Introduction

Dementia is currently recognised as an umbrella term for a number of neurological conditions, of which the major symptom is the decline in brain function due to physical changes in the brain [1]. 60–70% of cases are attributed to Alzheimer’s disease (AD), although other subtypes exist including vascular (VD), frontotemporal and Lewy body dementia [2]. Owing to ageing populations, dementia prevalence is increasing. It is now the most common ageing-related disease, with over 55 million cases reported worldwide [3]. With the incidence rate of dementia at 10 million, it is estimated that 140 million people will be living with dementia by 2050 [3]. At present, there is no cure for dementia, and as such research prioritises disease prevention and retarding progression. With ~ 35% of dementia attributable to modifiable risk factors [4], the association between metabolic disease and dementia is emerging. Dementia risk increases with every additional component of the metabolic syndrome [5]. In addition, socioeconomic status (SES), strongly associated with metabolic disease [6], also increases dementia risk [7], suggesting that health inequalities contribute to increasing dementia prevalence [8].

The substantial transformation of global food systems, with accelerating ultra-processed food (UPF) production and consumption, provides fertile ground for widening health inequalities along the socioeconomic gradient [9]. UPFs are manufactured foods, such as confectionary sweets, sugar sweetened beverages (SSBs) and packaged ‘ready-meals’, formulated from by-products of high yield crops and remnants of intense animal agriculture and characterised by high energy–density with poor nutritional profiles [10]. UPFs were designed to be convenient, enjoyable and cheaper than minimally processed foods; as such they contribute ~ 50% of total energy intake (TEI) in the western world, increasing to 70% in children and lower income households [11, 12]. NOVA is the full and proper name of a tool used to stratify food products to one of four groups based on processing-related criteria (Supplementary Table S1). It has been deemed the most comprehensive classification system for UPFs [13].

To date, meta-analyses demonstrate associations between diet quality and dementia, with adherence to a Mediterranean diet being protective [14, 15], as well as convincingly demonstrating associations between UPF and non-communicable diseases including obesity, type 2 diabetes (T2D), fatty liver disease (NAFLD) and cardiovascular disease (CVD) [16,17,18,19,20]. Moreover, experimental studies highlight associations between UPFs and cognitive decline [21,22,23]. Therefore, it is imperative to objectively review the association between UPFs and dementia through meta-analysis, whilst accounting for confounding variables such as age, SES and co-morbidity that independently increase dementia risk [3].

Thus, the primary aim of the current review is to assess and quantify the relationship between UPF consumption and dementia prevalence, through systematic review and meta-analysis. The secondary aims are to assess and quantify the relationship between UPFs and dementia subtypes, and to determine whether a dose–response relationship exists between UPFs and dementia.

Methods

The protocol for this review was registered on PROSPERO (CRD42023388363).

Search strategy and selection criteria

The Preferred Reporting Items of Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses guidelines were used to construct this review [24]. Medline and Web of Science were searched (AH and CG) on 24.12.2022 for all original research describing associations between UPF and dementia. The search algorithm used was comprised of two groups of keywords described in Supplementary Table 2, with the Boolean operator ‘OR’ separating words within groups, and ‘AND’ being used between groups. We also performed manual searches of reference lists of relevant studies and contacted experts in the field to identify additional articles. No restriction was placed on the earliest search date.

Selection criteria

To be included in our study, the criteria highlighted in Supplementary Table S3 were applied. We included MCI in our definition of dementia as per previous meta-analyses which have studied the relationship between diet with dementia [14, 15]. NOVA assigns food to one of four groups: (NOVA1) unprocessed or minimally processed foods; (NOVA2) culinary ingredients; (NOVA3) processed foods; and (NOVA4) ultra-processed foods [13]. A more detailed breakdown of what foods make up these four groups can be found in Supplementary Table S1. To be selected, studies needed to define foods as being in NOVA4 using the NOVA classification tool, or, alternatively, provide enough information on the foods consumed to enable us to determine their NOVA group.

Outcome

The primary outcome of our study was the difference in dementia prevalence between patients with low vs. high UPF intake. Low UPF intake, defined as the non- or lowest consumption, was our reference group. High UPF intake was defined as the highest value. The secondary outcomes were (1) the difference in prevalence of dementia subtypes between patients with low vs. high intake of UPF, and (2) assessment of a dose–response association between UPF consumption and dementia. Secondary outcome 1 was assessed by stratifying dementia into all-cause dementia and the dementia subtypes, whilst outcome 2 was assessed by the difference in dementia prevalence in patients with a low vs moderate and high UPF intake. We considered moderate UPF intake to be the group after the reference group; in other words, the first exposure group (most commonly the second quartile or tertile of intake). The exception to this was if intake was stratified by quintiles, in which case the third quintile was used.

