMN, and their activation likelihood estimation maps were then computed. An additional map of task-induced deactivations was also created. A multidimensional scaling indicated that such maps could be arranged along an anatomo-psychological gradient, which goes from midline core activations, associated with the most internal functions, to that of lateral cortices, involved in more external tasks. Further multivariate investigations suggested that such extrinsic mode is especially related to reward, semantic, and emotional functions. However, an important finding was that the various activation maps were often different from the canonical representation of the resting-state DMN, sometimes overlapping with it only in some peripheral nodes, and including external regions such as the insula. Altogether, our findings suggest that the intrinsic–extrinsic opposition may be better understood in the form of a continuous scale, rather than a dichotomy.
Introduction
Network neuroscience has partitioned the human connectome (Sporns et al. 2005) into a set of canonical networks (Damoiseaux et al. 2006; De Luca et al. 2006; Laird et al. 2011; Yeo et al. 2011). Among these, the Default Mode Network (DMN) is given special attention. At the time of writing, 6238 published papers were returned by the PubMed search “default mode network”, while only 2951 records for “salience network”, and 3329 for “frontoparietal network”. At least in part, the interest for the DMN likely stems from its clinical relevance, since it is found to be altered in a wide range of diseases across psycho- and neuropathology (Mohan et al. 2016; Sha et al. 2018, 2019). However, despite the attention received by the scientific community, the function of DMN remains unclear. As a matter of fact, its elusiveness might be what makes investigating such brain system so compelling.
Initially, the DMN was outlined as a network specifically related to the resting state. Its first image comes from a meta-analysis by Shulman et al. (1997) about the so-called Task-Induced Deactivations (TID), depicting the areas consistently deactivated during attention-demanding tasks. Afterwards, trying to better characterize the concept of deactivation (Raichle and Snyder 2007), Raichle et al. (2001) observed that the metabolism was mostly uniform across the brain during resting state. For this reason, they suggested that the brain at rest was in a state of physiological baseline (Raichle et al. 2001; Gusnard and Raichle 2001), a default mode that represents a form of tonic activation for those regions commonly deactivated by tasks (Raichle et al. 2001). Finally, Greicius et al. (2003) proved that such default mode system was indeed a network, denoted by functional connectivity (FC) at rest. Furthermore, the DMN functional signal was reported to be negatively correlated with the signal of task-positive regions at rest (Fox et al. 2005a; Uddin et al. 2009), as well as during a task and with the experimental model itself (Greicius and Menon 2004; Golland et al. 2007; Lin et al. 2011; Newton et al. 2011). However, this rest-task distinction has soon shown its limitations, in favor of an internal–external characterization of different modes of cognition (Fransson 2005; Buckner et al. 2008; Spreng 2012; Dixon et al. 2014).
From the early beginning of the investigations, it was observed that the DMN is not deactivated by any task, as self-referential and emotional paradigms activated it (Gusnard et al. 2001). Since those observations, many further functions were shown to be associated with this network. Other than self-referential and emotional processes (Fossati et al. 2003; Ochsner et al. 2004, 2005; Northoff and Bermpohl 2004; Northoff et al. 2006; Buckner and Carroll 2007; Uddin et al. 2007; D’Argembeau et al. 2010; Denny et al. 2012; Molnar-Szakacs and Uddin 2013; Engen et al. 2017; Satpute and Lindquist 2019; Fingelkurts et al. 2020; Knyazev et al. 2020), the DMN turned out to be related to memory and mental time-travel (Cabeza et al. 1997; Svoboda et al. 2006; Schacter et al. 2007, 2008; Addis et al. 2007; Foster et al. 2012; Yang et al. 2013; Rugg and Vilberg 2013; Spreng et al. 2015; Kim 2016; Murphy et al. 2018), mental simulation and scene construction (Hassabis and Maguire 2007; Hassabis et al. 2007; Spreng and Grady 2010; Gerlach et al. 2011), Theory of Mind (ToM) and social cognition (Saxe and Kanwisher 2003; Rilling et al. 2004; Ruby and Decety 2004; Saxe and Powell 2006; Mar 2011; Mars et al. 2012; Spreng and Andrews-Hanna 2015; Amft et al. 2015; Mwilambwe-Tshilobo and Spreng 2021), moral judgment (Greene et al. 2001; Harrison et al. 2008; Pujol et al. 2008; Bzdok et al. 2012), and semantic processing (Binder et al. 1999; Chiou et al. 2020; Evans et al. 2020; Lanzoni et al. 2020). However, most of these psychological functions can still be somewhat associated with the resting state, in the sense that these activities might be easily carried out during rest. In fact, the resting mind has not to be considered as idle (Raichle 2009), but it is continuously involved in activities collectively known as mind-wandering (Giambra 1989; Spiers and Maguire 2006; Mason et al. 2007; Fox et al. 2015; Seli et al. 2016). During mind-wandering, the brain is expected to engage in DMN-related functions such as remembering the past, imagining the future, thinking about others, and displacing the self in imaginary situations (Andreasen et al. 1995; Andrews-Hanna 2012; Andrews-Hanna et al. 2014; Christoff et al. 2016), the same functions typically associated to the DMN. When some of these activities intrude into the execution of an attentional task as stimulus-independent thoughts, the task performance may be affected by errors (Sonuga-Barke and Castellanos 2007; Smallwood et al. 2008; Prado and Weissman 2011; Kam and Handy 2013). Likewise, expected task-related activations may be found disrupted, and DMN areas activated (Weissman et al. 2006; Mason et al. 2007; Li et al. 2007; Eichele et al. 2008; Kam et al. 2011).
Still, these observations are consistent with a rest-task dichotomy, and do not really suggest a proactive role for the DMN. In contrast, it has been proposed that, during task, DMN-driven stimulus-oriented thoughts may possibly appear (Mantini and Vanduffel 2013; Sormaz et al. 2018) with the purpose of supporting task performance (Gilbert et al. 2007). As a matter of fact, the DMN has been implicated in problem-solving and creativity (Kounios et al. 2006; Gerlach et al. 2011; Abraham et al. 2012; Ellamil et al. 2012; Jung et al. 2013; Benedek et al. 2014; Mayseless et al. 2015; Marron et al. 2018; Huo et al. 2020), suggesting that its activity can also be engaged in external demands. Indeed, DMN activations can enhance cognitive control during extrinsic tasks requiring internal mentation (Spreng et al. 2014), possibly in coordination with frontoparietal control systems (Spreng et al. 2010; Cocchi et al. 2013; Gerlach et al. 2014). Furthermore, the anteromedial prefrontal cortex (amPFC), node of the DMN, was found to be activated in monitoring the external environment, contributing to faster reaction times (Gilbert et al. 2005, 2006). It also seems that the DMN is recruited in switching tasks, in the case of a demanding shift from a cognitive context to a different one (Crittenden et al. 2015). Moreover, the DMN was also found to be activated both in decision-making (Smith et al. 2021) and when subjects have to automatically apply learned rules (Vatansever et al. 2017).
The critical role of DMN for task execution is highlighted not only by activation studies, but also by functional connectivity investigations that questioned the supposed DMN anticorrelation with task execution and task-positive regions. It has been shown that nodes of the DMN are positively correlated with task-positive areas during acquisition and retrieval phases of a working memory task (Piccoli et al. 2015), as well as during the preparation phase (Koshino et al. 2011, 2014). In addition, during such tasks, the connectivity within the DMN was found to be correlated with behavioral performance (Hampson et al. 2006). Moreover, the sign and the strength of the correlations between DMN and task-positive regions remarkably varies between the nodes of these networks, and according to the tasks and the different resting-state time epochs (Chang and Glover 2010; Bluhm et al. 2011; Leech et al. 2011; Elton and Gao 2015; Dixon et al. 2017; Denkova et al. 2019). At the subject level, interactions between DMN and attentional and control networks were found during a semantic memory retrieval task (Fornito et al. 2012). In sum, there is considerable evidence that the DMN functionality is crucial, not only for internal mind-wandering, but also for the execution of extrinsic activities.
Just as its role in human cognition, the anatomical and topological representation of the DMN has proven to be puzzling. From a theoretical point of view, the DMN could be described as a whole and unfractionated network, with hub nodes in the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and more peripheral nodes in the medial and lateral temporal lobe, angular gyrus (AG), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), and inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) (Buckner et al. 2008; Yeo et al. 2011). Nonetheless, it is widely acknowledged that it can be further divided into subnetworks (Abou-Elseoud et al. 2010; Abou Elseoud et al. 2011; Yeo et al. 2011; Shirer et al. 2012; Ray et al. 2013). In this regard, Andrews-Hanna et al. (2010) reported that the subdivisions of such network might have different functions. In particular, they identified two subsystems structured around a midline core: the former, formed by the PCC was thought to be involved in self-referential processes, and the latter, composed by mPFC, has considered to be related to future-oriented thoughts (Andrews-Hanna et al. 2010). What is more, the midline core itself might be a fractionated structure. In fact, by analyzing single-subject data with minimal spatial smoothing, Braga and colleagues (Braga and Buckner 2017; Braga et al. 2019; DiNicola et al. 2020) have recently found that the DMN seems to be composed of two parallel and interdigitated networks, interleaved even within PCC and mPFC. The two subsystems were found to be related to different roles: one to social cognition and the other one to mnestic functions (DiNicola et al. 2020). Similarly, Wang et al. (2020) parcellated the DMN nodes into different parts, each one associated with a specific functional profile. Likewise, Gordon et al. (2020) were able to divide the individuals’ DMN into nine subnetworks showing differential task engagement. Thus, the most recent developments in the research of the DMN are indicating that such network, far from being a monolithic entity, consists of multiple systems with intersecting functions and anatomies (Buckner and DiNicola 2019).
The present study aims to delve into this matter, using a coordinate-based meta-analytical methodology to investigate the functions related to the activity of the DMN regions and the spatial variability associated with them. The use of a meta-analytical approach allows to overcome the heterogeneity of results, a typical issue of neuroimaging experiments (Botvinik-Nezer et al. 2020). After all, the DMN research has a long meta-analytical tradition. In fact, the first images of the network come from meta-analyses of TID (Shulman et al. 1997; Mazoyer et al. 2001), subsequently confirmed by an Activation Likelihood Estimation (ALE) meta-analysis by Laird et al. (2009). Another ALE study (Schilbach et al. 2012) showed that the areas of TID, social cognition, and emotional processing converged on PCC and mPFC. Similarly, an ALE meta-analysis from Spreng et al. (2009) noted a correspondence between autobiographical memory, spatial navigation, theory of mind activations, and TID. It could be said that these two latter studies used a meta-analytic approach to put forward a consistent and comprehensive view of DMN functions. On the contrary, the current study wants to highlight the functional variety of the DMN.
