Skip to main content
Log in

Insights into numerical cognition: considering eye-fixations in number processing and arithmetic

  • Review
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Considering eye-fixation behavior is standard in reading research to investigate underlying cognitive processes. However, in numerical cognition research eye-tracking is used less often and less systematically. Nevertheless, we identified over 40 studies on this topic from the last 40 years with an increase of eye-tracking studies on numerical cognition during the last decade. Here, we review and discuss these empirical studies to evaluate the added value of eye-tracking for the investigation of number processing. Our literature review revealed that the way eye-fixation behavior is considered in numerical cognition research ranges from investigating basic perceptual aspects of processing non-symbolic and symbolic numbers, over assessing the common representational space of numbers and space, to evaluating the influence of characteristics of the base-10 place-value structure of Arabic numbers and executive control on number processing. Apart from basic results such as reading times of numbers increasing with their magnitude, studies revealed that number processing can influence domain-general processes such as attention shifting—but also the other way round. Domain-general processes such as cognitive control were found to affect number processing. In summary, eye-fixation behavior allows for new insights into both domain-specific and domain-general processes involved in number processing. Based thereon, a processing model of the temporal dynamics of numerical cognition is postulated, which distinguishes an early stage of stimulus-driven bottom-up processing from later more top-down controlled stages. Furthermore, perspectives for eye-tracking research in numerical cognition are discussed to emphasize the potential of this methodology for advancing our understanding of numerical cognition.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Besides the reading times of numerals, eye-tracking was also employed to investigate the readability of different number formats which seem to differ with regard to their comprehensibility. Rello, Bautista, Baeza-Yates, Gerv, and Herv (2013) found that participants with dyslexia improved in text comprehension when digits and percentages were used instead of number words and fractions, respectively.

  2. While further investigating the mental representation of arithmetic facts, Zhou, Zhao, Chen, and Zhou (2012) employed horizontal electrooculography (HEOG) in single-digit addition and multiplication problems. This method only provides information about the horizontal direction (i.e., left/right) of eye-movements. In their HEOG data the authors found converging evidence for a preferred operand order in the representation of arithmetic facts. In particular, the HEOG data indicated that participants preferentially fixated the larger operand first. Again relying on HEOG, Yu, Liu, Li, Cui, & Zhou (in press, in this issue) showed that the direction of eye movements in mental arithmetic is influenced by the relative magnitude of the operands.

  3. Please note that there is an ongoing debate on whether symbolic and non-symbolic presentation formats address the same underlying representation of numbers or not (e.g., Cohen Kadosh & Walsh, 2009 for an overview and discussion). However, this is beyond the scope of this article.

