Skip to main content
Log in

Cue-target contingencies modulate voluntary orienting of spatial attention: dissociable effects for speed and accuracy

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Voluntary orienting of spatial attention is typically investigated by visually presented directional cues, which are called predictive when they indicate where the target is more likely to appear. In this study, we investigated the nature of the potential link between cue predictivity (the proportion of valid trials) and the strength of the resulting covert orienting of attention. Participants judged the orientation of a unilateral Gabor grating preceded by a centrally presented, non-directional, color cue, arbitrarily prompting a leftwards or rightwards shift of attention. Unknown to them, cue predictivity was manipulated across blocks, whereby the cue was only predictive for either the first or the second half of the experiment. Our results show that the cueing effects were strongly influenced by the change in predictivity. This influence differently emerged in response speed and accuracy. The speed difference between valid and invalid trials was significantly larger when cues were predictive, and the amplitude of this effect was modulated at the single trial level by the recent trial history. Complementary to these findings, accuracy revealed a robust effect of block history and also a different time-course compared with speed, as if it mainly mirrored voluntary processes. These findings, obtained with a new manipulation and using arbitrary non-directional cueing, demonstrate that cue-target contingencies strongly modulate the way attention is deployed in space.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Of note, the two levels of cue predictivity/proportion valid used here are only indicative because “real” cue predictivity/proportion valid changes on trial-by-trial basis.

References

  • Abrahamse, E., Braem, S., Notebaert, W., & Verguts, T. (2016). Grounding cognitive control in associative learning. Psychological Bulletin, 142, 693–728.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alamia, A., Orban de Xivry, J. J., San Anton, E., Olivier, E., Cleeremans, A., & Zenon, A. (2016). Unconscious associative learning with conscious cues. Neuroscience of Consciousness, 1–10. doi:10.1093/nc/niw016

  • Awh, E., Belopolsky, A. V., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 437–443.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bartolomeo, P., Decaix, C., & Siéroff, E. (2007). The phenomenology of endogenous orienting. Consciousness and Cognition, 16, 144–161.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2014). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version 1.1-7. Retrieved from http://cran.r-project.org/package=lme4.

  • Bonato, M. (2012). Neglect and extinction depend greatly on task demands: a review. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 195.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bonato, M. (2015). Unveiling residual, spontaneous recovery from subtle hemispatial neglect three years after stroke. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 413.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bonato, M., Priftis, K., Marenzi, R., & Zorzi, M. (2009). Normal and impaired reflexive orienting of attention after central nonpredictive cues. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 745–759.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Box, G. E. P., & Cox, D. R. (1964a). An analysis of transformations. Journal of the Royal Statisistical Society, Series B, 26, 211–246.

    Google Scholar 

  • Box, G. E. P., & Cox, R. (1964b). An Analysis of transformations. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological), 26(2), 211–252.

    Google Scholar 

  • Braem, S., Hickey, C., Duthoo, W., & Notebaert, W. (2014). Reward determines the context-sensitivity of cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 1769–1778. doi:10.1037/a0037554.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carrasco, M. (2011). Visual attention: The past 25 years. Vision Research, 51, 1484–1525.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chica, A. B., & Bartolomeo, P. (2010). Unconscious strategies? Commentary on Risko and Stolz (2010): The proportion valid effect in covert orienting: Strategic control or implicit learning? Consciousness and Cognition, 91, 443–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chica, A. B., Martín-Arévalo, E., Botta, F., & Lupiáñez, J. (2014). The Spatial Orienting paradigm: how to design and interpret spatial attention experiments. Neuroscience & Biobehavioural Reviews, 40, 35–51. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2011). Spatial neglect and attention networks. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 34, 569–599.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cronbach, L. J., & Furby, L. (1970). How we should measure “change”: Or should we? Psychological Bulletin, 74, 68–80.

  • Cutini, S., Scatturin, P., Menon, E., Bisiacchi, P. S., Gamberini, L., Zorzi, M., & Dell’Acqua, R. (2008). Selective activation of the superior frontal gyrus in task-switching: an event-related fNIRS study. NeuroImage, 42, 945–955.