To make these assessments, we performed a Paradigm Analysis (Lancaster et al. 2012), capitalizing on the BrainMap database and on its taxonomy of behavioral ontologies (Fox and Lancaster 2002; Fox et al. 2005b; Laird et al. 2005). This allowed us to identify the task categories significantly associated with the network in a data-driven fashion. Activation coordinates of such paradigms were then obtained from the same database, and used to perform an ALE meta-analysis for each one of them. As indicated by Raichle et al. (2001), TID correspond to rest tonic activations, and it has been suggested that the rest should be seen just as another active state (Buckner et al. 2013). Thus, a TID ALE map was calculated to represent the DMN configuration during resting state. This also constitutes a replication of Laird et al. (2009) with a larger database and updated algorithms. The resulting set of maps underwent a series of analyses, namely, Multidimensional Scaling (MDS), Principal Component Analysis (PCA), and Independent Component Analysis (ICA). These were to explain the DMN task-based variability in the form of axes along which the network arranges itself to meet external demands. We expected that different operative domains related to the DMN would recruit distinctive sets of areas, including some regions typically assigned to other resting-state networks (RSNs). Given the wide range of functions implicated with the mPFC (Delgado et al. 2016; Schneider and Koenigs 2017; Hiser and Koenigs 2018; Lieberman et al. 2019; Toro-Serey et al. 2020), we anticipated that this region would express a large variability in its activations, possibly organized in a rostral-caudal arc revolving around the callosal genu (Amodio and Frith 2006). The PCC might show some internal differentiation as well (Leech et al. 2011).
Methods
Paradigm Analysis
The Paradigm Analysis (Lancaster et al. 2012), as implemented in the dedicated plugin for Mango (http://ric.uthscsa.edu/mango/) (Lancaster et al. 2010, 2011), was performed to get the profile of involvement of the DMN with different fMRI paradigms. The BrainMap database is coded with an articulated cognitive, anatomic and operational taxonomy (Fox et al. 2005b). In particular, each experimental contrast is characterized by the domain of the task utilized. A complete list of paradigms with detailed definitions can be found at: http://www.brainmap.org/taxonomy/paradigms.html. Given a region of interest (ROI), the Paradigm Analysis leverages on the BrainMap database to test if executing a task significantly activates that area, versus a null model of spatial uniformity of that paradigm’s activations. The output of the technique is a series of z-scores for each paradigm, whose significance is set above the Bonferroni-corrected threshold of z = 3.3. A paradigm being significant in a ROI does not mean that it activates only that area and not the rest of the brain, but that there is a density of activation higher than chance within the mask. Thus, we are not looking for the tasks activating specifically the DMN, but simply the task activating the DMN more than chance, while other RSNs might be activated as well.
Of note, numerous and different functional parcellations of the human brain exist, and there are no methodological criteria or gold standard to prefer one to the others (Eickhoff et al. 2015; Arslan et al. 2018). Therefore, to maximize the representativity of our results, three different masks of the DMN obtained with different methodologies were fed to the Paradigm Analysis. The paradigms that were found significantly associated with at least two out of these three masks, were taken in consideration in the subsequent analyses. The first ROI was extracted from the seven Network parcellation by Yeo et al. (2011), which was produced with a clustering algorithm. The second one was derived from the ICA by Shirer et al. (2012), merging the originally split ventral and dorsal components of the DMN. Lastly, we selected the DMN from the CAREN 5 atlas by Doucet et al. (2019), which was obtained by the consensus between six different parcellations. Since the Paradigm Analysis tool works in Talairach space, the three masks were registered to Talairach using FLIRT (Jenkinson et al. 2002).
Activation likelihood estimation and fail-safe analysis
To trace the studies related to the 8 significant paradigms identified by such consensus approach (see Sect. 3.1), the software Sleuth has then been used to search the BrainMap functional database (Fox and Lancaster 2002; Fox et al. 2005b; Laird et al. 2005). For each one of the paradigms individuated with the method detailed above, the queries were composed as follows:
[Experiment Context is Normal Mapping] AND [Experiment Activation is Activations Only] AND [Experiment Paradigm Class is …]
where the latter field was completed with the given paradigm. Furthermore, to obtain the TID, we replicated the search used by Laird et al. (Laird et al. 2009):
[Experiment Context is Normal Mapping] AND [Experiment Activation is Deactivations Only] AND [Experiment Control is Low Level]
Coordinates were exported by Sleuth in Talairach space. To minimize within group effects and ensure independence between the observations (Turkeltaub et al. 2012; Müller et al. 2018), the experimental contrasts calculated on the same group of subjects were merged in a single set of foci, using the pertaining option in the tool.
Each one of the resulting lists of coordinates was then fed to GingerALE 3.0.2 (Turkeltaub et al. 2002; Eickhoff et al. 2009) to calculate its ALE map. A family wise error (FWE) correction for multiple comparisons was adopted (Eickhoff et al. 2012), with cluster-level inference of p < 0.05 and a cluster-forming threshold of p < 0.001.
To take into account the file-drawer effect, a Fail-Safe procedure was implemented (Acar et al. 2018). The file-drawer problem, also known as publication bias, hypothesizes the existence of unpublished null results that would reduce the representativity of a meta-analysis (Rosenthal 1979). The Fail-Safe algorithm (https://github.com/NeuroStat/GenerateNull) proposed by Acar et al. (2018) generates a given number of experiments having random foci, to be added to the dataset to be tested. The purpose of these random experiments, which do not corroborate our findings, is to simulate unpublished results. The number of foci and subjects of those experiments were such to match the distribution they had in the original data. Samartsidis et al. (2020) estimated that the file-drawer effect of the BrainMap database amounts to 6%. Thus, we decided to perform a series of Fail-Safe analyses adding the 6% and the 60% of random experiments to each dataset, so as to evaluate the robustness of our results. The outputs were two series of maps showing the activations that were still significant after the injection of the specified level of noise.
Data reduction techniques
The subsequent analyses were carried out in Python, using the NiBabel 3.2.1 package (Brett et al. 2020) to access the NIfTI file format, the NumPy library (Harris et al. 2020) to calculate the Pearson correlation coefficients and scikit-learn 0.24.1 (Pedregosa et al. 2011) to compute MDS, PCA and ICA. The Bartlett’s test and the Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin index were calculated with FactorAnalyzer (https://factor-analyzer.readthedocs.io/en/latest/index.html). Matplotlib (Hunter 2007) was used for visualizations.
To start with, the 9 ALE maps (8 paradigms and the TID) were vectorized using a Talairach standard as brain mask with 2 mm3 voxel size (https://www.brainmap.org/ale/colin_tlrc_2x2x2.nii.gz), to obtain a voxels × maps matrix. To perform the MDS, a Pearson’s correlation matrix was derived first. Then, the distance measure dij between the element i and j of the dissimilarity matrix was calculated as dij = 1—rij where r is their Pearson’s correlation, as in Kriegeskorte et al. (2008). Such distance matrix was then fed to the scikit-learn MDS algorithm. The MDS attempts to reproduce the distances of the input matrix in an Euclidean space of reduced dimensionality that preserves the original dissimilarities as closely as possible. To avoid random rotations of the solutions, the iterative MDS algorithms were initialized using PCA scores as starting condition (Bécavin et al. 2011). We calculated the solutions with 2 and 3 dimensions, calculating the standardized stress S (Kruskal 1964) to evaluate the goodness-of-fit of the two solutions. The stress was computed implementing the code proposed at: https://github.com/scikit-learn/scikit-learn/pull/10168.
The voxels × 9 maps matrix was again used as input for PCA to obtain both the principal component (PC) loadings (map coefficients for each component) and scores (voxel projections on the component). The scores were then plotted on the Talairach standard to obtain PC maps. Finally, the same voxels × maps matrix was fed to the scikit-learn FastICA algorithm (Hyvärinen and Oja 1997; Hyvarinen 1999). In doing so, we obtained the voxel scores on each independent component (IC) to be plotted in Talairach space to obtain the corresponding voxel-wise maps, as well as each map’s coefficient, or loading, on each component.
Comparison with the resting-state principal gradients
To further characterize our results, we compared our IC maps with those of the first and third principal gradients (PG) by Margulies et al. (2016). Since our maps were not restricted to the cortex, we chose to work with PG whole-brain volumes, downloaded from https://neurovault.org/collections/1598/ and converted to Talairach space with FLIRT. PG1 is known to be related to cortical hierarchy, and PG3 is taken here as another dimension of associativity. To confirm this, we performed a meta-analysis similar to that used by Margulies et al. (see Supplementary Materials for more details).
A first comparison was made correlating the ICs with the PGs, using the same mask as above (see Data reduction techniques) to exclude non-brain voxels from the computation. In addition, for a given IC and PG, we extracted the PG values corresponding to the positive part of the IC, and compared them to the remaining PG values with a two-sample t test (H0: PGIC ≤ PG¬IC). Lastly, we portrayed the heteromodality of our ICs as the percentage of their positive part overlapping with the positive voxels of the given PG. The same analyses were performed for the ALE and PC maps as well.
Results
Paradigm Analyses and activation likelihood estimations
The Paradigm Analysis results obtained for the three selected DMN masks (Yeo et al. 2011; Shirer et al. 2012; Doucet et al. 2019) are presented in Table 1. These tasks were found to activate the respective DMN ROIs significantly more than by chance, that is, the activations for a given task were found within the DMN more frequently than those expected if they were distributed randomly in the brain. The three DMN masks are shown in Supplementary Fig. S1. Eight paradigms were found to be significant for at least two out of the 3 DMN masks: ToM, Semantic Monitor/Discrimination, Episodic Recall, Emotion Induction, Self-Reflection, Deception, Imagined Object/Scenes, and Reward. Most of these paradigms are related to social, mnestic, or other internal mentation functions typically associated with the DMN (Buckner et al. 2008). To the best of our knowledge, deception tasks were not previously related to the network, although the social nature of such paradigms likely justifies this result. Similarly, we are not aware of many explicit links between reward functions and DMN in the literature (Lopez-Persem et al. 2020; Martins et al. 2021), albeit there is strong evidence to associate the mPFC to such mechanisms (Xue et al. 2009; Schneider and Koenigs 2017; Hiser and Koenigs 2018; Lieberman et al. 2019). Interestingly, reasoning and problem-solving paradigms had a significant effect on the CAREN mask (Doucet et al. 2019), suggesting that the DMN might then play a role outside of what is considered internal mentation in the strictest sense.
Details about the results from Sleuth searches for the eight queries associated with each significant paradigm are presented in Table 2 and in the Prisma Flow chart in Supplementary Fig. S2. We point out that we found a limited number of experiments for the Self-Reflection condition. According to Eickhoff and colleagues (Eickhoff et al. 2016; Müller et al. 2018), at least 17 experiments should be gathered to perform a statistically sound ALE. Although the query returned 28 experiments, 7 sets of foci remained after merging them according to groups of subjects (Müller et al. 2018). To rule out possible bias induced by the inclusion of this underpowered domain, the subsequent analyses were repeated with and without the related ALE map. Since excluding this condition did not significantly influence their outcomes, we decided to keep it in the dataset.
We also computed a TID meta-analysis, representing rest. The ALE results are presented in Fig. 1. The TID ALE map replicates the one by Laird et al. (2009), with the exception of the mPFC cluster, that we found in a more dorsal position.
