Skip to main content
Log in

Shared mechanisms underlying the location-, word- and arrow-based Simon effects

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A left or right keypress response to a relevant stimulus attribute (e.g., color) is faster when irrelevant left or right stimulus-location information corresponds with the correct response than when it does not. This phenomenon, known as the Simon effect, is obtained not only for physical locations, but also location words “left” and “right” and left- or right-pointing arrows. However, these location-, word-, and arrow-based Simon effects show different patterns in the reaction-time (RT) distributions, as evident in delta plots. In the present study, we employed procedures, analysis of survival curves and divergence point analysis, which have not previously been applied to the Simon effect, to investigate differences in time course of these various Simon effects in more detail. Also, we examined whether the diffusion model for conflict tasks (DMC), which assumes that automatic activation of task-irrelevant information occurs in a pulse-like function, can capture not only features of the RT distributions for the location-based Simon effect, to which it has been fit previously, but also features of the word- and arrow-based Simon effects, to which it has not. Results showed different survival curves and earliest, maximum, and latest divergence points for the three Simon effects, but DMC was able to capture the basic features of the RT distributions reflected by delta plot and survival curves for all effects. The results imply that the location-, word-, and arrow-based Simon effects have shared mechanisms, although they have different RT distributions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ando, E., Matsuki, K., Sheridan, H., & Jared, D. (2015). The locus of Katakana-English masked phonological priming effects. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition,18, 101–117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ansorge, U., & Wühr, P. (2004). A response-discrimination account of the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,30, 365–377.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baroni, G., Pellicano, A., Lugli, L., Nicoletti, R., & Proctor, R. W. (2012). Influence of temporal overlap on time course of the Simon effect. Experimental Psychology,59, 88–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burle, B., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2005). Dynamics of facilitation and interference in cue-priming and Simon tasks. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17, 619–641.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Jong, R., Liang, C.-C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus–response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,20, 731–750.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Efron, B., & Tibshirani, R. J. (1994). An introduction to the bootstrap. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman & Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellinghaus, R., Karlbauer, M., Bausenhart, K. M., & Ulrich, R. (2018). On the time-course of automatic response activation in the Simon task. Psychological Research,82, 734–743.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geisser, S., & Greenhouse, S. W. (1958). An extension of box’s results on the use of the F distribution in multivariate analysis. The Annals of Mathematical Statistics,29, 885–891.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoedemaker, R. S., & Gordon, P. C. (2017). The onset and time course of semantic priming during rapid recognition of visual words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,43, 881–902.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B. (1993). The relationship between stimulus processing and response selection in the Simon task: Evidence for a temporal overlap. Psychological Research,55, 280–290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B. (1994). Spontaneous decay of response-code activation. Psychological Research,56, 261–268.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Khalid, S., & Ansorge, U. (2013). The simon effect of spatial words in eye movements: Comparison of vertical and horizontal effects and of eye and finger responses. Vision Research, 86, 6–14.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility–a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. (1980). Attention and automaticity in Stroop and priming tasks: Theory and data. Cognitive Psychology,12, 523–553.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lu, C.-H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review,2, 174–207.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lu, C.-H., & Proctor, R. W. (2001). Influence of irrelevant information on human performance: Effects of S–R association strength and relative timing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,54, 95–136.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luo, C., & Proctor, R. W. (2017). How different location modes influence responses in a Simon-like task. Psychological Research,81, 1125–1134.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luo, C., & Proctor, R. W. (2018a). How different direct association routes influence the indirect route in the same Simon-like task. Psychological Research. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1024-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luo, C., & Proctor, R. W. (2018b). The location-, word-, and arrow-based Simon effects: An ex-Gaussian analysis. Memory and Cognition,46, 497–506.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miles, J. D., & Proctor, R. W. (2012). Correlations between spatial compatibility effects: Are arrows more like locations or words? Psychological Research,76, 777–791.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Modern Chinese Frequency Dictionary. (1985). Beijing, People’s Republic of China: Beijing Language Institute Press.

  • Nelder, B. J. A., & Mead, R. (1964). A simplex method for function minimization. The Computer Journal, 7, 308–313.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pellicano, A., Lugli, L., Baroni, G., & Nicoletti, R. (2009). The Simon effect with conventional signals: A time-course analysis. Experimental Psychology,56, 219–227.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pratte, M. S., Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., & Feng, C. (2010). Exploring the differences in distributional properties between Stroop and Simon effects using delta plots. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics,72, 2013–2025.

    Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, R. W., Marble, J. G., & Vu, K.-P. L. (2000). Mixing incompatibly mapped location-relevant trials with location-irrelevant trials: Effects of stimulus mode on the reverse Simon effect. Psychological Research,64, 11–24.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, R. W., Miles, J. D., & Baroni, G. (2011). Reaction time distribution analysis of spatial correspondence effects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review,18, 242–266.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, R. W., Yamaguchi, M., Zhang, Y., & Vu, K.-P. L. (2009). Influence of visual stimulus mode on transfer of acquired spatial associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,35, 434–445.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R. (1978). A theory of memory retrieval. Psychological Review,85, 59–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R., & Smith, P. L. (2004). A comparison of sequential sampling models for two-choice reaction time. Psychological Review,111, 333–367.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R., Smith, P. L., Brown, S. D., & McKoon, G. (2016). Diffusion decision model: Current issues and history. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,20, 260–281.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Reingold, E. M., Reichle, E. D., Glaholt, M. G., & Sheridan, H. (2012). Direct lexical control of eye movements in reading: Evidence from a survival analysis of fixation durations. Cognitive Psychology,65, 177–206.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Reingold, E. M., & Sheridan, H. (2018). On using distributional analysis techniques for determining the onset of the influence of experimental variables. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 260–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reingold, E. M., & Sheridan, H. (2014). Estimating the divergence point: A novel distributional analysis procedure for determining the onset of the influence of experimental variables. Frontiers in Psychology,5, 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reingold, E. M., & Sheridan, H. (2018). On using distributional analysis techniques for determining the onset of the influence of experimental variables. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,71, 260–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scerrati, E., Lugli, L., Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (2017). Comparing Stroop-like and Simon effects on perceptual features. Scientific Reports,7, 17815.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, W., & Miller, J. (2012). Response time models of delta plots with negative-going slopes. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review,19, 555–574.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Servant, M., White, C., Montagnini, A., & Burle, B. (2016). Linking theoretical decision-making mechanisms in the Simon task with electrophysiological data: A model-based neuroscience study in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,28, 1501–1521.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, J. R. (1990). The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus–response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 31–86). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ulrich, R., Schröter, H., Leuthold, H., & Birngruber, T. (2015). Automatic and controlled stimulus processing in conflict tasks: Superimposed diffusion processes and delta functions. Cognitive Psychology,78, 148–174.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ulrich, R., Schröter, H., Leuthold, H., & Birngruber, T. (2016). Corrigendum to automatic and controlled stimulus processing in conflict tasks: Superimposed diffusion processes and delta functions [Cogn. Psychol. 78 (2015) 148–174]. Cognitive Psychology, 91, 150.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Umiltà, C., & Nicoletti, R. (1990). Spatial stimulus–response compatibility. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus–response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 89–143). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wascher, E., Schatz, U., Kuder, T., & Verleger, R. (2001). Validity and boundary conditions of automatic response activation in the Simon task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,27, 731–751.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiegand, K., & Wascher, E. (2005). Dynamic aspects of stimulus–response correspondence: Evidence for two mechanisms involved in the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,31, 453–464.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiegand, K., & Wascher, E. (2007). The Simon effect for vertical S–R relations: Changing the mechanism by randomly varying the S–R mapping rule? Psychological Research,71, 219–233.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yamaguchi, M., & Proctor, R. W. (2012). Multidimensional vector model of stimulus–response compatibility. Psychological Review,119, 272–303.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zorzi, M., & Umiltà, C. (1995). A computational model of the Simon effect. Psychological Research,58, 193–205.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Dr. Rolf Ulrich and Ruben Ellinghaus for providing us the script of DMC in Experiment 1 in Ellinghaus et al. (2018) and making some comments on the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by grants from National Science Foundation of China (31470984).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Chunming Luo.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Informed consent

Written consent was obtained from all participants prior to participation.

Ethical standards

The protocol was approved by the institutional review board (IRB) at the Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Luo, C., Proctor, R.W. Shared mechanisms underlying the location-, word- and arrow-based Simon effects. Psychological Research 84, 1655–1667 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01175-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01175-5

Navigation