Introduction

Patients with peripheral vestibular dysfunction (PVD) suffer from cognitive problems [11]. Cognitive problems are present even in mild PVD [104] and can persist long after a patient has clinically recovered [10, 88]. Several studies showed that patients with PVD have problems in spatial cognitive domains, such as spatial memory, spatial navigation, or mental rotation [8, 10, 20, 22, 39, 62, 65, 87]. However, recent research fosters the claim that patients with PVD also suffer from cognitive problems in nonspatial domains such as nonspatial memory, processing speed, or executive functions [19, 74].

Executive functions are a collection of cognitive processes responsible for purposeful, goal-directed behavior [37]. They include basic and complex cognitive processes. Basic executive functions include initiation, inhibition, cognitive flexibility, or working memory. Complex executive functions are for example problem solving, planning, or monitoring [3, 21, 29, 69]. Executive functions are essential for mental and physical health, academic and life success, and daily life functioning [27]. Impaired executive functions have intense negative consequences and cause a poor quality of life [99].

There are various hypotheses for cognitive problems in patients with PVD. Visuo-spatial problems have been associated with altered brain structures, especially in the hippocampus [15]. However, the nonspatial cognitive problems are unlikely due to changes in the hippocampus [88]. Alternative explanations are plasticity processes in the neocortex or the vestibular nuclei [54, 88], changed neuronal connections from the vestibular nerve to cortical areas involved in cognitive processing [32, 75], affective disorders [11], or prioritized attentional resources on maintaining balance [83].

Subjective reports indicate that patients with PVD have serious executive problems. Most patients in a group of patients with bilateral PVD complained about difficulties with daily life activities such as an inability to prioritize tasks [12] or problems with dual tasking [64]. Unexpectedly, cognitive-based daily life activities such as managing finances were more impaired than mobility-based activities [44]. Other studies focused specifically on the executive deficits that underlie worse cognitive performance in patients with PVD. For example, patients with acute neuritis performed worse in generating sequences of random numbers [70] or a math achievement task [71] than healthy controls. These problems indicate a deficit in working memory.

Behavioral measures in patients with PVD show executive deficits, but evidence is conflicting. Patients with bilateral as well as patients with chronic unilateral PVD performed worse in a general initiation task than healthy controls [75]. However, patients with bilateral PVD performed equally as healthy controls in a verbal fluency task, a subdomain of initiation [101]. Inhibition and working memory were found to be reduced in patients with bilateral, chronic unilateral, and acute unilateral PVD [1, 4, 23, 26, 31, 56, 70, 71, 75, 77, 98]. Some other studies, however, did not find reduced inhibition or working memory performance in patients with bilateral or chronic unilateral PVD [1, 23, 75, 98]. Cognitive flexibility was reduced in patients with chronic unilateral [26, 31], but normal in patients with bilateral [101] or a mixed sample of bilateral and chronic unilateral PVD [74, 98].

Previous studies that behaviorally measured executive functions in patients with PVD are limited for the following reasons: First, all mentioned studies investigated some isolated executive components instead of administering a comprehensive executive test battery. Therefore, previous studies do not allow for a general conclusion about impaired executive functions. Second, in some studies, executive components were measured with spatial tests [26, 31, 56], thus making it impossible to disentangle spatial from executive deficits. Third, some studies did not control for processing speed when assessing reaction time in executive tasks [31, 77]. Fourth, some studies refer to executive dysfunction when non-executive functions were assessed, e.g., short-term memory instead of working memory or motor speed instead of cognitive flexibility [42, 101].

We investigated whether executive performance in a large sample of patients with PVD (n = 83) differs from carefully pairwise matched healthy controls. We integrated several conditions of PVD (bilateral, chronic unilateral, acute unilateral). Solving limitations of previous studies, we used a comprehensive executive test battery with validated neuropsychological tests including basic and complex executive functions. Executive tests were nonspatial if possible, and we controlled for processing speed in reaction time measurements. This methodology allows thorough conclusions about executive impairments ruling out influences of impaired spatial cognition or processing speed. We also assessed subjective executive performance, the impact of PVD on daily life functioning, and as control variables  intelligence and global cognitive level. Moreover, we investigated whether non-vestibular related variables in patients and controls (hearing loss, affective disorders) and vestibular related variables in patients (disease duration, symptoms, dizziness handicap, deafferentation degree, and compensation) influenced our results.