As for the other maps, Episodic Recall activates the left insula along with DMN areas. Imagining objects and scenes activate the left hemisphere in the precuneus, lateral prefrontal cortex (lPFC) including IFG, and supplementary motor area (SMA). Deception tasks activate the bilateral insula, other than AG and SMA, in accordance to Farah et al. (2014). Semantic monitoring and discrimination activate the left lPFC including IFG, but also the SMA, the left lateral and medial temporal lobe, and the PCC. This map is very similar to a recent ALE about semantic control (Jackson 2021), with the exception of PCC, which is a novel result. Reward activates the basal ganglia (BG), the thalamus, the whole mPFC, and the bilateral insula. ToM activates the temporal and temporoparietal cortices along with PCC and dorsomedial PFC (dmPFC), especially on the left. Inducing emotion activates the left insula and the bilateral occipito-temporal cortices, along with typical DMN nodes. The Self-Reflection map involves PCC, the right SMA, and an occipital cluster in the fusiform gyrus. The maps obtained by the Fail-safe analyses, presented in Supplementary Fig. S3 and S4, show that our ALE maps remain substantially unchanged when accounting for the file-drawer effect.
In summary, few maps matched the prototypical representation of the DMN. Some of them showed either weak activation or no activation at all in the midline core, and a strong expression of lateral areas of the network such as AG, IFG, and middle temporal gyrus. In addition, the insula and SMA/dorsal ACC, hubs of the salience network (SN), were often present. Figure 2 shows the proportions of voxels of each ALE map falling within each one of the RSNs of the three functional parcellations. The overlap between the ALE maps of the main analysis and the three DMN masks is shown in Supplementary Fig. S5. The Jaccard indices between the main analysis ALE maps and the various RSN reported by Shirer et al. Yeo et al. and Doucet et al. are presented in Supplementary Table S1 and Fig. S6.
Pie charts with the proportions of each ALE voxels for each one of the resting-state networks proposed by Shirer et al., Yeo et al., and Doucet et al. As reference, each parcellation is presented in form of volume renderings. ECN executive control network, sal salience network, precun precuneus, BG basal ganglia, visuospat visuospatial network, sensmot sensorimotor network. The ECN by Shirer et al. roughly corresponds to the frontoparietal network by Yeo et al. and Doucet et al. The ventral attention network by Yeo et al. includes the salience network. The language network by Shirer et al. includes large parts of the DMN as depicted by others
Multidimensional scaling
The standardized stress of the 2D solution is S = 0.25, which is considered as a “poor” solution (Kruskal 1964), while the 3D solution has a “fair” stress of S = 0.15. Thus, only the 3D solution will be considered between the main results. The 1 − r dissimilarity matrix (Kriegeskorte et al. 2008) between maps and the resulting 3-dimensional MDS can be seen in Fig. 3 (the 2D solution is presented in Supplementary Fig. S7). The MDS graphs obtained excluding the Self-Reflection condition are presented in Supplementary Fig. S8. Both with and without the Self-Reflection map, the 3D MDS solution seems to suggest a first axis standing for medial–lateral, or a core-lPFC, spatial representation. The maps found on one side of the axis (e.g., TID, ToM, and Episodic Recall) show activations in the midline DMN core, while those found on the other side (e.g., Deception, Imagined Objects/Scenes, Semantic Monitor/Discrimination) display a weaker involvement of such regions, especially PCC. Conversely, the latter have significant clusters in the lPFC and insula, and the midline activations are especially located in the SMA. The maps activating the midline core are not only those presenting more similarity with its stereotypical image, but also those whose functions were more commonly associated with the DMN (Buckner et al. 2008). As for the opposite side, these maps are related to tasks more rarely associated with the network, and they bear a similarity with the spatial distribution of SN, rather than the one of DMN. Moreover, they are reminiscent of the semantic regions (i.e., SemN) found by Chiou et al. (2020) as belonging to a more outward-leaning DMN subsystem (see also Evans et al. 2020). Hence, this anatomical midline-lateral axis could also be seen as a psychological internal/external dimension. As for the second axis, the distribution of the maps may suggest a dorsal–ventral labeling. On one side, activations are more focused in SMA, PCC, or dorsal frontal and parietal cortices. On the other side, there are insular, temporopolar, and medial temporal areas. The third axis added in the 3D solution does not have an obvious explanation. A quantitative analysis confirming our interpretation is presented in the Supplementary Materials at Table S2 and Fig. S9.
Multidimensional scaling (MDS) of the nine activation likelihood estimation (ALE) maps. A: 1 − r distance matrix of the nine ALE maps. B First two axis of the MDS 3-dimensional solution. ALEs volume mappings are shown next to their respective MDS coordinates. C First and third axis of the MDS 3-dimensional solution. D MDS 3-dimensional solution, seen from different perspectives. From left to right, the views are progressively rotated for a better understanding of tridimensionality
Component decompositions
Given the lack of common ground between different DMN expressions, we performed a PCA to summarize the inter-paradigm similarities. Even if the high significance of the Bartlett’s test (p < 0.001) seems to suggest otherwise, the Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin index (KMO = 0.61) indicates a sufficient but mediocre relation between the maps. The first four principal components (PC) explain 96.3% of the variance, and loads particularly on Reward (PC1, 51.8% of explained variance), Semantic Monitor/Discrimination (PC2, 30.5%), Emotion Induction (PC3, 10.1%), TID and ToM (PC4, 4%). Supplementary Fig. S10 shows the PC maps and respective loadings. The PCA results computed excluding the Self-Reflection map were identical (not shown).
As the PCA indicated that four principal components provide a significant decomposition of the variance, we selected a four-component solution for the ICA as well (Fig. 4). When excluding the Self-Reflection map from the data, the ICA results did not change (not shown). The independent components (IC) were similar to those of the PCA. Thus, for an easier argumentation, we ordered them so as to match the principal ones.
IC1 is associated with Reward and shows positive coefficients in most mPFC, anterior PCC, insula, and thalamus/BG. However, it presents negative values with dmPFC, PCC proper, occipito-temporal cortices, and amygdala. IC2, related to Semantic Monitor/Discrimination, shows positive coefficients especially in the left hemisphere, in a cluster formed by the PFC (including IFG), AI, and most of the premotor cortex, as well as in the temporal cortex, SMA and PCC. Large parts of the midline display negative values, in particular the dmPFC, as the component loads negatively to Emotion Induction and this area is activated by such task (Fig. 1). IC3, associated with Emotion Induction, is positively associated with AI, temporo-occipital cortices, dmPFC, a small part of PCC, thalamus and amygdala. Negative coefficients are closely juxtaposed to positive areas, in BG, around PCC, in prefrontal and temporal cortices. IC4 is linked to TID and ToM, and it is clearly expressed in the canonical DMN regions and anticorrelated with the insula, SMA, and amygdala.
Intriguingly, when the ICA maps were fed back into a Paradigm Analysis (positive voxels only), the output returned a much longer list of significant experimental tasks (Supplementary Table S3 and Fig. S11), comprising the paradigms heavily loaded by the component. IC4 represents an exception, as it gives only few paradigms other than those found by the three original DMN masks. This is not surprising, as IC4 is associated with TID. This analysis suggests that IC1, IC2, and IC3, compared to the canonical DMN, are more closely associated with different modes of extrinsic cognition.
Exploratory analyses pointed out that solutions with more than five components produced patchy maps that may be considered as noise, or as evidence that our data have no information to be further decomposed. The five-component solution was rather similar to the one presented here, except for TID and ToM split into two different components. The ToM component involves a smaller and more dorsal mPFC cluster, and a more posterior temporoparietal cluster than the TID one (Supplementary Fig. S12). On the contrary, solutions two and three are not particularly meaningful. They showed maps similar to the canonical DMN or its anti-network, but their components were unable to load on more than a paradigm each one (Supplementary Fig. S13).
Comparison with the resting-state principal gradients
The first PG by Margulies et al. (2016) is a non-linear component resembling the DMN and accounting for most resting-state signal variance, but it has also been shown to be related to cortical hierarchy, and thus interpreted as a model of heteromodal representation (Mesulam, 1998). The third one, conversely, shows resemblance with the task-positive network (TPN) by Fox et al. (2005a). The two PG maps are shown in Supplementary Fig. S14. A series of Behavioral Analyses (see Supplementary Materials for methods) confirmed that the whole-brain PG1 is anchored at one side to regions with primary functions and at the other to associative areas. The same method illustrates a sort of bimodal distribution of PG3 heteromodality, with DMN areas, at the lower part of the scale, associated with social cognition and emotions, and TPN regions, at the top, related to attention, working memory and reasoning, with primary functions half-way (Supplementary Fig. S14). Thus, PG3 could be used to distinguish between socioaffective and executive associative areas.
The correlations between our four ICs and these two PGs is presented in Table 3. Most correlation approaches 0, except for the one between IC1 and PG3 (r = 0.20), and those between IC4 and PG1 and PG3 (r = 0.18 and r = − 0.16, respectively). According to Cohen (1992), r = 0.10 is to be considered a small effect size, while a medium effect size equates to r = 0.30. This suggests that resting-state variance is not able to recapitulate brain activity fully.
However, a series of two-sample t tests indicates that the average gradient value of positive IC voxels was significantly higher than that of the rest of the brain (p ≈ 0 for all ICs and with both PGs). Figure 5 illustrates the overlaps between the positive voxels of the four IC and the two PGs. The percentage of positive voxels of each IC overlapping with those of the two PGs is reported in Table 3. All ICs overlap with at least 50% of their positive voxels with heteromodal regions as defined by PG1, and IC1 also shows high convergence with PG3.
Surface mapping of the overlaps between the volumes of positive voxels of the four ICs and PG1 and 3 by Margulies et al. (2016). For each comparison, the ratio of positive IC voxels overlapping with the positive PG is reported
The comparisons of PGs with ALE and PC maps produced similar results: low correlations and high overlaps. All the two-sample t tests were highly significant. Correlations and percentages of overlap are shown in Supplementary Table S4. Lastly, we tested if the correlations between ALE maps and PGs could, in turn, be correlated with their placement on the MDS x axis, that is, if the alignment between an ALE map and a PG could be predictive of its placement on our internal–external gradient. MDS x coordinates correlate strongly and significantly with PG3 (r = − 0.69; p = 0.02, one-tailed t-test), but not so much with PG1 (r = 0.43; p = 0.12). This means that the internal–external axis is mostly orthogonal with heteromodality, but is related to a socioaffective-executive gradient.
Discussion
The present work provides compelling evidence that regions of the DMN are engaged in several tasks, not restricted to those conventionally associated with the resting state and the mind-wandering, and also including semantic reasoning and reward mechanisms. In light of the resulting ALE maps, it is important to bear in mind that all these paradigms were found to activate at least two out of three DMN masks more than chance. Thus, the density of their foci in literature is particularly high within the DMN. However, the density in other RSNs might be just as high as in the DMN. Furthermore, there was no reason to always find meta-analytic activations in the central hubs of the network. In fact, while most maps show at least a cluster in the PCC of mPFC, Imagined Objects/Scenes and Deception seem to involve only areas such as IFG, the precuneus, AG. Conversely, many maps include non-DMN areas associated with task-positive networks such as the SN and frontoparietal network (FPN). This means that there are tasks involving nodes of the DMN along with those included in other networks, thus forming patterns of activation that cross the boundaries of canonical RSNs.