References

  • Alameda, J. R., Cuetos, F., & Brysbaert, M. (2003). The number 747 is named faster after seeing Boeing than after seeing Levi’s: Associative priming in the processing of multidigit Arabic numerals. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology, 56(747), 1009–1019. doi:10.1080/02724980244000783.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ansari, D. (2008). Effects of development and enculturation on number representation in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(4), 278–291. doi:10.1038/nrn2334.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ansari, D., Garcia, N., Lucas, E., Hamon, K., & Dhital, B. (2005). Neural correlates of symbolic number processing in children and adults. NeuroReport, 16(16), 1769–1773. doi:10.1097/01.wnr.0000183905.23396.f1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bahnmueller, J., Huber, S., Nuerk, H.-C., Göbel, S. M., & Moeller, K. (2015). Processing multi-digit numbers: a translingual eye tracking study. Psychological Research. doi:10.1007/s00426-015-0729-y.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barth, H. C., & Paladino, A. M. (2011). The development of numerical estimation: Evidence against a representational shift. Developmental Science, 14(1), 125–135. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00962.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blythe, H. I., Liversedge, S. P., Joseph, H. S. S. L., White, S. J., & Rayner, K. (2009). Visual information capture during fixations in reading for children and adults. Vision Research, 49(12), 1583–1591. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2009.03.015.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624–652.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Brysbaert, M. (1995). Arabic number reading: On the nature of the numerical scale and the origin of phonological recoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 124(4), 434–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Bulf, H., Cassia, V. M., & De Hevia, M. D. (2014). Are numbers, size and brightness equally efficient in orienting visual attention? Evidence from an eye-tracking study. PLoS ONE, 9(6), e99499. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0099499.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • *Bulf, H., de Hevia, M. D., & Macchi Cassia, V. (2015). Small on the left, large on the right: Numbers orient visual attention onto space in preverbal infants. Developmental Science. doi:10.1111/desc.12315.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burr, D., & Ross, J. (2008). A visual sense of number. Current Biology, 18(6), 425–428. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.02.052.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Calvo, M. G., & Meseguer, E. (2002). Eye movements and processing stages in reading: Relative contribution of visual, lexical, and contextual factors. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 5(1), 66–77. doi:10.1017/S1138741600005849.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Ceulemans, A., Loeys, T., Hoppenbrouwers, K., & Desoete, A. (2014). To be or not to be: An informative non-symbolic numerical magnitude processing study about small versus large numbers in infants. Journal of Special Education and Rehabilitation, 15(1–2), 75–90. doi:10.2478/jser-2014-0005.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clifton, C., Staub, A., & Rayner, K. (2007). Eye movements in reading words and sentences. In R. P. G. van Gompel, M. H. Fischer, W. S. Murray, & R. L. Hill (Eds.), Eye movements: A window on mind and brain (pp. 341–372). Oxford: Elsevier Ltd.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen Kadosh, R., & Walsh, V. (2009). Numerical representation in the parietal lobes: Abstract or not abstract? The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(3–4), 313–373. doi:10.1017/S0140525X09990938.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *De Corte, E., Verschaffel, L., & Pauwels, A. (1990). Influence of the semantic structure of word problems on second graders’ eye movements. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(2), 359–365. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.82.2.359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Hevia, M. D., Girelli, L., Addabbo, M., & Cassia, V. M. (2014a). Human infants’ preference for left-to-right oriented increasing numerical sequences. PLoS ONE. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0096412.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Hevia, M. D., Girelli, L., & Cassia, V. M. (2012). Minds without language represent number through space: Origins of the mental number line. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 1–4. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Hevia, M. D., Izard, V., Coubart, A., Spelke, E. S., & Streri, A. (2014b). Representations of space, time, and number in neonates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(13), 4809–4813. doi:10.1073/pnas.1323628111.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dearborn, W. F. (1906). The psychology of reading. Psychology and Scientific Methods: Archives of Philosophy. 4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (2005). Evolution of human cortical circuits for reading and arithmetic: The “neuronal recycling” hypothesis. In S. Dehaene, J.-R. Duhamel, M. D. Hauser, & G. Rizzolatti (Eds.), From monkey brain to human brain (pp. 133–157). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Retrieved from http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.57.9491&rep=rep1&type=pdf

  • Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 122(3), 371–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (1995). Towards an anatomical and functional model of number processing. Mathematical Cognition, 1, 83–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., Dupoux, E., & Mehler, J. (1990). Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(3), 626–641.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., & Mehler, J. (1992). Cross-linguistic regularities in the frequency of number words. Cognition, 43, 1–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Delabarre, E. B. (1898). A method of recording eye-movements. The American Journal of Psychology, 9(4), 572–574.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Den Buurman, R., Roersema, T., & Gerrissen, J. F. (1981). Eye movements and the perceptual span in reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 16(2), 227–235. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(86)90037-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deubel, H., & Schneider, W. X. (1996). Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism. Vision Research, 36(12), 1827–1837. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(95)00294-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deutsch, A., Frost, R., Pelleg, S., Pollatsek, A., & Rayner, K. (2003). Early morphological effects in reading: Evidence from parafoveal preview benefit in Hebrew. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(2), 415–422. doi:10.3758/BF03196500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dobel, C., Diesendruck, G., & Bölte, J. (2007). How writing system and age influence spatial representations of actions. Psychological Science, 18(6), 487–491.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dockeray, F. C., & Pillsbury, W. B. (1910). The span of vision in reading and the legibility of letters. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1(3), 123–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dodge, R., & Cline, T. S. (1901). The angle velocity of eye movements. Psychological Research, 8(2), 145–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dooley, C. M. (2010). Young children’s approaches to books: The emergence of comprehension. The Reading Teacher, 64(2), 120–130. doi:10.1598/RT.64.2.4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duchowski, A. T. (2007). Eye tracking methodology: Theory and practice. London: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engbert, R., Nuthmann, A., Richter, E. M., & Kliegl, R. (2005). SWIFT: A dynamical model of saccade generation during reading. Psychological Review, 112(4), 777–813. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.112.4.777.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Epelboim, J., & Suppes, P. (2001). A model of eye movements and visual working memory during problem solving in geometry. Vision Research, 41(12), 1561–1574.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Erdmann, B., & Dodge, R. (1898). Psychologische Untersuchungen über das Lesen. Halle: Niemeyer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fattorini, E., Pinto, M., Rotondaro, F., & Doricchi, F. (2015). Perceiving numbers does not cause automatic shifts of spatial attention. Cortex. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2015.09.007