We hypothesized that executive performance differs between patients with PVD and healthy controls. In addition, we hypothesized that non-vestibular related variables may explain the differences in executive performance between patients with PVD and healthy controls. Further, we hypothesized that vestibular related variables in patients predict executive performance.

Methods

Ethical considerations

The study was conducted in agreement with the Declaration of Helsinki. The study protocol was approved by the ethics committee of the Canton Bern, Switzerland. All participants gave their written informed consent prior to study participation. Participants received a compensation of 80 Swiss francs.

Participants

Patients

Ninety-eight patients were recruited from the Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Head and Neck Surgery, University Hospital of Bern, Switzerland. According to the criteria of the Bárány Society [92, 94], patients suffered from either chronic bilateral (n = 40), chronic unilateral (n = 36), or acute unilateral (n = 22) PVD. PVD was defined as chronic if the diagnosis lasted for at least 6 months, or acute if a patient was diagnosed maximum 1 month ago [14, 93, 94]. Patients with chronic PVD (chronic bilateral and chronic unilateral) were recruited from the neurotological database, whereas patients with acute PVD were recruited at the ENT emergency station between 2018 and 2020. PVD had different etiologies: the most common etiology was idiopathic in patients with bilateral PVD (representative for bilateral PVD, see [57]), vestibular schwannoma in patients with chronic unilateral PVD, and vestibular neuritis in patients with acute PVD. Detailed diagnosis of patients included in the study is presented in “Results”. Patients were asked to participate in our study by written letter.

All patients underwent a neurotological examination with Videonystagmography (VNG, NysStar II®, Difra Instrumentation, Belgium). Vestibulo-ocular reflex was measured using the bithermal caloric test (low frequencies). The function of the six semicircular canals at high frequencies was assessed using the video head impulse test (v-HIT, ICS Impulse®, Otometrics, Denmark). The function of the otolithic organs was recorded using the click-evoked cervical, and ocular vestibular evoked myogenic potentials (c-VEMP, o-VEMP, Eclipse VEMP®, Intercoustics GmbH, Germany). The vestibulo-spinal reflex was measured with dynamic posturography (Unterberger testing, SwayStar, [2]).

Patients were excluded from the study if they fulfilled one of the following criteria: age below 18 or above 80 years; central vestibular disorders [51], peripheral polyneuropathy; serious cardiovascular, metabolic, neurologic, or degenerative disease; neuroleptics; cerebral concussion during the year prior to the study, or insufficient German language skills.

To investigate whether vestibular related variables influenced our main results, we measured and defined five vestibular related variables in patients: disease duration, symptoms, dizziness handicap, deafferentation degree, and compensation.

Disease duration was defined as the time between the diagnosis of PVD and study participation. The timepoint of diagnosis was taken from the patient medical reports.

Symptoms were measured by asking patients about the intensity of actual vestibular symptoms on the day of study participation. Symptoms included vertigo, vertigo in head and body movements, unsteady gait in light and darkness, fall tendency, oscillopsia, and movement sensations. Each symptom was evaluated on a Likert scale from 1 to 10.

Dizziness handicap quantifies the self-perceived impact of dizziness on daily life. Dizziness handicap was assessed with a German version of the Dizziness Handicap Inventory (DHI-G, [34, 53]).

Deafferentation degree was classified into two groups: complete or incomplete vestibular deafferentation. Complete vestibular deafferentation was defined by pathological semicircular canal and pathological otolithic functions. Incomplete vestibular deafferentation was used when semicircular canal function at low frequencies was pathological while otolithic functions remained intact. In case of unilateral PVD, areflexia of the lateral semicircular canal in caloric testing was defined as an absence of nystagmic response, and hyporeflexia as a unilateral vestibular paresis of > 20% side difference, calculated according to Jongkee’s formula. In case of bilateral PVD, areflexia was defined as an absence of nystagmic response on both sides, and a hyporeflexia as a reduced caloric response calculated according to the criteria of the Bárány Society [92]. Within v-HIT, gains of VOR < 0.8 (horizontal canals) and < 0.6 (vertical canals) were considered as decreased. Within cervical and ocular VEMPS, the presence or absence of P1 and N1 determined the presence or absence of responses, respectively.

Vestibular compensation was evaluated by the degree of body sway using the roll angle parameter of the dynamic posturography [2] during the task walking in place with eyes closed (Unterberger testing). The central compensation mechanism in the brain following PVD is a complex mechanism involving vestibular, visual, and somatosensory cues. The Unterberger testing corresponds to one of the clinical signs of the central compensation mechanism.