It has been argued that to assess its function, a network should be assessed considering it a spatially and temporally coherent system as a whole, and not in terms of its constituting regions (Jackson et al. 2019). Following this approach, Jackson et al. (2019) reported that the coherent DMN is not involved in semantic cognition, concluding that the DMN and SemN are two distinct networks. However, such consideration could be extended to other cognitive domains, leading to the paradox that no function should be associated to the DMN. In fact, our results indicate that DMN task-related activations span across RSNs, suggesting that canonical networks might have limited heuristic value to understand brain functioning when involved in extrinsic processing. Therefore, task engagement may induce the cooperation between areas belonging to different canonical RSNs. Although relying on intrinsic brain topology, such recruitment would not be strictly constrained by it (Cole et al. 2014; Krienen et al. 2014). Thus, it might involve a flexible shift in brain hubness (Cole et al. 2013; Fransson and Thompson 2020) and a remodulation of cooperative and competitive long-range connectivity patterns (Fornito et al. 2012; Piccoli et al. 2015; Dixon et al. 2017).
There are several obvious reasons explaining why canonical RSNs are a simplistic representation of brain complexity (Pessoa 2014). For instance, they partition the gray matter into non-overlapping volumes, even if in the brain, as in most real-world networks, a node is usually connected to more than one community (Palla et al. 2005; Ferrarini et al. 2009; Yeo et al. 2014; Najafi et al. 2016). In addition, FC changes from rest to task (Arbabshirani et al. 2013; Mennes et al. 2013; Spreng et al. 2013; Cole et al. 2014; Goparaju et al. 2014; Vatansever et al. 2015a, b; Krieger-Redwood et al. 2016; Najafi et al. 2016; Bolt et al. 2017; Moraschi et al. 2020) and dynamically fluctuates over time (Chang and Glover 2010; Hutchison et al. 2013; Calhoun et al. 2014; Preti et al. 2017). The non-stationarity of FC indicates a fluid node recruitment by whole-brain connectivity modules, resulting in time-varying networks. In this regard, de Pasquale et al. (2012) found the DMN to be the system most connected with extra-network regions during epochs of strong internal correlation. More in general, several dynamic FC studies (Chang and Glover 2010; Kiviniemi et al. 2011; Liu and Duyn 2013; Karahanoğlu and Van De Ville 2015) portrayed the DMN as a moving landscape, with a changing spatial distribution and whole-brain correlations over time.
Thus, it is not puzzling that our maps did not reproduce the resting-state DMN. Still, it is interesting to notice that not even TID or the functions more canonically associated with the network are able to show a strong affinity with the DMN masks (Supplementary Table S1 and S4, and Fig. S5 and S6). The lack of a perfect match between the phenomenon of activations (and deactivations) and FC suggests that the resting-state connectivity, while usually regarded as intrinsic (Fox et al. 2007; Vincent et al. 2007; Van Dijk et al. 2010; Cole et al. 2014, 2016; Tavor et al. 2016), can inform us only partially about brain functioning. This reasoning does not invoke a complete abandonment of RSNs, as it can be argued that they maintain a sort of taxonomic utility, in the sense that they are well-known structures that help to categorize brain anatomy and topology (Uddin et al. 2019). In this regard, it makes sense to investigate the functions of the resting-state DMN. For instance, it may be reasonable to conclude that the canonical DMN could be seen as an entity having the role of integrating different cognitive aspects thanks to the interactions between its subcomponents (Wen et al. 2020). This is in line with the observation that the DMN displays a diversified cytoarchitecture and connectivity with the rest of the brain (Paquola et al. 2021). It is also consistent with its strong dynamical connectivity with external nodes, including task-positive regions (de Pasquale et al. 2012; Karahanoğlu and Van De Ville 2015), which might explain their involvement in our maps. These considerations also resonate with the theoretical proposals that the function of DMN might be that of reducing the entropy or the free energy of the brain (Carhart-Harris and Friston 2010; Carhart-Harris et al. 2014), or that of a sense-making network integrating new information with past knowledge (Yeshurun et al. 2021), in agreement with its dense connectivity and its topologically central position in the connectome (van den Heuvel and Sporns 2011; Tomasi and Volkow 2011). In any case, besides any possible interpretation, our findings highlight that the tasks that significantly activate the canonical DMN cannot be simply reduced to it, and that they show anatomical and functional diversity between each other.
Although these tasks may be easily linked to certain internal forms of mentation, the nature of some of them highlight that such internal cognition seems to be crucial for external tasks. It is important to point out that since we tested the DMN for significant paradigms, the resulting ALE maps represent the activations of a specific set of experimental tasks, defined in an operational way. On the contrary, psychological definitions such as those implemented by the Behavioral Analysis (Lancaster et al. 2012) span across paradigms. For instance, the behavioral domain of Semantics entails Covert Word Generation, Self-Reflection, Encoding, Passive Listening, Visuospatial Attention, and other tasks. These range from the most extrinsic and active functions to forms of internal cognition that may be involved in mind-wandering. Therefore, working at the level of paradigms allowed us to focus on the kind of activities the DMN regions are engaged in.
For instance, we found Reward paradigms to be associated with canonical DMN. This is mostly due to a large amount of foci covering the whole mPFC. The latter is known to modulate reward mechanisms (Ferenczi et al. 2016), to respond to the outcome of risky decisions (Xue et al. 2009), and to activate when receiving a social reward (Martins et al. 2021). Reward mechanisms could be considered as an example of functions meant to monitor internal states, yet crucial for the implementation of behavior. As a matter of fact, reward dynamics involve the perception of somatic states and their emotional and self-related processing, which implies the activity of mPFC, ACC, SMA, along with BG, amygdala, and insula (Verdejo-García and Bechara 2009; but see also Dunn et al. 2006). At the same time, reward functions are critical for learning, risk-taking, and behavior in general (Schultz 2015). Furthermore, as an example of collaboration between internal and external cognition, the reward system is functionally connected to the DMN during mental simulation of the outcome of goal-directed behavior (Gerlach et al. 2014).
Deception is another paradigm class arguably standing in between internal and external cognition. Deceiving someone requires social cognition, which is typically associated with the DMN internal mentation (Buckner et al. 2008). However, it might be argued that deceiving is a more external activity than ToM. While the latter only requires to represent other people’s mental states, the former also implies goal-directed programming of one’s own behavior to successfully deceive the other (Lisofsky et al. 2014). Moreover, specific attentional and executive functions are likely to be necessary to perform a deception paradigm (Christ et al. 2009; Farah et al. 2014). As a matter of fact, ToM appeared close to TID on the first MDS axis and Deception was at the extreme opposite side.
In general, the MDS results suggested that the DMN-related experimental paradigms and their associated activation maps could be arranged along an internal–external, midline-lateral axis. This observation is consistent with the growing body of evidence pointing out that the DMN is recruited during task execution (Crittenden et al. 2015; Vatansever et al. 2017; Murphy et al. 2018), and suggests that its function may be related to some form of high-level cognition, detached from the here and now, but still crucial for goal-directed behavior (Konishi et al. 2015; Benedek et al. 2016). At the same time, our results also clearly illustrate that when engaged in external operations, the network activations shift from the spatial representation typical of the rest condition. In fact, the ALE maps associated with more extrinsic paradigms display a clear dissimilarity from the canonical representation of the DMN. Specifically, they involve peripheral nodes of the system such as AG and IFG, they often show weak or no activation at all within the midline core, and they sometimes engage SN and FPN considered to be anticorrelated to the DMN at rest (Fox et al. 2005a).
To summarize the anatomical variance of such extrinsic and intrinsic brain activity, we performed a PCA and an ICA. A large part of the task-related variance is explained in the form of proactive modes of internal cognition (PC1, Reward; PC2, Semantic Monitor/Discrimination), in opposition to core areas active during rest and social cognition paradigms (PC4). The four-component ICA replicated the PCA results closely, with the meaningful difference that the first two components were found to be anticorrelated with regions associated with Emotion Induction. Therefore, affective functions may constitute an important factor of DMN reorganization during task execution. We also note that PC2 and IC2 (but PC1 as well) resemble the SemN (Noonan et al. 2013; Chiou et al. 2020; Evans et al. 2020; Milton et al. 2021). Furthermore, they remind the transitional module serving an integrative function with FPN observed by Fornito et al. (2012), as well as with the Overlapping Community six found by Najafi et al. (2016). Consequently, we suspect that they both serve some form of integration between the DMN core and the task-positive areas for the execution of more external tasks.
It has been proposed that the DMN topography might be crucial to understand its function: its placement at the furthest distance from areas with primary functions could imply that it carries out the most abstract representations and that it integrates the broadest range of information (Smallwood et al. 2021). According to this, it could be expected that our ICs of DMN activity were somewhat related to PG1 by Margulies et al. (2016), which is associated with the topography of cortical hierarchy while explaining most of the resting-state variance. However, none of our ICs (or ALE maps) showed even a mediocre correlation with PG1, and, most importantly, the similarity of ALE maps with PG1 was not correlated with the first MDS axis. This does not imply that PGs (or, once again, resting-state FC in general) are not relevant for the understanding of brain activity. On the basis of the existing literature (Smith et al. 2009; Laird et al. 2013), we would expect that if data reduction techniques were applied to a wider meta-analytic repertoire of functions, the resulting components would match the resting-state PGs much better. However, it points out that the DMN-to-primary areas PG1 is not sufficient to predict DMN active modes, but just to identify the associative regions that such modes are more likely to load. Therefore, we might be in need of more detailed models to understand brain dynamical reorganizations. In line with this, recent work by Paquola et al. (2021) reports that DMN connectivity is aligned to a cytoarchitectural axis rather than with PG1. Interestingly, the similarity between our ALE maps and the TPN-to-DMN PG3 correlates with our external-to-internal MDS axis, suggesting that a socioaffective-executive differentiation between heteromodal regions might play a role in explaining DMN functional rearrangements.
The absence of the ventromedial PFC (vmPFC) from our results deserves to be briefly commented. The only difference between our TID ALE map and that by Laird et al. (2009) is that our mPFC cluster does not include the most ventral areas of the region. Of the other maps, only Reward involves vmPFC. IC1 (and PC1) includes the vmPFC, but IC4 (and PC4), which loads on TID and ToM, does not. This is a bit surprising, as the vmPFC is considered part of the DMN, and it is present in the ROIs utilized for the Paradigm Analysis (Supplementary Fig. S1). A possible explanation might be related to an attenuation of the detected effects in this area by the studies included in the meta-analysis, as the result of strategies having the purpose to minimize artifacts, which are common in the most ventral parts of the brain. However, it would be unclear how this could explain the difference between our findings and those by Laird and colleagues. The difference may be explained by our larger database and updated ALE algorithms (Eickhoff et al. 2016, 2017). If the vmPFC absence from our maps was justified, it would be possible that it would be another difference between activations and connectivity.
