Skip to main content
Log in

Delta plots with negative-going slopes as a potential marker of decreasing response activation in masked semantic priming

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Delta plots with negative-going slopes (nDPs) reflect the phenomenon that an RT difference between two conditions is greater for relatively fast than for relatively slow responses. This unusual distributional pattern has predominantly been observed in the spatial Simon task, where it has been interpreted as reflecting the selective inhibition of an automatically activated response. The literature suggesting that a similar fading mechanism influences RTs in masked identity priming inspired us to check an analogous semantic priming paradigm for nDPs. Consistent with the findings in other paradigms, two masked semantic priming experiments revealed stronger priming effects for relatively fast than for relatively slow responses, thus reflecting an nDP. These findings are compatible with the ideas that the activation produced by masked semantic primes decreases over the course of a trial, such as that of irrelevant spatial information and of masked identity primes, and that nDPs are a general signature of within-trial decreases in response activation across different tasks and paradigms.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. As an alternative DP analysis, we carried out a within-subjects ANOVA on RT with RT Bin and congruency as factors. The decrease of the PCE across bins was reflected in the significant interaction, F(4,60) = 4.75, p = .002 However, in this case the choice of bin number potentially influences the outcome strongly, since the sum of squares for the overall effect is divided by the number of bins (minus one), so the mean square for the interaction tends to decrease as the number of bins increases. With the present data we found that the interaction was also significant when the ANOVA was run with 3 bins, 10 bins, or 20 bins, F(2,30) = 5.14, p = .012, F(9,135) = 3.35, p = .002, F(19,285) = 2.97, p < .001, respectively. Furthermore, the reported t test on the slopes was significant not only with 5 bins but also with 3 bins, 10 bins or 20 bins, t(15) = 2.46, p = .026, t(15) = 2.3, p = .037, t(15) = 2.20, p = .043, respectively.

  2. For DP analysis, we also carried out a three-factor repeated measures ANOVA with SOA, congruency, and RT bin as factors and RT as dependent variable. The main effect of congruency reached significance, F(1,44) = 62.90, p < .001. As one expects, the main effect of bin was highly significant, too, F(4,176) = 664.20, p < .001. Most importantly, the interaction between congruency and bin was highly significant, F(4,176) = 11.17, p < .001, reflecting the expected decrease of the congruency effect across RT bins (averaged across the two SOAs) and thereby replicating the nDP in the data of Ellinghaus (2015). The decrease was significantly steeper for the long SOA compared to the short SOA, as was reflected in the three-way interaction of congruency, bin and SOA, F(4, 176) = 4.63, p = .022.

References

  • Atas, A., & Cleeremans, A. (2015). The temporal dynamic of automatic inhibition of irrelevant actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41, 289–305.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Cortese, M. J., & Watson, J. M. (2008). Beyond mean response latency: Response time distributional analyses of semantic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 495–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 433–436.

  • Burle, B., Possamaï, C. A., Vidal, F., Bonnet, M., & Hasbroucq, T. (2002). Executive control in the Simon effect: an electromyographic and distributional analysis. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 66, 324–336. doi:10.1007/s00426-002-0105-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Jong, R., Liang, C. C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 731–750.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Wit, B., & Kinoshita, S. (2014a). Relatedness proportion effects in semantic categorization: reconsidering the automatic spreading activation process. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 40(6), 1733–1744.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Wit, B., & Kinoshita, S. (2014b). An RT distribution analysis of relatedness proportion effects in lexical decision and semantic categorization reveals different mechanisms. Memory & Cognition, 99–110.

  • De Wit, B., & Kinoshita, S. (2015). The masked semantic priming effect is task dependent: Reconsidering the automatic spreading activation process. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(4), 1062–1075.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Debey, E., Ridderinkhof, R. K., De Houwer, J., De Schryver, M., & Verschuere, B. (2015). Suppressing the truth as a mechanism of deception: Delta plots reveal the role of response inhibition in lying. Consciousness and Cognition, 37, 148–159.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Donders, F. C. (1969/1868). On the speed of mental processes. Acta Psychologica, 30, 412–431.

  • Eimer, M. (1999). Facilitatory and inhibitory effects of masked prime stimuli on motor activation and behavioural performance. Acta Psychologica, 101, 293–313.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (1998). Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1737–1747.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ellinghaus, R. (2015). The conditional automaticity of unconscious vision: Attentional sensitization of masked prime processing (Unpublished Master’s Thesis). Georg-August-Universität, Göttingen, Germany.