  • Feng, G., Miller, K., Shu, H., & Zhang, H. (2009). Orthography and the development of reading processes: An eye-movement study of Chinese and English. Child Development, 80(3), 720–735. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01293.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M. H., & Shaki, S. (2014). Spatial associations in numerical cognition—From single digits to arithmetic. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1461–1483. doi:10.1080/17470218.2014.927515.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Fischer, M. H., Warlop, N., Hill, R. L., & Fias, W. (2004). Oculomotor bias induced by number perception. Experimental Psychology, 51(2), 91–97. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.51.2.91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, A. F. (1967). Saccadic and smooth pursuit eye movements in the monkey. Journal of Physiology, 191, 609–631.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Galfano, G., Rusconi, E., & Umiltà, C. (2003). Automatic activation of multiplication facts: Evidence from the nodes adjacent to the product. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56(1), 31–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Gandini, D., Lemaire, P., & Dufau, S. (2008). Older and younger adults’ strategies in approximate quantification. Acta Psychologica, 129(1), 175–189. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.05.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ganor-Stern, D., & Weiss, N. (2015). Tracking practice effects in computation estimation. Psychological Research. doi:10.1007/s00426-015-0720-7.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gebuis, T., Gevers, W., & Cohen Kadosh, R. (2014). Topographic representation of high-level cognition: Numerosity or sensory processing? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(1), 1–3. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.10.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Gielen, I., Brysbaert, M., & Dhondt, A. (1991). The syllable-length effect in number processing is task-dependent, 50(87), 449–458.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ginsburg, V., & Gevers, W. (2015). Spatial coding of ordinal information in short- and long-term memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9(5), 1–10. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2015.00008.