An unexpected finding was that our meta-analysis was powerful enough to produce juxtapositions of components that were reminiscent of the works by Braga and colleagues (Braga and Buckner 2017; Braga et al. 2019; DiNicola et al. 2020). As their results were originally obtained with minimally smoothed individual data, it is remarkable that something similar was achieved by our method. Another recent meta-analysis (Ngo et al. 2019) obtained a similar result, decomposing the inter-experiment DMN variability in two components. However, our methodology was able to highlight sharp contrasts between neighboring areas just analyzing the final ALE maps. This is particularly evident for PC3 and IC3, both related to Emotion Induction. In both components, the mPFC is parcellated in alternated bands of network and anti-network, with SMA and anterior dmPFC positively associated with the paradigm, and posterior dmPFC and central mPFC showing negative values. The opposite pattern was shown by IC1, related to Reward. The PCC was tightly segmented as well, particularly in IC2 and IC3, where a small section of positive voxels was surrounded by negative values. More in general, PC2, PC3, IC2, and IC3 indicated a preferential engagement of a more posterior portion of PCC in semantic monitoring and induction of emotions, with negative scores in a more anterior part. On the contrary, reward mechanisms showed the opposite pattern in IC1 and, to some extent, in PC1. Such rostro-caudal segmentation of the PCC was also observed by Leech and colleagues during the execution of an attentional task, with the caudal portion displaying less integration with the DMN and less segregation with the task-positive regions (Leech et al. 2011). To summarize, the midline core, clearly associated with TID (PC4 and IC4), appeared much more jagged in other components, presenting patterns of correlation and anticorrelation in a gradient around the corpus callosum.
Limitations and future directions
The main limitation of this work derived from the choice of using the BrainMap database as the only source of activation foci. This was done for the sake of consistency, as the tasks significantly associated with the DMN were found using the Paradigm Analysis, which operates on the BrainMap database. Obviously, we could have integrated our data with experiments found through a systematic search on PubMed. However, a larger dataset could have possibly translated into ALE maps in disagreement with the Paradigm Analysis results, for instance without any significant activation within DMN nodes. Importantly, this would have not been necessarily due to a better representativity of the larger database, but possibly just because of a different coding of the paradigms. Having to choose between internal consistency and a larger sample size, and considering the amplitude of the functional BrainMap data archive (more than 18,000 experiments in total), we preferred to conduct our whole research within the same database. This choice returned an underpowered Self-Reflection ALE. However, by removing it from the data, we obtained a similar MDS and identical PCA and ICA results. Moreover, an exploratory ALE using a liberal uncorrected threshold with p = 0.001 (not shown) revealed additional clusters in the dmPFC, mPFC, dlPFC, left insula, and IFG. A similar map would be rather consistent with our general results. Therefore, a more representative Self-Reflection map would probably be more heavily loaded by those components representing the DMN internal modes of cognition such as PC4 and IC4, rather than leading to radically different findings.
Our results indicate that several co-activation networks converge on the resting-state DMN nodes. For instance, the Semantic Monitor/Discrimination ALE map seems to indicate that the hubness of the DMN has been moved from the midline core to the left IFG and middle temporal gyrus, which are peripheral nodes during rest, and to left insula and SMA, these latter parts of the SN. This evidence suggests that during task execution the nodes of the DMN could update their FC and dynamically modify their topological centrality, as observed by Cole et al. (2013) for the FPN. However, the present work did not directly test this hypothesis. In particular, during semantic monitoring tasks, left IFG could be coupled with the middle temporal cortex, and insula with SMA, forming two relatively independent modules. Alternatively, they could be all reciprocally co-activated in a rich-club fashion. The methods used in the present research cannot disambiguate between these and other possible hypotheses. Thus, future works could be addressed towards the implementation of some methods to estimate networks of co-activations from meta-analytical data (Toro et al. 2008; De La Vega et al. 2016; Mancuso et al. 2019; Cauda et al. 2020) so as to assess the centrality of the nodes across tasks. Task-based stationary or dynamic FC could be used as well.
The present study raises a compelling question concerning the mechanism arranging the dynamical shifts from the midline core during task. An influential model proposed that the anterior insula could be responsible for coordinating the interplay between the DMN and frontoparietal task-positive regions (Sridharan et al. 2008; Menon and Uddin 2010). The insula was actually found by Najafi et al. (2016) to be connected to several modules despite a relatively low degree centrality, both during rest and emotional tasks. The AG was implicated in the same role and identified, by Kernbach et al. (2018), as the mediator of the interplay between different RSNs. Alternatively, the amPFC was shown to be activated during switches between stimulus-independent and stimulus-oriented thoughts (Gilbert et al. 2005), suggesting to play a role in the coordination of internal and external modes of mentation. Future studies could further investigate the issue to clarify which areas or mechanisms are involved in the task-based DMN rearrangements.
Conclusions
The present study indicates a series of tasks activating the DMN that are not exactly internal, nor completely external. These activations involve DMN regions but also large parts of other canonical RSNs, in particular SN and FPN. Furthermore, they appear to be arranged in an anatomo-psychological gradient starting from the most internal functions, which activate the midline core, towards such relatively extrinsic mode of brain function, which involves the lateral cortices. In the light of our results, such extrinsic mode is especially related to reward, semantic, and emotional functions.
Ultimately, our findings highlight that resting-state scaffoldings do not suffice to explain the task-related anatomical variance of the active brain, which displays a much richer functional diversity, and shows more spatial complexity than it could be expected just observing intrinsic connectivity.
Data and code availability of data and material
Data used in this meta-analysis were obtained from BrainMap (http://brainmap.org/), a publicly available database. Analyses were performed with open source software. The results are available at https://github.com/SCCabanillas/DMN-task-based-modulation.
References
Brett M, Markiewicz CJ, Hanke M et al (2020) nipy/nibabel: 3.2.1. 10.5281/ZENODO.4295521
Abou-Elseoud A, Starck T, Remes J et al (2010) The effect of model order selection in group PICA. Hum Brain Mapp 31:1207–1216. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20929
Abou Elseoud A, Littow H, Remes J et al (2011) Group-ICA model order highlights patterns of functional brain connectivity. Front Syst Neurosci 5:1–18. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2011.00037
Abraham A, Pieritz K, Thybusch K et al (2012) Creativity and the brain: uncovering the neural signature of conceptual expansion. Neuropsychologia 50:1906–1917. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.04.015
Acar F, Seurinck R, Eickhoff SB, Moerkerke B (2018) Assessing robustness against potential publication bias in activation likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analyses for fMRI. PLoS ONE 13:1–23. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208177
Addis DR, Wong AT, Schacter DL (2007) Remembering the past and imagining the future: Common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration. Neuropsychologia 45:1363–1377. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.016
Amft M, Bzdok D, Laird AR et al (2015) Definition and characterization of an extended social-affective default network. Brain Struct Funct 220:1031–1049. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-013-0698-0
Amodio DM, Frith CD (2006) Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 7:268–277. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1884
Andreasen NC, O’Leary DS, Cizadlo T et al (1995) Remembering the past: two facets of episodic memory explored with positron emission tomography. Am J Psychiatry 152:1576–1585. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.152.11.1576
Andrews-Hanna JR (2012) The brain’s default network and its adaptive role in internal mentation. Neurosci 18:251–270. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858411403316
Andrews-Hanna JR, Reidler JS, Sepulcre J et al (2010) Functional-anatomic fractionation of the brain’s default network. Neuron 65:550–562. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.005
Andrews-Hanna JR, Smallwood J, Spreng RN (2014) The default network and self-generated thought: component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1316:29–52. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12360
Arbabshirani MR, Havlicek M, Kiehl KA et al (2013) Functional network connectivity during rest and task conditions: a comparative study. Hum Brain Mapp 34:2959–2971. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22118
Arslan S, Ktena SI, Makropoulos A et al (2018) Human brain mapping: a systematic comparison of parcellation methods for the human cerebral cortex. Neuroimage 170:5–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.04.014
Bécavin C, Tchitchek N, Mintsa-Eya C et al (2011) Improving the efficiency of multidimensional scaling in the analysis of high-dimensional data using singular value decomposition. Bioinformatics 27:1413–1421. https://doi.org/10.1093/bioinformatics/btr143
Benedek M, Beaty R, Jauk E et al (2014) Creating metaphors: the neural basis of figurative language production. Neuroimage 90:99–106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.046
Benedek M, Jauk E, Beaty RE et al (2016) Brain mechanisms associated with internally directed attention and self-generated thought. Sci Rep 6:1–8. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep22959
Binder J, Frost J, Hammeke T et al (1999) Conceptual processing during the conscious resting state: a functional MRI study. J Cogn 11:80–93. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892999563265
Bluhm RL, Clark CR, Mcfarlane AC et al (2011) Default network connectivity during a working memory task. Hum Brain Mapp 32:1029–1035. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21090
Bolt T, Nomi JS, Rubinov M, Uddin LQ (2017) Correspondence between evoked and intrinsic functional brain network configurations. Hum Brain Mapp 38:1992–2007. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23500
Botvinik-Nezer R, Holzmeister F, Camerer CF et al (2020) Variability in the analysis of a single neuroimaging dataset by many teams. Nature 582:84–88. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-020-2314-9
Braga RM, Buckner RL (2017) Parallel interdigitated distributed networks within the individual estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. Neuron 95:457-471.e5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.038
Braga RM, Van Dijk KRA, Polimeni JR et al (2019) Parallel distributed networks resolved at high resolution reveal close juxtaposition of distinct regions. J Neurophysiol 121:1513–1534. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00808.2018
Buckner RL, Carroll DC (2007) Self-projection and the brain. Trends Cogn Sci 11:49–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.004
Buckner RL, DiNicola LM (2019) The brain’s default network: updated anatomy, physiology and evolving insights. Nat Rev Neurosci. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-019-0212-7
Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL (2008) The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1124:1–38. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1440.011
Buckner RL, Krienen FM, Yeo BTT (2013) Opportunities and limitations of intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Nat Neurosci 16:832–837. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3423
Bzdok D, Schilbach L, Vogeley K et al (2012) Parsing the neural correlates of moral cognition: ALE meta-analysis on morality, theory of mind, and empathy. Brain Struct Funct 217:783–796. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-012-0380-y
Cabeza R, Mangels J, Nyberg L et al (1997) Brain regions differentially involved in remembering what and when: a PET study. Neuron 19:863–870. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80967-8
Calhoun VD, Miller R, Pearlson G, Adali T (2014) The chronnectome: time-varying connectivity networks as the next frontier in fMRI data discovery. Neuron 84:262–274. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2014.10.015
Carhart-Harris RL, Friston KJ (2010) The default-mode, ego-functions and free-energy: a neurobiological account of Freudian ideas. Brain 133:1265–1283. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awq010
Carhart-Harris RL, Leech R, Hellyer PJ et al (2014) The entropic brain: a theory of conscious states informed by neuroimaging research with psychedelic drugs. Front Hum Neurosci 8:1–22. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00020
Cauda F, Mancuso L, Nani A et al (2020) Hubs of long-distance co-alteration characterize brain pathology. Hum Brain Mapp. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25093
Chang C, Glover GH (2010) Time-frequency dynamics of resting-state brain connectivity measured with fMRI. Neuroimage 50:81–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.011
Chiou R, Humphreys GF, Lambon Ralph MA (2020) Bipartite functional fractionation within the default network supports disparate forms of internally oriented cognition. Cereb Cortex 30:5484–5501. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhaa130
Christ SE, Van Essen DC, Watson JM et al (2009) The contributions of prefrontal cortex and executive control to deception: evidence from activation likelihood estimate meta-analyses. Cereb Cortex 19:1557–1566. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn189
