Skip to main content
Log in

Effects of element features on discrimination of relative numerosity: comparison of search symmetry and asymmetry pairs

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research PRPF Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We investigated effects of element features on statistical description of relative frequency. In Experiment 1, we manipulated the proportion of two elements types forming a set and the set size and measured response time and the accuracy of relative numerosity discrimination. We did this with element pairs that had been shown to produce search symmetries or asymmetries in visual search task. We found that pop-out elements in the search asymmetry pair were numerically overestimated. In Experiment 2, we used sets of circles and circles with gaps to eliminate the possibility that the overestimation found in Experiment 1 was due to larger contour length of pop-out elements. In Experiment 3, we manipulated proportion of two elements types and the set size to measure point of subjective equality (PSE) and the slopes of z-score functions in relative numerosity discrimination to support the results of Experiments 1 and 2. The results generally showed that the proportion of pop-out elements is likely to be overestimated and that set size had no effect, suggesting that the types of features characterized by visual search could influence the accuracy and precision in discrimination of relative numerosity.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allik, J., Tuulmets, T., & Vos, P. G. (1991). Size invariance in visual number discrimination. Psychological Research, 53, 290–295.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Averbach, E. (1963). The span of apprehension as a function of exposure duration. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2, 60–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barth, H., Kanwisher, N., & Spelke, E. (2003). The construction of large number representations in adults. Cognition, 86, 201–221.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, D. M., & Kastner, S. (2005). Stimulus context modulates competition in human extrastriate cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1110–1116.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burgess, A., & Barlow, B. H. (1983). The precision of numerosity discrimination in arrays of random dots. Vision Research, 23, 811–820.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cantlon, J. F., & Brannon, E. M. (2006). Shared system for ordering small and large numbers in monkeys and humans. Psychological Science, 17, 401–406.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chong, S. C., & Treisman, A. (2004). Attentional spread in the statistical processing of visual displays. Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 1282–1294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (1997). The Number Sense: How the mind creates mathematics. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., Dupoux, E., & Mehler, J. (1990). Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 626–641.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Durgin, F. H. (1995). Texture density adaptation and the perceived numerosity and distribution of texture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 149, 149–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hollands, J. G., & Spence, L. (2001). The discrimination of graphical elements. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15, 413–431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hurewitz, F., Gelman, R., & Schnitzer, B. (2006). Sometimes area counts more than number. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103, 19599–19604.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Julesz, B. (1984). A brief outline of the texton theory of human vision. Trends in Neuroscinece, 7, 41–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kadosh, R. C., Kadosh, K. C., & Henik, A. (2008). When brightness counts: The neuronal correlate of mumerical–luminance interference. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 337–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moyer, R. S., & Landauer, T. K. (1967). Time required for judgments of numerical inequality. Nature, 215, 1519–1520.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nothdurft, H. C. (1993). The role of features in preattentive vision: Comparison of orientation, motion and color cues. Vision Research, 33, 1937–1958.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, J. H., & Desmone, R. (2003). Interacting roles of attention and visual salience in V4. Neuron, 37, 853–863.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, J. (2003). Visual discrimination of number without counting. Perception, 32, 867–870.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rubenstein, B. S., & Sagi, D. (1990). Spatial variability as a liming factor in texture-discrimination tasks: Implications for performance asymmetries. Journal of Optical Society of America, 7, 1632–1643.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shuford, E. M. (1961). Percentage estimation of proportion as a function of element type, exposure time, and task. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 430–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A. (2006). How the deployment of attention determines what we see. Visual Cognition, 14, 411–443.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A., & Gormican, S. (1988). Feature analysis in early vision: Evidence for search asymmetries. Psychological Review, 95, 15–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A., & Souther, J. (1985). Search asymmetry: A diagnostic for preattentive processing of separable features. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 285–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trick, L. M., & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1993). What enumeration studies can show us about spatial attention: Evidence for limited capacity preattentive processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 231–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trick, L. M., & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1994). Why are small and large numbers enumerated differently? A limited-capacity preattentive stage in vision. Psychological Review, 101, 80–102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Varey, C. A., Mellers, B. A., & Birnbaum, M. H. (1990). Judgments of proportion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 613–625.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M. (1998). What do 1,000,000 trials tell us about visual search? Psychological Science, 9, 33–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M. (2001). Asymmetries in visual search: An introduction. Perception & Psychophysics, 63, 381–389.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Midori Tokita.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Tokita, M., Ishiguchi, A. Effects of element features on discrimination of relative numerosity: comparison of search symmetry and asymmetry pairs. Psychological Research 74, 99–109 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-008-0183-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-008-0183-1

Keywords

Navigation