Healthy controls

Fifty-four healthy controls were recruited by newspaper advertisements and word of mouth. Controls were recruited pairwise comparable to each patient in sex, age, and education. Differences between a patient and a control were maximal 4 years of age (three exceptions of 5 years) and two levels in maximal reached education (four exceptions of more than two levels) (assessment of education level is described in “Procedure”). In some cases, the same healthy control served as multiple control for patients from different patient groups (e.g., the same healthy control for a bilateral and a chronic unilateral patient). This procedure allowed us to have as few controls as necessary. Pairwise matches of patients and controls are presented in the Supplementary Material (Supplementary Table S1). We created three different control groups for the three patient groups (bilateral controls, chronic unilateral controls, acute unilateral controls). Using this careful pairwise matching strategy, we can minimize demographic influences on executive performance. Controls had no past neurotological disease. To exclude present vestibular dysfunction, controls underwent a neurotological examination. The neurotological examination included the same tests as for patients (described above). Three controls preferred not to undergo the neurotological examination. They answered screening questions of PVD instead and indicated no vestibular related problems. Exclusion criteria for healthy participants were the same as in patients (described above).

Procedure

The neuropsychological examination lasted 90–120 min with a break halfway of the test battery. The order of the neuropsychological tests was the same for each participant. Tests requiring a high need for concentration were placed at the beginning and after the break. Examiners had at least bachelor’s degree in Psychology and were intensively trained in doing the neuropsychological assessments in a highly standardized manner. Standardized assessments are crucial to exclude subjective influences of examiners on the evaluation of neuropsychological performance.

Demographic variables and non-vestibular related variables were quantified with paper pencil questions. Education was measured as the maximal education reached (0 = primary education; 1 = secondary education, first level; 2 = secondary education, second level; 3 = apprenticeship or further training; 4 = high school; 5 = bachelor’s degree; 6 = master’s degree; 7 = Ph.D.). The classification of education in gradual levels rather than years of schooling is considered to better assess the link between education and the capacity of the brain to cope with neuropathology or age-related changes [35]. To assess non-vestibular related variables, we asked participants in written form about hearing loss and psychological disorders.

Neuropsychological assessment

We comprehensively measured executive functions with several validated neuropsychological tests. Our test battery included basic executive functions (initiation, inhibition, cognitive flexibility, working memory) and complex ones (problem solving, planning, monitoring). An overview of the executive tests is shown in Table 1 and illustrated in Fig. 1. Executive tests were nonspatial if possible. We chose tests that are solvable despite hearing loss, and we prepared written instructions for participants suffering from hearing loss. We added one subjective measure of executive impairments (Frontal System Behaviour Scale, [40]). Beside executive functions, we measured the impact of PVD on daily life functioning (Neuropsychological Vertigo Inventory, [55]). As control variables, we assessed intelligence and global cognitive level. The neuropsychological tests used in this study are described in detail in the appendix.

Table 1 Overview of the neuropsychological tests and questionnaires to assess executive functions, impact of peripheral vestibular dysfunction on daily life functioning, and control variables (intelligence, global cognitive level)
Fig. 1
figure 1

Neuropsychological tests to assess basic and complex executive functions. Basic and complex executive functions were measured with validated neuropsychological tests. Basic executive functions (on the left) were measured with the following tests: a initiation: a1 general initiation with the Alertness test, a2 nonverbal initiation with the Design Fluency test, a3 verbal initiation with the Word Fluency test; b inhibition with the Inhibition condition of the Color–Word Interference test; c cognitive flexibility with the Switching condition of the Color–Word Interference test, d working memory: d1 performance with the 2-back task, d2 maximal span with the digit span backwards test. Complex executive functions (on the right) were measured with the following tests: e problem solving with the two groups test; f planning with the Tower of London; g monitoring by computing a mean score of errors, rule violations, and perseverations of the other neuropsychological tests. See appendix for detailed task descriptions and references

Data analysis

We used double data entry approaches for neuropsychological evaluations and data entry. Two independent examiners evaluated the neuropsychological data by judging or summing up scores. Two independent examiners transformed paper–pencil to digital values. Using double data entry approaches, we avoided both rater subjectivity and transmission errors. We used raw values from the neuropsychological tests to compare performance of patients with PVD to controls. By recruiting pairwise matched healthy controls, we can rule out influences of sex, age, and education on interpretations of neuropsychological raw values.