Christoff K, Irving ZC, Fox KCR et al (2016) Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: a dynamic framework. Nat Rev Neurosci 17:718–731. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.113
Cocchi L, Zalesky A, Fornito A, Mattingley JB (2013) Dynamic cooperation and competition between brain systems during cognitive control. Trends Cogn Sci 17:493–501. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.08.006
Cohen J (1992) A power primer. Psychol Bull 112:155–159. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155
Cole MW, Reynolds JR, Power JD et al (2013) Multi-task connectivity reveals flexible hubs for adaptive task control. Nat Neurosci 16:1348–1355. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3470
Cole MW, Bassett DS, Power JD et al (2014) Intrinsic and task-evoked network architectures of the human brain. Neuron 83:238–251. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.014
Cole MW, Ito T, Bassett DS, Schultz DH (2016) Activity flow over resting-state networks shapes cognitive task activations. Nat Neurosci 19:1718–1726. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4406
Crittenden BM, Mitchell DJ, Duncan J (2015) Recruitment of the default mode network during a demanding act of executive control. Elife 2015:1–12. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.06481
D’Argembeau A, Stawarczyk D, Majerus S et al (2010) The neural basis of personal goal processing when envisioning future events. J Cogn Neurosci 22:1701–1713. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21314
Damoiseaux JS, Rombouts SARB, Barkhof F et al (2006) Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects. Proc Natl Acad Sci 103:13848–13853. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0601417103
De La Vega A, Chang LJ, Banich MT et al (2016) Large-scale meta-analysis of human medial frontal cortex reveals tripartite functional organization. J Neurosci 36:6553–6562. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4402-15.2016
De Luca M, Beckmann CF, De Stefano N et al (2006) fMRI resting state networks define distinct modes of long-distance interactions in the human brain. Neuroimage 29:1359–1367. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.035
de Pasquale F, Della Penna S, Snyder AZ et al (2012) A cortical core for dynamic integration of functional networks in the resting human brain. Neuron 74:753–764. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.031
Delgado MR, Beer JS, Fellows LK et al (2016) Viewpoints: dialogues on the functional role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci 19:1545–1552. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4438
Denkova E, Nomi JS, Uddin LQ, Jha AP (2019) Dynamic brain network configurations during rest and an attention task with frequent occurrence of mind wandering. Hum Brain Mapp 40:4564–4576. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24721
Denny BT, Kober H, Wager TD, Ochsner KN (2012) A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of self- and other judgments reveals a spatial gradient for mentalizing in medial prefrontal cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 24:1742–1752. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00233
DiNicola LM, Braga RM, Buckner RL (2020) Parallel distributed networks dissociate episodic and social functions within the individual. J Neurophysiol 123:1144–1179. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00529.2019
Dixon ML, Fox KCR, Christoff K (2014) A framework for understanding the relationship between externally and internally directed cognition. Neuropsychologia 62:321–330. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.05.024
Dixon ML, Andrews-Hanna JR, Spreng RN et al (2017) Interactions between the default network and dorsal attention network vary across default subsystems, time, and cognitive states. Neuroimage 147:632–649. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.12.073
Doucet GE, Lee WH, Frangou S (2019) Evaluation of the spatial variability in the major resting-state networks across human brain functional atlases. Hum Brain Mapp. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24722
Dunn BD, Dalgleish T, Lawrence AD (2006) The somatic marker hypothesis: a critical evaluation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 30:239–271. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.07.001
Eichele T, Debener S, Calhoun VD et al (2008) Prediction of human errors by maladaptive changes in event-related brain networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105:6173–6178. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0708965105
Eickhoff S, Laird A, Grefkes C et al (2009) Coordinate-based ALE meta-analysis of neuroimaging data: a random-effects approach based on empirical estimates of spatial uncertainty. Hum Brain Mapp 30:2907–2926. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20718
Eickhoff SB, Bzdok D, Laird AR et al (2012) Activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis revisited. Neuroimage 59:2349–2361. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.017
Eickhoff SB, Thirion B, Varoquaux G, Bzdok D (2015) Connectivity-based parcellation: critique and implications. Hum Brain Mapp 36:4771–4792. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22933
Eickhoff SB, Nichols TE, Laird AR et al (2016) Behavior, sensitivity, and power of activation likelihood estimation characterized by massive empirical simulation. Neuroimage 137:70–85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.072
Eickhoff SB, Laird AR, Fox PM et al (2017) Implementation errors in the GingerALE software: description and recommendations. Hum Brain Mapp 38:7–11. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23342
Ellamil M, Dobson C, Beeman M, Christoff K (2012) Evaluative and generative modes of thought during the creative process. Neuroimage 59:1783–1794. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.008
Elton A, Gao W (2015) Task-positive functional connectivity of the default mode network transcends task domain. J Cogn Neurosci 27:2369–2381. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00859
Engen HG, Kanske P, Singer T (2017) The neural component-process architecture of endogenously generated emotion. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 12:197–211. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw108
Evans M, Krieger-Redwood K, Gonzalez Alam TR et al (2020) Controlled semantic summation correlates with intrinsic connectivity between default mode and control networks. Cortex 129:356–375. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2020.04.032
Farah MJ, Hutchinson JB, Phelps EA, Wagner AD (2014) Functional MRI-based lie detection: Scientific and societal challenges. Nat Rev Neurosci 15:123–131. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3665
Ferenczi EA, Zalocusky KA, Liston C et al (2016) Prefrontal cortical regulation of brainwide circuit dynamics and reward-related behavior. Science. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aac9698
Ferrarini L, Veer IM, Baerends E et al (2009) Hierarchical functional modularity in the resting-state human brain. Hum Brain Mapp 30:2220–2231. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20663
Fingelkurts AA, Fingelkurts AA, Kallio-Tamminen T (2020) Selfhood triumvirate: from phenomenology to brain activity and back again. Conscious Cogn 86:103031. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2020.103031
Fornito A, Harrison BJ, Zalesky A, Simons JS (2012) Competitive and cooperative dynamics of large-scale brain functional networks supporting recollection. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 109:12788–12793. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1204185109
Fossati P, Hevenor SJ, Graham SJ et al (2003) In search of the emotional self: an fMRI study using positive and negative emotional words. Am J Psychiatry 160:1938–1945. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.160.11.1938
Foster BL, Dastjerdi M, Parvizi J (2012) Neural populations in human posteromedial cortex display opposing responses during memory and numerical processing. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 109:15514–15519. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1206580109
Fox PT, Lancaster JL (2002) Mapping context and content: the BrainMap model. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:319–321. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn789
Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL et al (2005a) The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci 102:9673–9678. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0504136102
Fox PT, Laird AR, Fox SP et al (2005b) BrainMap taxonomy of experimental design: description and evaluation. Hum Brain Mapp 25:185–198. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20141
Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Raichle ME (2007) Intrinsic Fluctuations within cortical systems account for intertrial variability in human behavior. Neuron 56:171–184. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2007.08.023
Fox KCR, Spreng RN, Ellamil M et al (2015) The wandering brain: meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of mind-wandering and related spontaneous thought processes. Neuroimage 111:611–621. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.039
Fransson P (2005) Spontaneous low-frequency BOLD signal fluctuations: an fMRI investigation of the resting-state default mode of brain function hypothesis. Hum Brain Mapp 26:15–29. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20113
Fransson P, Thompson WH (2020) Temporal flow of hubs and connectivity in the human brain. Neuroimage 223:117348. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117348
Gerlach KD, Spreng RN, Gilmore AW, Schacter DL (2011) Solving future problems: default network and executive activity associated with goal-directed mental simulations. Neuroimage 55:1816–1824. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.030
Gerlach KD, Spreng RN, Madore KP, Schacter DL (2014) Future planning: default network activity couples with frontoparietal control network and reward-processing regions during process and outcome simulations. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 9:1942–1951. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu001
Giambra LM (1989) Task-unrelated thought frequency as a function of age: a laboratory study. Psychol Aging 4:136–143. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.4.2.136
Gilbert SJ, Frith CD, Burgess PW (2005) Involvement of rostral prefrontal cortex in selection between stimulus-oriented and stimulus-independent thought. Eur J Neurosci 21:1423–1431. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.03981.x
Gilbert SJ, Simons JS, Frith CD, Burgess PW (2006) Performance-related activity in medial rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) during low-demand tasks. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 32:45–58. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.32.1.45
Gilbert SJ, Dumontheil I, Simons JS et al (2007) Comment on “wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought.” Science 317:43b–43b. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1140801
Golland Y, Bentin S, Gelbard H et al (2007) Extrinsic and intrinsic systems in the posterior cortex of the human brain revealed during natural sensory stimulation. Cereb Cortex 17:766–777. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhk030
Goparaju B, Rana KD, Calabro FJ, Vaina LM (2014) A computational study of whole-brain connectivity in resting state and task fMRI. Med Sci Monit 20:1024–1042. https://doi.org/10.12659/MSM.891142
Gordon EM, Laumann TO, Marek S et al (2020) Default-mode network streams for coupling to language and control systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2005238117
Greene JD, Sommerville RB, Nystrom LE et al (2001) An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science 293:2105–2108. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1062872
Greicius MD, Menon V (2004) Default-mode activity during a passive sensory task: uncoupled from deactivation but impacting activation. J Cogn Neurosci 16:1484–1492. https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929042568532
Greicius MD, Krasnow B, Reiss AL, Menon V (2003) Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci 100:253–258. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0135058100
Gusnard DA, Raichle ME (2001) Searching for a baseline: functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 2:685–694. https://doi.org/10.1038/35094500
Gusnard DA, Akbudak E, Shulman GL, Raichle ME (2001) Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain function. PNAS 98:4259–4264. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.071043098
Hampson M, Driesen NR, Skudlarski P et al (2006) Brain connectivity related to working memory performance. J Neurosci 26:13338–13343. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3408-06.2006
Harris CR, Millman KJ, van der Walt SJ et al (2020) Array programming with NumPy. Nature 585:357–362. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-020-2649-2
Harrison BJ, Pujol J, López-Solà M et al (2008) Consistency and functional specialization in the default mode brain network. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105:9781–9786. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0711791105
Hassabis D, Maguire EA (2007) Deconstructing episodic memory with construction. Trends Cogn Sci 11:299–306. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.001
Hassabis D, Kumaran D, Maguire EA (2007) Using imagination to understand the neural basis of episodic memory. J Neurosci 27:14365–14374. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4549-07.2007
Hiser J, Koenigs M (2018) The multifaceted role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in emotion, decision making, social cognition, and psychopathology. Biol Psychiatry 83:638–647. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.10.030
Hunter JD (2007) Matplotlib: a 2D graphics environment. Comput Sci Eng 9:90–95. https://doi.org/10.1109/MCSE.2007.55
Huo T, Li Y, Zhuang K et al (2020) Industriousness moderates the link between default mode network subsystem and creativity. Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2019.11.049
Hutchison RM, Womelsdorf T, Allen EA et al (2013) Dynamic functional connectivity: promise, issues, and interpretations. Neuroimage 80:360–378. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.079
Hyvarinen A (1999) Fast and robust fixed-point algorithms for independent component analysis. IEEE Trans Neural Networks 10:626–634. https://doi.org/10.1109/72.761722
Hyvärinen A, Oja E (1997) A fast fixed-point algorithm for independent component analysis. Neural Comput 9:1483–1492. https://doi.org/10.1162/neco.1997.9.7.1483