    Google Scholar 

  • Göbel, S. M., Shaki, S., & Fischer, M. H. (2011). The cultural number line: A review of cultural and linguistic influences on the development of number processing. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42(4), 543–565. doi:10.1177/0022022111406251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Godau, C., Wirth, M., Hansen, S., Haider, H., & Gaschler, R. (2014). From marbles to numbers—Estimation influences looking patterns on arithmetic problems. Psychology, 05(02), 127–133. doi:10.4236/psych.2014.52020.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Green, H. J., Lemaire, P., & Dufau, S. (2007). Eye movement correlates of younger and older adults’ strategies for complex addition. Acta Psychologica, 125(3), 257–278. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.08.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann, M. (2015). Numbers in the eye of the beholder: What do eye movements reveal about numerical cognition? Cognitive Processing, 16, 245–248.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann, M., & Fischer, M. H. (2014). Pupillometry: The eyes shed fresh light on the mind. Current Biology, 24(7), R281–R282. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2014.02.028.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann, M., Mast, F. W., & Fischer, M. H. (2015a). Counting is a spatial process: Evidence from eye movements. Psychological Research. doi:10.1007/s00426-015-0722-5.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Hartmann, M., Mast, F. W., & Fischer, M. H. (2015). Spatial biases during mental arithmetic: Evidence from eye movements on a blank screen. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(January), 1–8. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00012.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, B. M., Klein, B. P., Petridou, N., & Dumoulin, S. O. (2013). Topographic representation of numerosity in the human parietal cortex. Science, 341(2013), 1123–1126. doi:10.1126/science.1239052.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Hegarty, M., Mayer, R. E., & Green, C. E. (1992). Comprehension of arithmetic word problems: Evidence from students’ eye fixations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84(1), 76–84. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.84.1.76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Heine, A., Thaler, V., Tamm, S., Hawelka, S., Schneider, M., Torbeyns, J., et al. (2010). What the eyes already “know”: Using eye movement measurement to tap into children’s implicit numerical magnitude representations. Infant and Child Development, 19(2), 175–186. doi:10.1002/icd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hendee, W. R., & Wells, P. N. T. (1997). The perception of visual information. New York: Springer Verlag.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, J. M. (1992). Object identification in context: The visual processing of natural scenes. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 46(3), 319–341. doi:10.1037/h0084325.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henik, A., & Tzelgov, J. (1982). Is three greater than five: The relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks. Memory & Cognition, 10(4), 389–395. doi:10.3758/BF03202431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hess, E. H., & Polt, J. M. (1964). Pupil size in relation to mental activity during simple problem-solving. Science, 143, 1190–1192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, J. E., & Subramaniam, B. (1995). The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements. Perception and Psychophysics, 57(6), 787–795. doi:10.3758/BF03206794.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holmqvist, K., Nyström, M., Andersson, R., Dewhurst, R., Jarodzka, H., & van de Weijer, J. (2011). Eye tracking: A comprehensive guide to methods and measures. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hubbard, T. L. (2014). Forms of momentum across space: Representational, operational, and attentional. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. doi:10.3758/s13423-014-0624-3.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Huber, S., Klein, E., Willmes, K., Nuerk, H.-C., & Moeller, K. (2014a). Decimal fraction representations are not distinct from natural number representations—Evidence from a combined eye-tracking and computational modeling approach. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 172. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00172.