Statistical analyses were performed in R (version 4.3.1, [76]). First, it was analyzed whether patient groups differ significantly from their respective control group in control variables of intelligence and global cognitive level. We verified normal distributions with Shapiro–Wilk tests. We used two-sided paired t-tests to examine whether mean performance of intelligence and global cognitive level in patient groups significantly differed from their respective control group.

Second, we analyzed whether PVD patient groups differed in executive performance from their respective control group. We verified normal distributions with Shapiro–Wilk tests. Most of the tests were distributed normally. Therefore, we used two-sided paired t-tests for the executive tests to examine whether mean performance in patient groups significantly differed from their respective control group. To correct for alpha inflation error because of multiple comparisons, we used Bonferroni–Holm corrections. We report uncorrected as well as alpha inflation corrected results.

Third, we exploratively analyzed whether non-vestibular related variables in patients and controls (hearing loss, affective disorders) explain differences in executive performance between patients and controls. We computed linear regression models for executive tests that differed significantly between patients and controls with the covariates hearing loss and affective disorders.

Fourth, we exploratively analyzed whether vestibular related variables in patients (disease duration, symptoms, dizziness handicap, deafferentation degree, and compensation) predict worse executive performance. We computed multiple regression analysis for the executive tests that differed significantly between patients and controls with the vestibular related variables as covariates. Two covariates (symptoms and dizziness handicap) showed high multicollinearity (R > 0.70, p < 0.001). Therefore, we took the mean of those variables into the multiple regression models.

Results

Data exclusion

Fifteen patients had to be excluded due to recovered vestibular function (n = 9), exclusion criteria that the patients did not disclose prior to study participation (n = 4), or incidental findings in magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) that was assessed for another study (n = 2). Therefore, we analyzed data from 83 patients whereof 34 had bilateral, 29 chronic unilateral (right-sided n = 15, left-sided n = 14), and 20 acute unilateral (right-sided n = 12, left-sided n = 8) PVD. PVD had different etiologies. In patients with bilateral PVD, the etiologies were: idiopathic on both sides (n = 16), meningitis (n = 8), vestibular schwannoma and idiopathic on the contralateral side (n = 3), vestibular schwannoma and vestibular neuritis on the contralateral side (n = 1), endolymphatic hydrops on both sides (n = 3), vestibular neuritis and idiopathic on the contralateral side (n = 1), vestibular neuritis on both sides (n = 1), and gentamycin ototoxicity (n = 1). In patients with chronic unilateral PVD, the etiologies were: vestibular schwannoma (n = 18), vestibular neurectomy (n = 3), labyrinthectomy (n = 1), endolymphatic hydrops (n = 3), vestibular neuritis (n = 2), Zoster oticus (n = 1), and meningitis (n = 1). In patients with acute unilateral PVD, the etiologies were: vestibular neuritis (n = 18), and idiopathic (n = 2). Demographic and clinical data of the patients are provided in Tables 2 and 3. Detailed clinical data of individual patients (diagnosis, semicircular function, otolithic function, and deafferentation degree) are presented in the Supplementary Material (Supplementary Table S2).

Table 2 Demographic data of the patient groups with different conditions of peripheral vestibular dysfunction (bilateral, chronic unilateral, acute unilateral) and their respective healthy controls
Table 3 Clinical data of the patient groups with different conditions of peripheral vestibular dysfunction (bilateral, chronic unilateral, acute unilateral)

Five controls had to be excluded due to exclusion criteria that the controls did not disclose prior to study participation (n = 2) or incidental findings in MRI that was assessed for another study (n = 3). Therefore, we analyzed data from 49 healthy controls. Demographic data of the controls are provided in Table 2.

For some neuropsychological tasks, we had to exclude patients and their paired healthy controls due to missing values of patients or their respective controls. We had missing values in verbal initiation (1 bilateral), inhibition and cognitive flexibility (1 bilateral, 1 acute unilateral), working memory performance (3 bilateral, 2 chronic unilateral, 3 acute unilateral), and global cognitive level (4 bilateral). Some questionnaires about subjective executive functions and impact of PVD on daily life functioning were not returned by patients or their respective healthy control (5 bilateral, 2 chronic unilateral, 1 acute unilateral).