Jackson RL (2021) The neural correlates of semantic control revisited. Neuroimage 224:117444. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117444
Jackson RL, Cloutman LL, Lambon Ralph MA (2019) Exploring distinct default mode and semantic networks using a systematic ICA approach. Cortex 113:279–297. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2018.12.019
Jenkinson M, Bannister P, Brady M, Smith S (2002) Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. Neuroimage 17:825–841. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1132
Jung RE, Mead BS, Carrasco J, Flores RA (2013) The structure of creative cognition in the human brain. Front Hum Neurosci. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00330
Kam JWY, Handy TC (2013) The neurocognitive consequences of the wandering mind: a mechanistic account of sensory-motor decoupling. Front Psychol 4:1–13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00725
Kam JWY, Dao E, Farley J et al (2011) Slow fluctuations in attentional control of sensory cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 23:460–470. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2010.21443
Karahanoğlu FI, Van De Ville D (2015) Transient brain activity disentangles fMRI resting-state dynamics in terms of spatially and temporally overlapping networks. Nat Commun 6:7751. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms8751
Kernbach JM, Thomas Yeo BT, Smallwood J et al (2018) Subspecialization within default mode nodes characterized in 10,000 UK Biobank participants. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 115:12295–12300. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1804876115
Kim H (2016) Default network activation during episodic and semantic memory retrieval: a selective meta-analytic comparison. Neuropsychologia 80:35–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.11.006
Kiviniemi V, Vire T, Remes J et al (2011) A sliding time-window ICA reveals spatial variability of the default mode network in time. Brain Connect 1:339–347. https://doi.org/10.1089/brain.2011.0036
Knyazev GG, Savostyanov AN, Bocharov AV et al (2020) Intrinsic connectivity networks in the self- and other-referential processing. Front Hum Neurosci. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.579703
Konishi M, McLaren DG, Engen H, Smallwood J (2015) Shaped by the past: the default mode network supports cognition that is independent of immediate perceptual input. PLoS ONE 10:1–18. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0132209
Koshino H, Minamoto T, Ikeda T et al (2011) Anterior medial prefrontal cortex exhibits activation during task preparation but deactivation during task execution. PLoS ONE 6:54–58. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0022909
Koshino H, Minamoto T, Yaoi K et al (2014) Coactivation of the default mode network regions and working memory network regions during task preparation. Sci Rep 4:34–39. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep05954
Kounios J, Frymiare JL, Bowden EM et al (2006) The prepared mind: neural activity prior to problem presentation predicts subsequent solution by sudden insight. Psychol Sci. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01798.x
Krieger-Redwood K, Jefferies E, Karapanagiotidis T et al (2016) Down but not out in posterior cingulate cortex: deactivation yet functional coupling with prefrontal cortex during demanding semantic cognition. Neuroimage 141:366–377. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.07.060
Kriegeskorte N, Mur M, Bandettini P (2008) Representational similarity analysis—connecting the branches of systems neuroscience. Front Syst Neurosci 2:1–28. https://doi.org/10.3389/neuro.06.004.2008
Krienen FM, Thomas Yeo BT, Buckner RL et al (2014) Reconfigurable task-dependent functional coupling modes cluster around a core functional architecture. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 369:20130526–20130526. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0526
Kruskal JB (1964) Multidimensional scaling by optimizing goodness of fit to a nonmetric hypothesis. Psychometrica 29:1–27
Laird AR, Lancaster JL, Fox PT (2005) BrainMap: the social evolution of a human brain mapping database. Neuroinformatics 3:065–078. https://doi.org/10.1385/NI:3:1:065
Laird AR, Eickhoff SB, Li K et al (2009) Investigating the functional heterogeneity of the DMN using coordinate base metaanalytic modeling. J Neurosci 29:14496–14505. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4004-09.2009
Laird AR, Fox PM, Eickhoff SB et al (2011) Behavioral interpretations of intrinsic connectivity networks. J Cogn Neurosci 23:4022–4037. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00077
Laird AR, Eickhoff SB, Rottschy C et al (2013) Networks of task co-activations. Neuroimage 80:505–514. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.073
Lancaster JL, Cykowski MD, McKay DR et al (2010) Anatomical global spatial normalization. Neuroinformatics 8:171–182. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12021-010-9074-x
Lancaster JL, McKay DR, Cykowski MD et al (2011) Automated analysis of fundamental features of brain structures. Neuroinformatics 9:371–380. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12021-011-9108-z
Lancaster JL, Laird AR, Eickhoff SB et al (2012) Automated regional behavioral analysis for human brain images. Front Neuroinform 6:1–12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fninf.2012.00023
Lanzoni L, Ravasio D, Thompson H et al (2020) The role of default mode network in semantic cue integration. Neuroimage 219:117019. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117019
Leech R, Kamourieh S, Beckmann CF, Sharp DJ (2011) Fractionating the default mode network: Distinct contributions of the ventral and dorsal posterior cingulate cortex to cognitive control. J Neurosci 31:3217–3224. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5626-10.2011
Li C-SR, Yan P, Bergquist KL, Sinha R (2007) Greater activation of the “default” brain regions predicts stop signal errors. Neuroimage 38:640–648. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.021
Lieberman MD, Straccia MA, Meyer ML et al (2019) Social, self, (situational), and affective processes in medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC): causal, multivariate, and reverse inference evidence. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 99:311–328. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.12.021
Lin P, Hasson U, Jovicich J, Robinson S (2011) A neuronal basis for task-negative responses in the human brain. Cereb Cortex 21:821–830. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhq151
Lisofsky N, Kazzer P, Heekeren HR, Prehn K (2014) Investigating socio-cognitive processes in deception: a quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychologia 61:113–122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.06.001
Liu X, Duyn JH (2013) Time-varying functional network information extracted from brief instances of spontaneous brain activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110:4392–4397. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1216856110
Lopez-Persem A, Roumazeilles L, Folloni D et al (2020) Differential functional connectivity underlying asymmetric reward-related activity in human and nonhuman primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 117:28452–28462. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2000759117
Mancuso L, Costa T, Nani A et al (2019) The homotopic connectivity of the functional brain: a meta-analytic approach. Sci Rep 9:3346. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-40188-3
Mantini D, Vanduffel W (2013) Emerging roles of the brain’s default network. Neuroscientist 19:76–87. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858412446202
Mar RA (2011) The neural bases of social cognition and story comprehension. Annu Rev Psychol 62:103–134. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145406
Margulies DS, Ghosh SS, Goulas A et al (2016) Situating the default-mode network along a principal gradient of macroscale cortical organization. Proc Natl Acad Sci 113:12574–12579. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1608282113
Marron TR, Lerner Y, Berant E et al (2018) Chain free association, creativity, and the default mode network. Neuropsychologia. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.03.018
Mars RB, Neubert FX, Noonan MAP et al (2012) On the relationship between the “default mode network” and the “social brain.” Front Hum Neurosci. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00189
Martins D, Rademacher L, Gabay AS et al (2021) Mapping social reward and punishment processing in the human brain: a voxel-based meta-analysis of neuroimaging findings using the social incentive delay task. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.12.034
Mason MF, Norton MI, Van HJD et al (2007) Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science 315:393–395. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1131295
Mayseless N, Eran A, Shamay-Tsoory SG (2015) Generating original ideas: the neural underpinning of originality. Neuroimage 116:232–239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.030
Mazoyer B, Zago L, Mellet E et al (2001) Cortical networks for working memory and executive functions sustain the conscious resting state in man. Brain Res Bull 54:287–298. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0361-9230(00)00437-8
Mennes M, Kelly C, Colcombe S et al (2013) The extrinsic and intrinsic functional architectures of the human brain are not equivalent. Cereb Cortex 23:223–229. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs010
Menon V, Uddin LQ (2010) Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct Funct 214:655–667. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0
Mesulam M-M (1998) From sensation to perception. Brain 121:1013–1052. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/121.6.1013
Milton CK, Dhanaraj V, Young IM et al (2021) Parcellation-based anatomic model of the semantic network. Brain Behav 11:1–13. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.2065
Mohan A, Roberto AJ, Mohan A et al (2016) The significance of the Default Mode Network (DMN) in neurological and neuropsychiatric disorders: a review. Yale J Biol Med 89:49–57
Molnar-Szakacs I, Uddin LQ (2013) Self-processing and the default mode network: interactions with the mirror neuron system. Front Hum Neurosci 7:1–11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00571
Moraschi M, Mascali D, Tommasin S et al (2020) Brain network modularity during a sustained working-memory task. Front Physiol 11:1–10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2020.00422
Müller VI, Cieslik EC, Laird AR et al (2018) Ten simple rules for neuroimaging meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 84:151–161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.11.012
Murphy C, Jefferies E, Rueschemeyer SA et al (2018) Distant from input: evidence of regions within the default mode network supporting perceptually-decoupled and conceptually-guided cognition. Neuroimage 171:393–401. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.017
Mwilambwe-Tshilobo L, Spreng RN (2021) Social exclusion reliably engages the default network: a meta-analysis of Cyberball. Neuroimage 227:117666. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117666
Najafi M, McMenamin BW, Simon JZ, Pessoa L (2016) Overlapping communities reveal rich structure in large-scale brain networks during rest and task conditions. Neuroimage 135:92–106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.054
Newton AT, Morgan VL, Rogers BP, Gore JC (2011) Modulation of steady state functional connectivity in the default mode and working memory networks by cognitive load. Hum Brain Mapp 32:1649–1659. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21138
Ngo GH, Eickhoff SB, Nguyen M et al (2019) Beyond consensus: embracing heterogeneity in curated neuroimaging meta-analysis. Neuroimage 200:142–158. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.06.037
Noonan KA, Jefferies E, Visser M, Lambon Ralph MA (2013) Going beyond inferior prefrontal involvement in semantic control: evidence for the additional contribution of dorsal angular gyrus and posterior middle temporal cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 25:1824–1850. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00442
Northoff G, Bermpohl F (2004) Cortical midline structures and the self. Trends Cogn Sci 8:102–107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.01.004
Northoff G, Heinzel A, de Greck M et al (2006) Self-referential processing in our brain—a meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. Neuroimage 31:440–457. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.002
Ochsner KN, Knierim K, Ludlow DH et al (2004) Reflecting upon feelings: an fMRI study of neural systems supporting the attribution of emotion to self and other. J Cogn Neurosci 16:1746–1772. https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929042947829
Ochsner KN, Beer JS, Robertson ER et al (2005) The neural correlates of direct and reflected self-knowledge. Neuroimage 28:797–814. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.069
Palla G, Derényi I, Farkas I, Vicsek T (2005) Uncovering the overlapping community structure of complex networks in nature and society. Nature 435:814–818. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature03607
Paquola C, Garber M, Frässle S et al (2021) The unique cytoarchitecture and wiring of the human default mode network. bioRxiv. https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.11.22.469533
Pedregosa F, Varoquaux G, Gramfort A et al (2011) Scikit-learn: machine learning in Python. J Mach Learn Res 12:2825–2830
Pessoa L (2014) Understanding brain networks and brain organization. Phys Life Rev 11:400–435. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.plrev.2014.03.005
Piccoli T, Valente G, Linden DEJ et al (2015) The default mode network and the working memory network are not anti-correlated during all phases of a working memory task. PLoS ONE 10:1–16. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0123354
Prado J, Weissman DH (2011) Heightened interactions between a key default-mode region and a key task-positive region are linked to suboptimal current performance but to enhanced future performance. Neuroimage 56:2276–2282. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.03.048
Preti MG, Bolton TA, Van De Ville D (2017) The dynamic functional connectome: state-of-the-art and perspectives. Neuroimage 160:41–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.12.061