Study selection

Two reviewers (AH and CG) used the selection criteria to identify appropriate literature from databases; using Rayyan to navigate the selection process. Articles were screened by titles and abstract before full texts of selected articles were reviewed. Disagreements were resolved via discussion between the two reviewers.

Data extraction

The following data was extracted by reviewer one (AH) and independently checked by reviewer two (CG): (1) basic study information (author name, year of publication, journal) and (2) study design, population, country, study type, sample size, follow up, adjustment for confounders, definitions for UPF and dementia (dietary assessment tool, use of NOVA, dementia diagnostic tool), study outcomes (reported risk estimates in relation to dementia development). For studies that did not report the necessary data, corresponding authors were contacted.

Quality of evidence

The Newcastle Ottawa scale (NOS) [25] was utilised to assess evidence quality. NOS is a validated tool recommended by Cochrane to assess observational research. It is composed of eight items that evaluate study selection, comparability and outcome, with a maximum score of nine. We stratified evidence into three groups: low quality scored < 5 stars, medium quality scored 5 or 6 stars and high-quality scored > 6 stars [16]. In addition, the NutriGrade scoring system was used to assess evidence credibility. The tool is an eight-item scale that evaluates evidence for meta-analyses related to nutrition. To interpret NutriGrade evaluation, the following scoring system was used: (a) very low (0–3.99); (b) low (4–5.99); (c) moderate (6–7.99); (d) high (8–10) [26].

Meta-analysis

A random-effects model was used to calculate a pooled relative risk (RR) ± 95% confidence interval (CI) because data was expected to be highly heterogenous. The random effects model used was Cochran-Mantel-Haenzel test [27]. This was perfomed using R Studio V4.0.2 (R Studio PBC, USA, meta, ggplot2, and metafor packages). The Higgins I2 statistical technique was used to assess heterogeneity [28], supported by the Paule-Mandel test to estimate tau2 [29]. To be considered highly heterogenous, values were required to be > 75% (p =  < 0.05). Thereafter, summary level meta-regression analysis was performed using a fixed-effects model to evaluate whether a dose–response association existed between increasing UPF intake and risk of dementia (metareg and bubble functions from meta package).

Sensitivity analyses assessed the impact of the following variables: study design, reporting of UPF (NOVA or non-NOVA classified), sample size, continent, type of dementia, and study quality. A further series of analyses were performed, based on adjustment for confounders, to strengthen any association between UPF and dementia. We used the result from the fully adjusted model in the included studies when conducting all analyses. Begg’s funnel plots were generated for visualisation of publication bias.

Results

Study characteristics

A flowchart demonstrating the selection process of the studies is illustrated in Fig. 1. After duplicates were excluded (n = 550), 7010 records were identified and nine met inclusion criteria. A further single study was included after searching reference lists, bringing the total number of included studies to ten. In total, 867,316 participants were analysed. In full text review, most studies were excluded due to not allowing classification of UPFs via NOVA (n = 87) or lacking sufficient data to perform meta-analysis (n = 17).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analysis (PRISMA) reported flow diagram for study selection process

The main demographics and results of the included studies are shown in Table 1. The studies included participants from Asia [30, 31], USA [32,33,34] and Europe [21, 35,36,37,38]. Eight studies were longitudinal, with follow-up ranging from 6.8 to 22 years [33, 37], one was case–control [36], and one was cross-sectional in design [31]. Sample sizes ranged from 108 to 493,888 [36, 38]. All studies adjusted for age and sex, alongside other confounders such as SES (including education level) (n = 9), TEI (n = 7), body mass index (BMI) (n = 9), CVD (n = 9) and T2D (n = 9). Nine studies included both males and females, with one study including only males [37]. One study directly referenced NOVA [21], with nine studies allowing indirect assessment of foods named as ultra-processed by NOVA. This included dietary patterns that were rich in UPFs [31,32,33], and focus on specific UPFs such as processed meats [35, 37, 38] or SSBs [34]. Three studies used data from the UK biobank, with two commenting on processed meats [35, 38] and one on all UPFs [21]. All studies collected dietary information via Food Frequency Questionnaires (FFQs). Seven studies evaluated the association between UPFs and dementia [21, 32, 34,35,36,37,38], and three with MCI [30, 31, 33]. Dementia was classified according to International Classification of Diseases (ICD) in five articles [21, 32, 35, 37, 38], with the Diagnostic and Statistical Manuscript of Mental Disorders (DSM) being used for two studies [34, 36]. For MCI, validated screening items were used [30, 31, 33]. All studies were published after 2017.

Table 1 Description of studies included in the systematic review (n = 10)

Supplementary Table S4 shows the quality of evidence as reported by the NOS. The studies had NOS scores between 6 and 8, with a mean of 7.5. Nine studies were considered high quality, with one study considered moderate quality and higher risk of bias [30]. Supplementary Table S5 shows the NutriGrade evaluation of evidence credibility which is considered high with an overall score of 9.