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • *Huber, S., Mann, A., Nuerk, H. C., & Moeller, K. (2014b). Cognitive control in number magnitude processing: Evidence from eye-tracking. Psychological Research, 78(4), 539–548. doi:10.1007/s00426-013-0504-x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Huber, S., Moeller, K., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2014c). Adaptive processing of fractions—Evidence from eye-tracking. Acta Psychologica, 148, 37–48. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.12.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Huber, S., Cornelsen, S., Moeller, K., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2015). Towards a model framework of generalized parallel componential processing of multi-symbol numbers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41, 732–745. doi:10.1037/xlm0000043.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huey, E. B. (1898). Preliminary experiments in the physiology and psychology of reading. The American Journal of Psychology, 9(4), 575–586.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inhoff, A. W. (1984). Two stages of word processing during eye fixations in reading of prose. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 23, 612–624.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inhoff, A. W. (1985). The effect of factivity on lexical retrieval and postlexical processes during eye fixations in reading. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 14(1), 45–56. doi:10.1007/BF01067473.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Irwin, D. E. (2004). Fixation location and fixation duration as indices of cognitive processing. In J. M. Henderson & F. Ferreira (Eds.), The interface of language, vision, and action: Eye movements and the visual world (pp. 105–133). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Irwin, D. E., & Thomas, L. E. (2007). The effect of saccades on number processing. Perception and Psychophysics, 69(3), 450–458.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joseph, H. S. S. L., Liversedge, S. P., Blythe, H. I., White, S. J., Gathercole, S. E., & Rayner, K. (2008). Children’s and adults’ processing of anomaly and implausibility during reading: Evidence from eye movements. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(5), 708–723. doi:10.1080/17470210701400657.Children.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joseph, H. S. S. L., Liversedge, S. P., Blythe, H. I., White, S. J., & Rayner, K. (2009). Word length and landing position effects during reading in children and adults. Vision Research, 49(16), 2078–2086. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2009.05.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). A theory of reading: From eye fixations to comprehension. Psychological Review, 87(4), 329–354.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, R. M. (1980). Does oculomotor readiness mediate cognitive control of visual attention? In R. S. Nickerson (Ed.), Attention and performance VIII (pp. 259–276). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Klein, E., Huber, S., Nuerk, H.-C., & Moeller, K. (2014). Operational momentum affects eye fixation behaviour. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1614–1625. doi:10.1080/17470218.2014.902976.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knops, A., Thirion, B., Hubbard, E. M., Michel, V., & Dehaene, S. (2009). Recruitment of an area involved in eye movements during mental arithmetic. Science, 324(5934), 1583–1585. doi:10.1126/science.1171599.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Kowler, E., & Steinman, R. M. (1977). The role of small saccades in counting. Vision Research, 17, 141–146.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Laeng, B., Sirois, S., & Gredeback, G. (2012). Pupillometry: A window to the preconscious? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(1), 18–27. doi:10.1177/1745691611427305.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Land, M. F., & Tatler, B. W. (2009). Loking and acting: Vision and eye movements in natural behaviour. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Landolt, E. (1891). Nouvelles recherches sur la physiologie des mouvements des yeux. Archives D’ophthalmologie, 11, 385–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Landy, D. H., Jones, M. N., & Goldstone, R. L. (2008). How the appearance of an operator affects its formal precedence. In Proceedings of the thirtieth annual conference of the cognitive science society, 2109–2114.