  • Dobbins, I. G. (2004). Cortical activity reductions during repetition priming can result from rapid response learning. Nature, 428, 316–319.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dombert, P. L., Fink, G. R., & Vossel, S. (2016). The impact of probabilistic feature cueing depends on the level of cue abstraction. Experimental Brain Research, 234, 685–694.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Doricchi, F., Macci, E., Silvetti, M., & Macaluso, E. (2010). Neural correlates of the spatial and expectancy components of endogenous and stimulus-driven orienting of attention in the posner task. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 1574–1585.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duthoo, W., Abrahamse, E. L., Braem, S., Boehler, C. N., & Notebaert, W. (2014). The heterogeneous world of congruency sequence effects: an update. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1001.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Efron, B., & Tibshirani, R. (1986). Bootstrap methods for standard errors, confidence intervals, and other measures of statistical accuracy. Statistical Science, 1(1), 54–75. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/2245500.

  • Eimer, M. (1997). Uninformative symbolic cues may bias visual-spatial attention: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Biological Psychology, 46, 67–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, C. W., & Yeh, Y. Y. (1985). Allocation of attention in the visual field. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11, 583–587.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, I., & Ansorge, U. (2012). Unconscious cueing via the superior colliculi: Evidence from searching for onset and color targets. Brain Sciences, 2, 33–60.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, I., Ansorge, U., Huber-Huber, C., Höflich, A., & Lanzenberger, R. (2015). S-ketamine influences strategic allocation of attention but not exogenous capture of attention. Consciousness and Cognition, 35, 282–294.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., Pratt, J., Colzato, L., & Godijn, R. (2001). Symbolic control of visual attention. Psychological Science, 12, 360–365.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, D. N., & Yantis, S. (1995). Allocating visual attention: Tests of a two-process model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 1376–1390.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jonides, J. (1981). Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind’s eye’s movement. In J. B. Long & A. D. Baddeley (Eds.), Attention and performance IX (pp. 187–203). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaernbach, C. (1991). Simple adaptive testing with the weighted up-down method. Perception and Psychophysics, 49(3), 227–229.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kinoshita, S., Mozer, M. C., & Forster, K. I. (2011). Dynamic adaptation to history of trial difficulty explains the effect of congruency proportion on masked priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(4), 622–636.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koshino, H., Warner, C. B., & Juola, J. F. (1992). Relative effectiveness of central, peripheral, and abrupt-onset cues in visual attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45(4), 609–631.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lambert, A., Naikar, N., McLachlan, K., & Aitken, V. (1999). A new component of visual orienting: Implicit effects of peripheral information and subthreshold cues on covert attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 321–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lambert, A., Roser, M., Wells, I., & Heffer, C. (2006). The spatial correspondence hypothesis and orienting in response to central and peripheral spatial cues. Visual Cognition, 13(1), 65–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1990). Repetition priming and automaticity: Common underlying mechanisms? Cognitive Psychology, 22, 1–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • López-Ramón, M. F., Chica, A. B., Bartolomeo, P., & Lupiáñez, J. (2011). Attentional orienting and awareness: Evidence from a discrimination task. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 745–755.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Losier, B. J., & Klein, R. M. (2001). A review of the evidence for a disengage deficit following parietal lobe damage. Neuroscience Biobehavioral Reviews, 25, 1–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Macaluso, E., & Doricchi, F. (2013). Attention and predictions: control of spatial attention beyond the endogenous-exogenous dichotomy. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience., 7, 685. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00685.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Melloni, L., Schwiedrzik, C. M., Müller, N., Rodriguez, E., & Singer, W. (2011). Expectations change the signatures and timing of electrophysiological correlates of perceptual awareness. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 1386–1396.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nelder, J. A. (1977). A reformulation of linear models. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series A (General), 140, 48–77. doi:10.2307/2344517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olk, B., Hildebrandt, H., & Kingstone, A. (2010). Involuntary but not voluntary orienting contributes to a disengage deficit in visual neglect. Cortex, 46, 1149–1164.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, S. A., & Gibson, T. N. (2011). Implicit attentional orienting in a target detection task with central cues. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 1532–1547.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pinheiro, J. C., & Bates, D. M. (2000). Mixed-effects models in S and S-PLUS. New York: Springer-Verlag. doi:10.1007/b98882.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prinzmetal, W., & Landau, A. N. (2008). Dissecting spatial visual attention. In V. Coltheart (Ed.), Tutorials in visual cognition (pp. 43–66). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinzmetal, W., McCool, C., & Park, S. (2005). Attention: Reaction time and accuracy reveal different mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 73–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • R Development Core Team (2015). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from http://www.r-project.org/.