Control variables

Means and standard deviations of the two control variables (intelligence, global cognitive level) are shown in Table 2. Intelligence did not differ significantly between patients with bilateral PVD and respective controls (t(33) = − 1.78, p = 0.084), patients with chronic unilateral PVD and respective controls (t(28) = 0.68, p = 0.501), or patients with acute unilateral PVD and respective controls (t(19) = 0.37, p = 0.715). Similarly, global cognitive level did not differ significantly between patients with bilateral PVD and respective controls (t(29) = − 1.54, p = 0.134), patients with chronic unilateral PVD and respective controls (t(28) = − 0.52, p = 0.610), or patients with acute unilateral PVD and respective controls (t(19) = − 0.46, p = 0.648).

Executive functions

Means and standard deviations of all executive tests from patients with bilateral, chronic unilateral, and acute unilateral PVD and their respective controls are shown in Table 4. To compare executive performance between patient groups and their respective control groups, t-values, p-values, and p-values corrected for multiple comparisons are shown in Table 4.

Table 4 Executive performance of the patient groups with different conditions of peripheral vestibular dysfunction (bilateral, chronic unilateral, acute unilateral) and their respective healthy controls

Verbal initiation differed significantly between patients with bilateral PVD and respective controls (t(32) = − 4.45, p < 0.001, d = 0.77), and between patients with acute unilateral PVD and respective controls (t(19) = − 2.10, p = 0.049, d = 0.47). After Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons, verbal initiation still significantly differed between patients with bilateral PVD and respective controls (t(32) = − 4.45, p < 0.001, d = 0.77). But patients with acute unilateral PVD did no longer significantly differ in verbal initiation from respective controls (t(19) = − 2.10, p = 0.497). Patients with chronic unilateral PVD did not differ in verbal initiation from respective controls (t(28) = − 1.64, p = 0.112). Results of verbal initiation are shown in Fig. 2.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Verbal initiation performance of patients with bilateral, chronic unilateral, and acute unilateral peripheral vestibular dysfunction and their respective pairwise matched healthy controls. Verbal initiation was assessed with a word fluency task. Results are presented as means. Error bars show standard errors. Significant differences between patients and controls are indicated with asterisks (*p < 0.001)

Similarly, working memory span differed significantly between patients with bilateral PVD and respective controls (t(33) = − 3.67, p < 0.001, d = 0.63), and between patients with acute unilateral PVD and respective controls (t(19) = − 2.78, p = 0.012, d = 0.62). After Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons, working memory span still significantly differed between patients with bilateral PVD and respective controls (t(33) = − 3.67, p = 0.009, d = 0.63). But patients with acute unilateral PVD did no longer significantly differ in working memory span from respective controls (t(19) = − 2.78, p = 0.119). Patients with chronic unilateral PVD did not differ in working memory span from respective controls (t(28) = − 0.79, p = 0.438). Results of working memory span are shown in Fig. 3.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Working memory span of patients with bilateral, chronic unilateral, and acute unilateral peripheral vestibular dysfunction and their respective pairwise matched healthy controls. Working memory span was assessed with a digit span backwards task. Results are presented as means. Error bars show standard errors. Significant differences between patients and controls are indicated with asterisks (*p < 0.001)

Performance in other executive tests did not differ significantly between patients with bilateral PVD and respective controls, patients with chronic unilateral PVD and respective controls, or patients with acute unilateral PVD and respective controls (p > 0.050).

Non-vestibular related variables and vestibular related variables

Numbers of participants with hearing loss and affective disorders are shown in Table 2. Numbers of participants with complete or incomplete vestibular deafferentation, as well as means and standard deviations of disease duration, symptoms, dizziness handicap, and vestibular compensation are shown in Table 3.

After adjusting for hearing loss, verbal initiation and working memory span differed statistically significant between patients with bilateral PVD and respective controls (F(3, 63) = 5.00, p = 0.004, d = 0.35), (F(3, 64) = 6.25, p < 0.001, d = 0.37). After adjusting for affective disorders, verbal initiation and working memory span differed statistically significant between patients with bilateral PVD and respective controls (F(3, 62) = 4.01, p = 0.011, d = 0.57), (F(3, 63) = 6.05, p = 0.001, d = 0.66).

Vestibular related variables did not predict verbal initiation performance in patients with bilateral PVD. The multiple regression model with verbal initiation as dependent variable and the vestibular related variables as covariates (disease duration, mean of symptoms and dizziness handicap, deafferentation degree, and compensation) was not significant (F(4, 27) = 2.20, p = 0.096). None of the vestibular related variables significantly predicted verbal initiation (p > 0.050).

Vestibular related variables did not predict working memory span in patients with bilateral PVD. The multiple regression model with working memory span as dependent variable and the vestibular related variables as covariates (disease duration, mean of symptoms and dizziness handicap, deafferentation degree, and compensation) was not significant (F(4, 28) = 1.26, p = 0.308). None of the vestibular related variables significantly predicted working memory span (p > 0.050).