  • Lipton, J. S., & Spelke, E. S. (2004). Discrimination of large and small numerosities by human infants. Infancy, 5(3), 271–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Loetscher, T., Bockisch, C. J., & Brugger, P. (2008). Looking for the answer: The mind’s eye in number space. Neuroscience, 151(3), 725–729. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.07.068.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Loetscher, T., Bockisch, C. J., Nicholls, M. E. R., & Brugger, P. (2010). Eye position predicts what number you have in mind. Current Biology, 20(6), 264–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Macchia Cassia, V., McCrink, K., De Hevia, M. D., Gariboldi, V., & Bulf, H. (under review). Operational momentum and size ordering in preverbal infants. Psychological Research.

  • Macizo, P., & Herrera, A. (2011). Cognitive control in number processing: Evidence from the unit-decade compatibility effect. Acta Psychologica, 136(1), 112–118. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.10.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Macizo, P., & Herrera, A. (2013). The processing of Arabic numbers is under cognitive control. Psychological Research, 77(5), 651–658. doi:10.1007/s00426-012-0456-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McConkie, G. W., Zola, D., Grimes, J., Kerr, P. W., Bryant, N. R., & Wolff, P. M. (1991). Children’s eye movements during reading. In J. F. Stein (Ed.), Vision and visual dyslexia (pp. 251–262). London: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCrink, K., Dehaene, S., & Dehaene-Lambertz, G. (2007). Moving along the number line: Operational momentum in nonsymbolic arithmetic. Perception and Psychophysics, 69(8), 1324–1333.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Merkley, R., & Ansari, D. (2010). Using eye tracking to study numerical cognition: The case of the ratio effect. Experimental Brain Research, 206(4), 455–460. doi:10.1007/s00221-010-2419-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Meyerhoff, H. S., Moeller, K., Debus, K., & Nuerk, H. C. (2012). Multi-digit number processing beyond the two-digit number range: A combination of sequential and parallel processes. Acta Psychologica, 140(1), 81–90. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.11.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Milosavljevic, M., Madsen, E., Koch, C., & Rangel, A. (2011). Fast saccades toward numbers: Simple number comparisons can be made in as little as 230 ms. Journal of Vision, 11(4), 1–12. doi:10.1167/11.4.4.Introduction.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Moeller, K., Fischer, M. H., Nuerk, H.-C., & Willmes, K. (2009a). Eye fixation behaviour in the number bisection task: Evidence for temporal specificity. Acta Psychologica, 131(3), 209–220. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.05.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Moeller, K., Fischer, M. H., Nuerk, H.-C., & Willmes, K. (2009b). Sequential or parallel decomposed processing of two-digit numbers? Evidence from eye-tracking. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(2), 323–334. doi:10.1080/17470210801946740.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Moeller, K., Klein, E., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2011a). (No) small adults: Children’s processing of carry addition problems. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36(6), 702–720. doi:10.1080/87565641.2010.549880.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Moeller, K., Klein, E., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2011b). Three processes underlying the carry effect in addition—Evidence from eye tracking. British Journal of Psychology, 102(3), 623–645. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02034.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Moeller, K., Neuburger, S., Kaufmann, L., Landerl, K., & Nuerk, H. C. (2009c). Basic number processing deficits in developmental dyscalculia: Evidence from eye tracking. Cognitive Development, 24(4), 371–386. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2009.09.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Myachykov, A., Ellis, R., Changelosi, A., & Fischer, M. H. (2015). Ocular drift along the mental number line. Psychological Research. doi:10.1007/s00426-015-0731-4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noton, D., & Stark, L. (1971). Scanpaths in eye movements during pattern perception. Science, 171, 308–311. doi:10.1126/science.171.3968.308.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nuerk, H.-C., Geppert, B. E., van Herten, M., & Willmes, K. (2002a). On the impact of different number representations in the number bisection task. Cortex, 38(5), 691–715. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70038-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nuerk, H.-C., Moeller, K., Klein, E., Willmes, K., & Fischer, M. H. (2011). Extending the mental number line. Zeitschrift Für Psychologie, 219(1), 3–22. doi:10.1027/2151-2604/a000041.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nuerk, H.-C., Moeller, K., & Willmes, K. (2015a). Multi-digit number processing: overview, conceptual clarifications, and language influences. In R. Cohen Kadosh & A. Dowker (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of numerical cognition (pp. 106–139). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nuerk, H.-C., Patro, K., Cress, U., Schild, U., Friedrich, C. K., & Göbel, S. M. (2015b). How space-number associations may be created in preliterate children: Six distinct mechanisms. Frontiers in Psychology,. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nuerk, H.-C., Weger, U., & Willmes, K. (2001). Decade breaks in the mental number line? Putting the tens and units back in different bins. Cognition, 82(1), B25–B33. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00142-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nuerk, H.-C., Weger, U., & Willmes, K. (2002b). A unit-decade compatibility effect in German number words. Current Psychology Letters: Behaviour, Brain, & Cognition, 2, 19–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nuerk, H.-C., & Willmes, K. (2005). On the magnitude representations of two-digit numbers Three models of two-digit magnitude representation. Psychology Science, 47(1), 52–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinhas, M., & Fischer, M. H. (2008). Mental movements without magnitude? A study of spatial biases in symbolic arithmetic. Cognition, 109(3), 408–415. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.09.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pinhas, M., Shaki, S., & Fischer, M. H. (2014). Heed the signs: Operation signs have spatial associations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1527–1540. doi:10.1080/17470218.2014.892516.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poltrock, S. E., & Schwartz, D. R. (1984). Comparative judgments of multidigit numbers. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10(1), 32–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Porter, G., Troscianko, T., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2007). Effort during visual search and counting: Insights from pupillometry. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(2), 211–229. doi:10.1080/17470210600673818.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Pynte, J. (1974). Readiness for pronunciation during the reading process. Perception and Psychophysics, 16(1), 110–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ranzini, M., Lisi, M., & Zorzi, M. (2015). Voluntary eye movements direct attention on the mental number space. Psychological Research. doi:10.1007/s00426-015-0741-2.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. (1975). The perceptual span and peripheral cues in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 65–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 372–422. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.124.3.372.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. (2009). Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006) (Vol. 62). doi:10.1080/17470210902816461

  • Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (1989). The psychology of reading. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reichle, E. D., Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (2003). The E–Z reader model of eye-movement control in reading: Comparisons to other models. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26, 445–476. doi:10.1017/S0140525X03000104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rello, L., Bautista, S., Baeza-Yates, R., Gerv, P., & Herv, R. (2013). One half or 50%? An eye-tracking study of number representation readability. Human-Computer Interaction, 8120, 229–245.