  • Rafal, R., & Henik, A. (1994). The neurology of inhibition: Integrating controlled and automatic processes. In D. Dagenbach, & T. H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory and language (pp. 1–51). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

  • Rieth, C. A., & Huber, D. E. (2013). Implicit learning of spatiotemporal contingencies in spatial cueing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39, 1165–1180.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Risko, E. F., Blais, C., Stolz, J. A., & Besner, D. (2008a). Covert orienting: A compound-cue account of the proportion cued effect. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 15, 123–127.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Risko, E. F., Blais, C., Stolz, J. A., & Besner, D. (2008b). Nonstrategic contributions to putatively strategic effects in selective attention tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 1044–1052.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Risko, E. F., & Stolz, J. A. (2010a). The proportion valid effect in covert orienting: Strategic control or implicit learning? Consciousness and Cognition, 91, 432–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Risko, E.F., & Stolz, J. A. (2010b). On the nature of cognitive control and endogenous orienting: A response to Chica and Bartolomeo (2010). Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 445–446.

  • Ristic, J., & Kingstone, A. (2006). Attention to arrows: Pointing to a new direction. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 1921–1930.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, J. (2013). Questioning conflict adaptation: proportion congruent and Gratton effects reconsidered. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 20, 615–630.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-prime user’s guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tassinari, G., Aglioti, S., Chelazzi, L., Marzi, C. A., & Berlucchi, G. (1987). Distribution in the visual field of the costs of voluntarily allocated attention and of the inhibitory after-effects of covert orienting. Neuropsychologia, 25, 55–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tipples, J. (2002). Eye gaze is not unique: Automatic orienting in response to uninformative arrows. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 314–318.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Venables, W. N. (1998). Exegeses on linear models. Paper presented to the S- Plus User’s Conference. Washington DC, October 8–9, 1998.

  • Vossel, S., Bauer, M., Mathys, C., Adams, R. A., Dolan, R. J., Stephan, K. E., & Friston, K. J. (2014). Cholinergic stimulation enhances Bayesian belief updating in the deployment of spatial attention. Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 15735–15742.

  • Vossel, S., Mathys, C., Daunizeau, J., Bauer, M., Driver, J., Friston, K. J., & Stephan, K. E. (2014). Spatial attention, precision, and Bayesian inference: A study of saccadic response speed. Cerebral Cortex, 24, 1436–1450.

  • Wansard, M., Bartolomeo, P., Vanderaspoilden, V., Geurten, M., & Meulemans T. (2015). Can the exploration of left space be induced implicitly in unilateral neglect? Consciousness and Cognition, 31, 115–123.

  • Zhao, J., Al-Aidroos, N., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2013). Attention is spontaneously biased toward regularities. Psychological Science, 24, 667–677.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

MB was funded by an FWO Pegasus/Marie Curie IEF Fellowship within the 7th framework program (Project 625378 “SpaceLOAD”. GP is funded by the Special Research Fund from Ghent University and by the Belgian Science Policy, Interuniversity Attraction Poles program (P7/11). The authors are grateful to Marco Zorzi for providing access to lab facilities and to two anonymous reviewers for constructive comments on the manuscript. MB is grateful to Carlo Umiltà for valuable discussions on the study and to L. Naert & H. Park for proofreading.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mario Bonato.

Ethics declarations

Funding

MB was funded by a FWO Pegasus/Marie Curie IEF Fellowship within the 7th framework program (Project 625378 “SpaceLOAD”).

Conflict of interest

MB declares that he has no conflict of interest. ML declares that he has no conflict of interest. SP declares that she has no conflict of interest. GP declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Mario Bonato and Matteo Lisi contributed equally.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 30 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bonato, M., Lisi, M., Pegoraro, S. et al. Cue-target contingencies modulate voluntary orienting of spatial attention: dissociable effects for speed and accuracy. Psychological Research 82, 272–283 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0818-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0818-6

Navigation