  • Gomez, P., Perea, M., & Ratcliff, R. (2013). A diffusion model account of masked versus unmasked priming: are they qualitatively different? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39(6), 1731–1740.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., & Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 480–506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heathcote, A., Popiel, S. J., & Mewhort, D. J. (1991). Analysis of response time distributions: an example using the Stroop task. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 340–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutchison, K. A., Heap, S. J., Neely, J. H., & Thomas, M. A. (2014). Attentional control and asymmetric associative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(3), 844–856.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kleiner, M., Brainard, D.H., & Pelli, D.G. (2007). What's new in psychtoolbox-3? Perception, 36(14), 1. (36 ECVP Abstract Supplement).

  • Kouider, S., & Dehaene, S. (2007). Levels of processing during non-conscious perception: a critical review of visual masking. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 362, 857–875.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Luce, R. D. (1986). Response times: Their role in inferring elementary mental organization. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. O., & Roüast, N. (2016). Dissociations of spatial congruence effects across response measures: an examination of delta plots. Psychological Research, 80(5), 805–820. doi:10.1007/s00426-015-0694-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 106(3), 226–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pelli, D.G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 437–442.

  • Popper, K. R. (1959). The logic of scientific discovery. London: Hutchinson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pratte, M. S., Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., & Feng, C. (2010). Exploring the differences in distributional properties between Stroop and Simon effects using delta plots. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 2013–2025.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quinn, W. M., & Kinoshita, S. (2008). Congruence effect in semantic categorization with masked primes with narrow and broad categories. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(2), 286–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ridderinkhof, K. R., Scheres, A., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (2005). Delta plots in the study of individual differences: new tools reveal response inhibition deficits in AD/HD that are eliminated by methylphenidate treatment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 197–215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ridderinkhof, K. R., van den Wildenberg, W. P., Wijnen, J., & Burle, B. (2004). Response inhibition in conflict tasks is revealed in delta plots. In M. Posner (Ed.), Cognitive neuroscience of attention (pp. 369–377). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlaghecken, F., Birak, K. S., & Maylor, E. A. (2011). Age-related deficits in low-level inhibitory motor control. Psychology and Aging, 26, 905–918.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schlaghecken, F., & Eimer, M. (1997). The influence of subliminally presented primes on response preparation. Sprache & Kognition, 16, 166–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, W., & Miller, J. (2012). Response time models of delta plots with negative-going slopes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 555–574.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: extensions of Donders’ method. Acta Psychologica, 30, 276–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, M. A., Neely, J. H., & O’Connor, P. (2012). When word identification gets tough, retrospective semantic processing comes to the rescue. Journal of Memory and Language, 66(4), 623–643.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Townsend, J. T., & Ashby, F. G. (1983). Stochastic modeling of elementary psychological processes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ulrich, R., Schröter, H., Leuthold, H., & Birngruber, T. (2015). Automatic and controlled stimulus processing in conflict tasks: Superimposed diffusion processes and delta functions. Cognitive Psychology, 78, 148–174.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vallesi, A., Mapelli, D., Schiff, S., Amodio, P., & Umilta, C. (2005). Horizontal and vertical Simon effect: different underlying mechanisms? Cognition, 96, B33–B43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wernicke, M. (2014). Approaching consciousness with masked priming (Doctoral dissertation). Georg-August-Universität, Göttingen, Germany.

  • Xiong, A., & Proctor, R. W. (2016). Decreasing auditory Simon effects across reaction time distributions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 23–38.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, J., & Kornblum, S. (1997). Distributional analysis and De Jong, Liang, and Lauber’s (1994) dual-process model of the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1543–1551.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by a research award from the Alexander von Humboldt foundation to Jeff Miller, and we thank Ouwen Liu for assistance in testing experimental participants. We thank Friederike Schlaghecken and Leendert van Maanen for helpful comments on earlier versions of this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Ruben Ellinghaus or Jeff Miller.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Human participants

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all participants included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ellinghaus, R., Miller, J. Delta plots with negative-going slopes as a potential marker of decreasing response activation in masked semantic priming. Psychological Research 82, 590–599 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0844-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0844-z

Navigation