    Google Scholar 

  • Restle, F. (1970). Speed of adding and comparing numbers. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 83(2), 274–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Rinaldi, L., Brugger, P., Bockisch, C. J., Bertolini, G., & Girelli, L. (2015). Keeping an eye on serial order: Ocular movements bind space and time. Cognition, 142, 291–298. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2015.05.022.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rivera, S. M., Reiss, A. L., Eckert, M. A., & Menon, V. (2005). Developmental changes in mental arithmetic: Evidence for increased functional specialization in the left inferior parietal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 15(11), 1779–1790. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi055.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, D. A. (1968). The oculomotor control system: A review. Proceedings of the IEEE, 56(6), 1032–1049.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, J., & Burr, D. C. (2010). Vision senses number directly. Journal of Vision, 10(2), 10.1–10.8. doi:10.1167/10.2.10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rugani, R., Vallortigara, G., Priftis, K., & Regolin, L. (2015). Number-space mapping in the newborn chick resembles humans’ mental number line. Science, 347(6221), 534–536.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Ruiz Fernández, S., Rahona, J. J., Hervás, G., Vázquez, C., & Ulrich, R. (2011). Number magnitude determines gaze direction: Spatial-numerical association in a free-choice task. Cortex, 47(5), 617–620. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2010.10.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274, 1926–1928. doi:10.1126/science.274.5294.1926.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salman, M. S., Sharpe, J. A., Eizenman, M., Lillakas, L., Westall, C., To, T., et al. (2006). Saccades in children. Vision Research, 46(8–9), 1432–1439. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2005.06.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schleger, F., Landerl, K., Muenssinger, J., Draganova, R., Reinl, M., Kiefer-Schmidt, I., et al. (2014). Magnetoencephalographic signatures of numerosity discrimination in fetuses and neonates. Developmental Neuropsychology, 39(4), 316–329.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Schleifer, P., & Landerl, K. (2011). Subitizing and counting in typical and atypical development. Developmental Science, 14(2), 280–291. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00976.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Schneider, M., Heine, A., Thaler, V., Torbeyns, J., De Smedt, B., Verschaffel, L., et al. (2008). A validation of eye movements as a measure of elementary school children’s developing number sense. Cognitive Development, 23(3), 424–437. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2008.07.002.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Schneider, E., Maruyama, M., Dehaene, S., & Sigman, M. (2012). Eye gaze reveals a fast, parallel extraction of the syntax of arithmetic formulas. Cognition, 125(3), 475–490. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.06.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Schot, W. D., van Viersen, S., van‘t Noordende, J. E., Slot, E. M., & Kroesbergen, E. H. (2015). Strategiegebruik op de getallenlijntaak geanalyseerd met behulp van eye-tracking. Pedagogische Studien, 92(1), 55–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Schwarz, W., & Keus, I. M. (2004). Moving the eyes along the mental number line: Comparing SNARC effects with saccadic and manual responses. Perception and Psychophysics, 66(4), 651–664.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siegenthaler, E., Costela, F. M., Mccamy, M. B., Di Stasi, L. L., Otero-Millan, J., Sonderegger, A., et al. (2014). Task difficulty in mental arithmetic affects microsaccadic rates and magnitudes. European Journal of Neuroscience, 39(2), 287–294. doi:10.1111/ejn.12395.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S., & Opfer, J. E. (2003). The development of numerical estimation: Evidence for multiple representations of numerical quantity. Psychological Science, 14(3), 237–250. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.02438.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, O., Mangin, J.-F., Cohen, L., Le Bihan, D., & Dehaene, S. (2002). Topographical layout of hand, eye, calculation, and language-related areas in the human parietal lobe. Neuron, 33, 475–487.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Sophian, C., & Crosby, M. E. (2008). What eye fixation patterns tell us about subitizing. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33(3), 394–409. doi:10.1080/87565640801982460.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Sullivan, J. L., Juhasz, B. J., Slattery, T. J., & Barth, H. C. (2011). Adults’ number-line estimation strategies: Evidence from eye movements. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 18(3), 557–563. doi:10.3758/s13423-011-0081-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Suppes, P., Cohen, M., Laddaga, R., Anliker, J., & Floyd, R. (1983). A procedural theory of eye movements in doing arithmetic. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 27(4), 341–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Susac, A., Bubic, A., Kaponja, J., Planinic, M., & Palmovic, M. (2014). Eye movements reveal students’ strategies in simple equation solving. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 12(3), 555–577. doi:10.1007/s10763-014-9514-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Traxler, M. J., Pickering, M. J., Communication, H., Kingdom, U., Liversedge, S., Rayner, K., & Re-, H. C. (1996). Plausibility and the processing of unbounded dependencies: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 454–475. doi:10.1006/jmla.1996.0025.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Dijck, J.-P., Abrahamse, E. L., Majerus, S., & Fias, W. (2013). Spatial attention interacts with serial order retrieval from verbal working memory. Psychological Science, 24(9), 1854–1859. doi:10.1177/0956797613479610.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Dijck, J.-P., & Fias, W. (2011). A working memory account for spatial-numerical associations. Cognition, 119(1), 114–119. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.12.013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *van Viersen, S., Slot, E. M., Kroesbergen, E. H., van’t Noordende, J. E., & Leseman, P. P. M. (2013). The added value of eye-tracking in diagnosing dyscalculia: A case study. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 679. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00679.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • van’t Noordende, J. E., van Hoogmoed, A. E., Schot, W. D., & Kroesbergen, E. H. (2015). Number line estimation strategies in childrenwith mathematical learning difficulties measured by eye tracking. Psychological Research. doi:10.1007/s00426-015-0736-z.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varma, S., & Karl, S. R. (2013). Understanding decimal proportions: Discrete representations, parallel access, and privileged processing of zero. Cognitive Psychology, 66(3), 283–301.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, G., Willmes, K., Nuerk, H.-C., & Fischer, M. H. (2008). On the cognitive link between space and number: A meta-analysis of the SNARC effect. Psychology Science Quarterly, 50(4), 489–525. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.52.3.187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynn, K. (1992). Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature, 358, 749–750.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, F., & Spelke, E. S. (2000). Large number discrimination in human infants. Cognition, 74, B1–B11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, F., Spelke, E. S., & Goddard, S. (2005). Number sense in human infants. Developmental Science, 8(1), 88–101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yu, X., Liu, J., Li, D., Cui, J., & Zhou, X. (in press). Dynamic mental number line in simple arithmetic. Psychological Research.