Discussion

We investigated whether executive performance in a large sample of patients with several conditions of PVD (bilateral, chronic unilateral, acute unilateral) differs from pairwise matched healthy controls. Verbal initiation and working memory span were impaired in patients with bilateral and patients with acute unilateral PVD, but not in patients with chronic unilateral PVD. After correcting for multiple comparisons only patients with bilateral PVD performed worse than controls in verbal initiation and working memory span. Performance in all other executive tests (general initiation, nonverbal initiation, inhibition, cognitive flexibility, working memory performance, problem solving, planning, and monitoring) was not impaired in patients with bilateral, chronic unilateral, or acute unilateral PVD compared to their respective controls. We also investigated whether non-vestibular related variables in patients and controls (hearing loss, affective disorders) and vestibular related variables in patients (disease duration, mean of symptoms and dizziness handicap, deafferentation degree, and compensation) influenced our results. Hearing loss and affective disorders did not explain differences between patients with bilateral PVD and controls. None of the vestibular related variables predicted verbal initiation or working memory span in patients with bilateral PVD.

Executive impairments in patients with bilateral PVD

Impaired verbal initiation in patients with bilateral PVD conflicts with a previous result showing normal verbal initiation in patients with bilateral PVD [101]. Two differences can account for the diverging results. First, Wang et al. [101] used a semantic fluency task to assess verbal initiation, whereas we used a word fluency task. These tasks might not be comparable and assess different aspects of verbal initiation [9, 60, 80]. Second, Wang et al.'s [101] sample included patients with only mild bilateral PVD, whereas we included patients with complete vestibular deafferentation. More severe cognitive deficits are expected in patients with complete vestibular deafferentation [38].

Impaired working memory span in patients with bilateral PVD confirms results from a previous study [23]. Using the same task, Danneels et al. [23] found impaired working memory span in patients with bilateral PVD with and without hearing loss. The mean working memory span of our patients with bilateral PVD (M = 4.35) is highly comparable to the bilateral patient group with hearing loss in the study of Danneels et al. (M = 4.37) [23]. However, our results conflict with normal working memory span in patients with bilateral PVD, which was also assessed with the same task [1]. Contrary to our study, Ahmad et al. [1] excluded patients with surgery, tumors, neurectomy, ototoxic medication, and hearing loss. Consequently, it is likely that patients with bilateral PVD in the study of Ahmad et al. [1] had a lower degree of vestibular deafferentation. Impaired working memory span may show up only in patients with a high degree of vestibular deafferentation [38]. In addition, Ahmad et al. [1] based diagnosis of bilateral PVD exclusively on rotary chair testing, and they did not assess normal vestibular function in healthy controls. Potential mild PVD in some healthy controls might have biased their results. Van Hecke et al. [98] found normal working memory span in children with bilateral PVD. However, compared to healthy controls, average performance of children with bilateral PVD was reduced. The small sample size (n = 9) might not have allowed enough statistical power to detect differences between children with bilateral PVD and controls.

Although patients with bilateral PVD performed worse in working memory span (digit span backwards), they performed normally in working memory performance (2-back task). Working memory span and working memory performance correlate weakly and seem to measure different aspects of working memory [50, 96]. In a 2-back task that we used in our study, the maximal span to be held in working memory is two. Patients may not have problems with working memory tasks limited to a span of two but are impaired in reaching longer spans. However, n-back tasks have been criticized in that they are insufficient reliable and do not purely measure working memory but rely more on information processing or motor speed [49, 50, 67]. Moreover, it has been suggested that n-back tasks are not a useful measure of individual differences in working memory [49]. Therefore, results from the digit span backwards task may be more reliable. The digit span backwards task is also assumed to be a representative measure of working memory [27, 45].

Considering our comprehensive assessment of executive functions, we argue that patients with bilateral PVD suffer from a specific working memory span impairment rather than general executive impairments. Specifically impaired working memory span, but preserved other executive functions have also been observed in healthy participants receiving high-current galvanic vestibular stimulation [81]. Patients with bilateral PVD performed equally as healthy controls in all other assessed executive components. In fact, the impaired verbal initiation that we also observed in these patients may be explained by working memory span problems, because verbal fluency tasks rely on working memory capacity [6, 30, 33, 66].