Pujol J, Reixach J, Harrison BJ et al (2008) Posterior cingulate activation during moral dilemma in adolescents. Hum Brain Mapp 29:910–921. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20436
Raichle ME (2009) a paradigm shift in functional brain imaging. J Neurosci 29:12729–12734. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4366-09.2009
Raichle ME, Snyder AZ (2007) A default mode of brain function: a brief history of an evolving idea. Neuroimage 37:1083–1090. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.041
Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ et al (2001) A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci 98:676–682. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.98.2.676
Ray KL, McKay DR, Fox PM et al (2013) ICA model order selection of task co-activation networks. Front Neurosci 7:1–12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2013.00237
Rilling JK, Sanfey AG, Aronson JA et al (2004) The neural correlates of theory of mind within interpersonal interactions. Neuroimage 22:1694–1703. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.04.015
Rosenthal R (1979) The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results. Psychol Bull 86:638–641. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.638
Ruby P, Decety J (2004) How would you feel versus how do you think she would feel? A neuroimaging study of perspective-taking with social emotions. J Cogn Neurosci 16:988–999. https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929041502661
Rugg MD, Vilberg KL (2013) Brain networks underlying episodic memory retrieval. Curr Opin Neurobiol 23:255–260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2012.11.005
Samartsidis P, Montagna S, Laird AR et al (2020) Estimating the prevalence of missing experiments in a neuroimaging meta-analysis. Res Synth Methods 11:866–883. https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.1448
Satpute AB, Lindquist KA (2019) The default mode network’s role in discrete emotion. Trends Cogn Sci 23:851–864. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2019.07.003
Saxe R, Kanwisher N (2003) People thinking about thinking people: the role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind.” Neuroimage 19:1835–1842. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00230-1
Saxe R, Powell LJ (2006) It’s the thought that counts. Psychol Sci 17:692–699. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01768.x
Schacter DL, Addis DR, Buckner RL (2007) Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 8:657–661. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2213
Schacter DL, Addis DR, Buckner RL (2008) Episodic simulation of future events: concepts, data, and applications. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1124:39–60. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1440.001
Schilbach L, Bzdok D, Timmermans B et al (2012) Introspective minds: using ALE meta-analyses to study commonalities in the neural correlates of emotional processing, social & unconstrained cognition. PLoS ONE. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0030920
Schneider B, Koenigs M (2017) Human lesion studies of ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia 107:84–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.09.035
Schultz W (2015) Neuronal reward and decision signals: from theories to data. Physiol Rev 95:853–951. https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00023.2014
Seli P, Risko EF, Smilek D, Schacter DL (2016) Mind-wandering with and without intention. Trends Cogn Sci 20:605–617. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.05.010
Sha Z, Xia M, Lin Q et al (2018) Meta-connectomic analysis reveals commonly disrupted functional architectures in network modules and connectors across brain disorders. Cereb Cortex 28:4179–4194. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhx273
Sha Z, Wager TD, Mechelli A, He Y (2019) Common dysfunction of large-scale neurocognitive networks across psychiatric disorders. Biol Psychiatry 85:379–388. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.11.011
Shirer WR, Ryali S, Rykhlevskaia E et al (2012) Decoding subject-driven cognitive states with whole-brain connectivity patterns. Cereb Cortex 22:158–165. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr099
Shulman GL, Fiez JA, Corbetta M et al (1997) Common blood flow changes across visual tasks: II. Decreases in cerebral cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 9:648–663. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.648
Smallwood J, McSpadden M, Schooler JW (2008) When attention matters: the curious incident of the wandering mind. Mem Cogn 36:1144–1150. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.36.6.1144
Smallwood J, Bernhardt BC, Leech R et al (2021) The default mode network in cognition: a topographical perspective. Nat Rev Neurosci. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-021-00474-4
Smith SM, Fox PT, Miller KL et al (2009) Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc Natl Acad Sci 106:13040–13045. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0905267106
Smith V, Duncan J, Mitchell DJ (2021) Roles of the default mode and multiple-demand networks in naturalistic versus symbolic decisions. J Neurosci. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.1888-20.2020
Sonuga-Barke EJS, Castellanos FX (2007) Spontaneous attentional fluctuations in impaired states and pathological conditions: a neurobiological hypothesis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 31:977–986. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.02.005
Sormaz M, Murphy C, Wang HT et al (2018) Default mode network can support the level of detail in experience during active task states. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 115:9318–9323. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1721259115
Spiers HJ, Maguire EA (2006) Spontaneous mentalizing during an interactive real world task: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 44:1674–1682. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.03.028
Sporns O, Tononi G, Kötter R (2005) The human connectome: a structural description of the human brain. PLoS Comput Biol 1:0245–0251. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.0010042
Spreng RN (2012) The Fallacy of a “task-negative” network. Front Psychol 3:1–5. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00145
Spreng RN, Andrews-Hanna JR (2015) The default network and social cognition. Brain Mapp an Encycl Ref 3:165–169. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-397025-1.00173-1
Spreng RN, Grady CL (2010) Patterns of brain activity supporting autobiographical memory, prospection, and theory of mind, and their relationship to the default mode network. J Cogn Neurosci 22:1112–1123. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21282
Spreng RN, Mar RA, Kim ASN (2009) The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: a quantitative meta-analysis. J Cogn Neurosci 21:489–510. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.21029
Spreng RN, Stevens WD, Chamberlain JP et al (2010) Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network, supports goal-directed cognition. Neuroimage 53:303–317. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.016
Spreng RN, Sepulcre J, Turner GR et al (2013) Intrinsic architecture underlying the relations among the default, dorsal attention, and frontoparietal control networks of the human brain. J Cogn Neurosci 25:74–86. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00281
Spreng RN, DuPre E, Selarka D et al (2014) Goal-congruent default network activity facilitates cognitive control. J Neurosci 34:14108–14114. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2815-14.2014
Spreng RN, Gerlach KD, Turner GR, Schacter DL (2015) Autobiographical planning and the brain: activation and its modulation by qualitative features. J Cogn Neurosci 27:2147–2157. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00846
Sridharan D, Levitin DJ, Menon V (2008) A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci 105:12569–12574. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0800005105
Svoboda E, McKinnon MC, Levine B (2006) The functional neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 44:2189–2208. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.05.023
Tavor I, Parker Jones O, Mars RB et al (2016) Task-free MRI predicts individual differences in brain activity during task performance. Science 352:216–220. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aad8127
Tomasi D, Volkow ND (2011) Functional connectivity hubs in the human brain. Neuroimage 57:908–917. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.024
Toro R, Fox PT, Paus T (2008) Functional coactivation map of the human brain. Cereb Cortex 18:2553–2559. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn014
Toro-Serey C, Tobyne SM, McGuire JT (2020) Spectral partitioning identifies individual heterogeneity in the functional network topography of ventral and anterior medial prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage 205:116305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116305
Turkeltaub PE, Eden GF, Jones KM, Zeffiro TA (2002) Meta-analysis of the functional neuroanatomy of single-word reading: method and validation. Neuroimage 16:765–780. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1131
Turkeltaub PE, Eickhoff SB, Laird AR et al (2012) Minimizing within-experiment and within-group effects in activation likelihood estimation meta-analyses. Hum Brain Mapp 33:1–13. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21186
Uddin LQ, Iacoboni M, Lange C, Keenan JP (2007) The self and social cognition: the role of cortical midline structures and mirror neurons. Trends Cogn Sci 11:153–157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.01.001
Uddin LQ, Clare Kelly AM, Biswal BB et al (2009) Functional connectivity of default mode network components: correlation, anticorrelation, and causality. Hum Brain Mapp 30:625–637. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20531
Uddin LQ, Yeo BTT, Spreng RN (2019) Towards a universal taxonomy of macro-scale functional human brain networks. Brain Topogr 32:926–942. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10548-019-00744-6
van den Heuvel MP, Sporns O (2011) Rich-club organization of the human connectome. J Neurosci 31:15775–15786. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3539-11.2011
Van Dijk KRA, Hedden T, Venkataraman A et al (2010) Intrinsic functional connectivity as a tool for human connectomics: theory, properties, and optimization. J Neurophysiol 103:297–321. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00783.2009
Vatansever D, Menon DK, Manktelow AE et al (2015a) Default mode network connectivity during task execution. Neuroimage 122:96–104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.053
Vatansever D, Menon DK, Manktelow AE et al (2015b) Default mode dynamics for global functional integration. J Neurosci 35:15254–15262. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2135-15.2015
Vatansever D, Menon DK, Stamatakis EA (2017) Default mode contributions to automated information processing. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 114:12821–12826. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1710521114
Verdejo-García A, Bechara A (2009) A somatic marker theory of addiction. Neuropharmacology 56:48–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.07.035
Vincent JL, Patel GH, Fox MD et al (2007) Intrinsic functional architecture in the anaesthetized monkey brain. Nature 447:83–86. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature05758
Wang S, Tepfer LJ, Taren AA, Smith DV (2020) Functional parcellation of the default mode network: a large-scale meta-analysis. Sci Rep. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-72317-8
Weissman DH, Roberts KC, Visscher KM, Woldorff MG (2006) The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention. Nat Neurosci 9:971–978. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1727
Wen T, Mitchell DJ, Duncan J (2020) The functional convergence and heterogeneity of social, episodic, and self-referential thought in the default mode network. Cereb Cortex. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhaa166
Xue G, Lu Z, Levin IP et al (2009) Functional dissociations of risk and reward processing in the medial prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 19:1019–1027. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn147
Yang XF, Bossmann J, Schiffhauer B et al (2013) Intrinsic default mode network connectivity predicts spontaneous verbal descriptions of autobiographical memories during social processing. Front Psychol 3:1–10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00592
Yeo BTT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J et al (2011) The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol 106:1125–1165. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00338.2011
Yeo BTT, Krienen FM, Chee MWL, Buckner RL (2014) Estimates of segregation and overlap of functional connectivity networks in the human cerebral cortex. Neuroimage 88:212–227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.10.046
Yeshurun Y, Nguyen M, Hasson U, Nguyen M (2021) The default mode network: where the idiosyncratic self meets the shared social world. Nat Rev Neurosci 22:181–192. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-020-00420-w
Funding
This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
LM.: conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis, visualization, and writing—original draft; SC-C.: investigation, formal analysis, visualization, and writing—review and editing; DL.: formal analysis, data curation, visualization, and writing—review and editing; JM.: formal analysis and writing—review and editing; GB.: writing—review and editing; FC.: project administration; TC.: methodology, supervision, and project administration.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of interest
All the authors declare of having no conflict of interest.
Ethics approval
This is a meta-analysis. No ethical approval is needed.
Consent to participate
Not applicable.
Consent for publication
All the authors approve the publication of this manuscript.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Mancuso, L., Cavuoti-Cabanillas, S., Liloia, D. et al. Tasks activating the default mode network map multiple functional systems. Brain Struct Funct 227, 1711–1734 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-022-02467-0
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-022-02467-0
Keywords
- Activation likelihood estimation
- Task-induced deactivations
- DMN
- Semantics
- Insula