  • Zangemeister, W. H., Shermant, K., & Stark, L. (1995). Evidence for a global scanpath strategy in viewing abstract compared to realistic images. Neuropsychologia, 33(8), 1009–1025.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zhao, M., Gersch, T. M., Schnitzer, B. S., Dosher, B. A., & Kowler, E. (2012). Eye movements and attention: The role of pre-saccadic shifts of attention in perception, memory and the control of saccades. Vision Research, 74, 40–60. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2012.06.017.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, F., Zhao, Q., Chen, C., & Zhou, X. (2012). Mental representations of arithmetic facts: Evidence from eye movement recordings supports the preferred operand-order-specific representation hypothesis. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(4), 661–674. doi:10.1080/17470218.2011.616213.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Julia Mock was supported by the German Research Foundation (DFG) providing funding to Korbinian Moeller and Elise Klein (MO 2525/2-1). Stefan Huber was supported by the Leibniz-Competition Fund (SAW-2014-IWM-4) providing funding to Elise Klein. Elise Klein was supported by a Margarete von Wrangell fellowship from the Ministry of Science, Research and the Arts Baden-Württemberg. Korbinian Moeller is principal investigator at the LEAD Graduate School [GSC1028], a project of the Excellence Initiative of the German federal and state governments. We wish to thank Margarete Ocker for proofreading our manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to J. Mock.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no actual or potential conflicts of interest concerning this work.

Ethical approval

The manuscript does not contain clinical studies or patient data.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mock, J., Huber, S., Klein, E. et al. Insights into numerical cognition: considering eye-fixations in number processing and arithmetic. Psychological Research 80, 334–359 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0739-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0739-9

Keywords

Navigation