A possible mechanism between bilateral PVD and impaired working memory span may be structural changes in or disturbed vestibular input to the inferior frontal gyrus. The inferior frontal gyrus is involved in vestibular processing [61, 68]. Gray matter volume increases in this area have been associated with vestibular compensation after vestibular neuritis [48]. Interestingly, the inferior frontal gyrus is also activated during working memory tasks and verbal fluency tasks [46, 73, 103]. Previous studies in patients with bilateral PVD reported structural changes predominantly in the hippocampus or hippocampal substructures following PVD [15, 38, 52, 82]. Future studies should use structural MRI to examine alterations in the inferior frontal gyrus in patients with bilateral PVD. In addition, the inferior frontal gyrus in patients with bilateral PVD should be investigated in functional imaging studies while patients are solving a working memory or verbal fluency task in the MRI scanner.

Alternatively, results could be explained by a speech problem in patients with bilateral PVD. Verbal initiation and working memory span were the only tasks in our test battery that required actively produced speech-based responses. However, we think that this explanation is unlikely. Previous studies showed no general speech problem in patients with bilateral PVD [13]. Moreover, previous studies reporting visuo-spatial working memory impairments did not use speech-based tasks [56, 75]. Nevertheless, future studies should use also non-speech-based working memory tasks to exclude an influence of speech.

We are able to exclude some alternative explanations for the impaired verbal initiation and working memory span in patients with bilateral PVD. First, contrary to some earlier studies [26, 31, 56], we used nonspatial tasks and can therefore rule out visuo-spatial influences on executive performance. The working memory span task may have a visuo-spatial component (mental number line), but the verbal initiation task does not rely on visuo-spatial performance. In addition, patients with bilateral PVD did not differ from healthy controls in other tasks using numbers in our test battery. Second, our results in patients with bilateral PVD remained significant after controlling for hearing loss or affective disorders. This confirms recent results showing that PVD is associated with cognitive dysfunction independent of hearing loss [86]. Third, variables such as disease duration, symptoms, dizziness handicap, deafferentation degree, or compensation did not predict verbal initiation performance or working memory span. However, the influence of vestibular related variables on executive performance was not the main aim of our study and we did not recruit patients respectively. Future studies should investigate executive performance in combination with vestibular related variables (e.g., compare patients with symptoms vs. no symptoms or patients with complete deafferentation vs. incomplete deafferentation). Fourth, impaired verbal initiation and working memory span in patients with bilateral PVD cannot be explained by impaired intelligence or global cognitive level, as those variables did not differ from those of healthy controls.

No executive impairments in patients with acute unilateral PVD?

After controlling for multiple comparisons, the effect of impaired verbal initiation and working memory span in patients with acute PVD diminished, although the exact same pattern of impairments as in patients with bilateral PVD has been observed descriptively. Patients with acute unilateral PVD might suffer from impaired verbal initiation and working memory span, but the effects are less strong when compared to patients with bilateral PVD. Nonsignificant results after correcting for multiple comparisons could be explained by a smaller sample size of patients with acute PVD (n = 20) compared to patients with bilateral PVD (n = 34). We assume that patients with acute PVD suffer from impaired verbal initiation and working memory span, but we were not able to demonstrate the effect with our data. Future studies should examine this line of research further.

No executive impairments in patients with chronic unilateral PVD

Patients with chronic unilateral PVD did not perform worse in executive tasks than healthy controls. This finding confirms some previous results [1, 75]. However, it conflicts results of previously observed impaired executive functions in patients with chronic unilateral PVD [26, 31, 56, 75, 77]. Conflicting results are most likely due to the fact that earlier studies used visuo-spatial measures [26, 31, 56] or did not control for processing speed when assessing executive functions [31, 77]. Fan et al. [31] found impaired working memory in patients with chronic unilateral PVD using a nonspatial task. However, their sample included exclusively patients with untreated acoustic neurinoma, whereas our sample included treated and untreated patients. Interestingly, Popp et al. [75] used the same task to assess general initiation that was used in our study. However, contrary to our study, Popp et al. [75] did not adapt the task for hearing loss, although this is suggested in a vestibular patient group [28].

We can rule out that normal executive performance in patients with chronic unilateral PVD is due to less symptoms or dizziness handicap than in patients with bilateral or patients with acute unilateral PVD. Patients with chronic unilateral PVD reported nearly equal intensity of symptoms as patients with acute PVD and dizziness handicap as patients with bilateral PVD. However, patients with chronic unilateral PVD had better vestibular compensation values than patients with bilateral or patients with acute unilateral PVD. Although this difference was not statistically significant, we argue that vestibular compensation can prevent executive problems. Acute unilateral PVD may lead to impaired working memory span, but vestibular compensation can help to recover from those impairments. Future longitudinal studies with patients with acute unilateral PVD should examine this line of research.

Implications

As patients with PVD are treated by various multidisciplinary work groups [79], the knowledge gained from our results are not only important for neurotologists, but also for neurologists, audiologists, or other patient support groups. Improving diagnostic and treatment procedures for patients with PVD requires awareness of cognitive impairments in those patients.

Reduced working memory span can have intense negative consequences, because intact working memory is crucial for everyday life, at the workplace, or in social situations (e.g., [16,17,18, 27, 36, 59, 78]). Therefore, impaired working memory span might lead to those deficiencies in daily life activities that have been reported in patients with PVD [44]. Compared to other executive functions, working memory best predicted impairments in daily life in older adults [99]. Moreover, working memory predicts academic performance (e.g., [24, 85, 100]).

Due to its wide-ranging negative consequences, impaired working memory span in patients with bilateral PVD should be treated. We suggest interventions to treat impaired working memory that have been investigated in other patient populations or healthy participants. These intervention suggestions should be investigated in patients with bilateral PVD in future studies.

The most obvious strategy to treat impaired working memory would be a cognitive training in the form of a working memory training. In such a training, participants learn, for example, to improve their working memory span by repeated sessions of a backward memory span task (e.g., [58, 106]). It has been shown that effects of a working memory training transfer to other tasks not specifically trained [16, 17, 58, 78, 106]. Therefore, such a training could improve a variety of tasks in daily life or at work. It has been shown that executive performance has a predictive effect on balance and gait in older adults or cognitively impaired participants [5, 89,90,91, 95, 97, 105]. Thus, a working memory training might have additional advantageous side effects on balance functions in patients with PVD.

It has to be pointed out that a cognitive training is demanding and needs a high patient motivation. Alternatively, patients could benefit from psychoeducational offers. Psychoeducation could include concrete suggestions in dealing with impaired working memory span or, for example, altered working strategies or working time adjustments at the workplace. Psychoeducational strategies are generally less demanding for patients. However, positive effects are limited to a specific problem and adaptations at the workplace need the consent of employers.

Integrating screenings for executive impairments (working memory span, verbal initiation) in standard neurotological assessments would help to identify patients with executive problems. This would allow to intervene in an early stage after PVD and prevent long-term consequences that may result from cognitive impairments (e.g., absence from work).

Strengths and limitations

This study has several strengths. First, we assessed a comprehensive range of executive functions including basic and complex executive functions. We used nonspatial, validated neuropsychological tests and controlled for processing speed when measuring reaction times. Therefore, we can make thorough conclusions about executive impairments ruling out influences of impaired spatial cognition or processing speed. Second, executive tests were administered in a highly standardized manner. In addition, using double data entry approaches for test evaluation and data entry, we avoided rater subjectivity and transmission errors. Third, we recruited pairwise matched healthy control groups highly comparable to patients in sex, age, and education. With our recruitment strategy, we minimized demographic influences on executive performance. Fourth, we integrated a large patient sample and several conditions of PVD (laterality, course). Conclusions about different PVD groups can facilitate strategies in clinical diagnostics and rehabilitation.

Besides its strengths, our study has two limitations. First, although we included patients with different conditions of PVD (bilateral, chronic unilateral, and acute unilateral), patients within the groups had heterogeneous diagnoses. However, the dysfunction of vestibular input is likely more influential on executive performance than the underlying specific diagnosis. Future studies will need to compare different conditions (laterality, course) and also different diagnosis leading to PVD. Second, as this was not the primary objective of the study, we assessed hearing loss and affective disorders by questioning participants instead of using validated questionnaires or objective measures. Therefore, we had no fine-grained assessment of hearing loss or affective disorders, and we could have missed weak expressions. Future studies should assess those variables with validated or objective tests. However, the results of this study can hardly be explained by hearing loss or affective disorders as these conditions would be expected to impair cognition more generally rather than specific executive functions in isolation (e.g., [41], [63]).

Conclusion

Patients with bilateral peripheral vestibular dysfunction performed worse in the specific executive functions of verbal initiation and working memory span when compared to pairwise matched healthy controls. Patients with bilateral peripheral vestibular dysfunction should be screened for executive impairments and if indicated, receive cognitive training or psychoeducation.