Systematic review

Three out of seven studies reported significant associations between high UPF consumption and the development of dementia (excluding MCI) [21, 34, 38]. The four non-significant studies reported a trend towards significance for dementia risk [32, 35,36,37]. Two studies reported significant associations between high UPF consumption and the development of MCI [30, 31]. The one non-significant study reported a trend towards significance [33]. Two studies reported significant associations between UPF consumption and the development of all-cause dementia with moderate intake of UPFs [34, 38].

Association between ultra-processed food intake and dementia

All cause dementia: High intake of UPFs was associated with increased risk of developing all-cause dementia (pooled RR 1.44 (1.09–1.90) (p = 0.02)), with high heterogeneity (97.0% (p < 0.01)) (Fig. 2a). Moderate intake of UPFs was not associated with increased risk of developing all-cause dementia (pooled RR 1.12 (0.96–1.31) (p = 0.13)), with high heterogeneity (I2 = 85.0% (p =  < 0.01)) (Fig. 2b).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Forest plots from a random-effects model portraying the association between a high (vs low) ultra-processed food intake and development of all-cause dementia; b moderate ultra-processed food intake and development of all-cause dementia

Dementia sub-types: No significant associations existed between high intake of UPFs and any of the following: dementia excluding MCI (pooled RR 1.24 (0.93–1.65) (p = 0.11)) (I2 = 94.2% (p =  < 0.01)), AD (1.08 (0.79–1.48) (0.52) (48.3% (0.10)), VD (2.05 (0.39–10.90) (0.12) (0.0% (0.43)) or MCI (2.01 (0.75–5.42) (0.09)) (95.7% (< 0.01)) (Fig. 3a–d).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Forest plots from a random-effects model portraying the association between a high ultra-processed food intake and development of dementia (excluding mild cognitive impairment); b high ultra-processed food intake and development of mild cognitive impairment; c high ultra-processed food intake and development of Alzheimer dementia; d high ultra-processed food intake and development of vascular dementia

Bubble plots demonstrate the dose–response relationship between UPF intake and all-cause dementia (Supplementary Fig. S5).

Sensitivity analysis

Significant associations remained between high UPF intake and all-cause dementia when: NOVA was not directly referenced (1.47 (1.08–2.01) (0.02)) (97.3% (< 0.01)), longitudinally designed (1.47 (1.02–2.11) (0.04)) (97.7% (< 0.01)) and low risk of bias (1.31 (1.05–1.64) (0.02)) (93.7% (< 0.01)). When the study by Dobreva et al. was excluded owing to using a similar cohort from the UK Biobank as the larger study by Zhang et al., significant associations remained (1.46 (1.07–2.01) (0.02)) (97.3% (< 0.01))[35, 38]. All other sensitivity analyses produced non-significant results and are presented in Supplementary Table S6 with corresponding forest plots in Supplementary Fig. S1 and funnel plots in Supplementary Fig. S2.

Adjusting for confounders

Significant associations remained between high UPF intake and all-cause dementia when studies adjusted for BMI (1.46 (1.07–2.00) (0.02)) (97.3% (< 0.01)), CVD (1.46 (1.07–2.00) (0.02)) (97.3% (< 0.01)) and SES (1.39 (1.02–1.90) (0.04)) (97.2% (< 0.01)). When studies adjusted for T2D (1.47 (0.97–2.00) (0.06)) (97.9% (< 0.01)) and TEI (1.26 (0.95–1.67) (0.09)) (93.9% (< 0.01)), significant associations were lost. Forest plots are presented in Supplementary Fig. S3 and funnel plots in Supplementary Fig. S4.

Publication bias

Begg’s funnel plots visually represent publication bias among the included studies. Funnel plots suggest low risk of publication bias with good relative symmetry (Fig. 4).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Funnel plots portraying risk of publication bias for studies assessing the association between a high ultra-processed food intake and development of all-cause dementia; b moderate ultra-processed food intake and development of all-cause dementia; c high ultra-processed food intake and development of dementia (excluding mild cognitive impairment); d high ultra-processed food intake and development of mild cognitive impairment; e high ultra-processed food intake and development of Alzheimer dementia; f high ultra-processed food intake and development of vascular dementia

Discussion

We present the first systematic review and meta-analysis to assess the association between ultra-processed food consumption and dementia, convincingly demonstrating that high UPF intake is associated with dementia and suggesting that ultra-processed diets could contribute to cognitive impairment. However, there was not a statistically significant relationship between moderate intake of UPFs and dementia, meaning we did not demonstrate a robust dose–response relationship between the quantity of UPFs consumed and dementia prevalence.

The beneficial effect of diet quality on cognitive health has previously been explored [14, 15]. The relationship between UPFs and dementia is likely multifactorial and related to several inter-related mechanisms including indirectly from the development of cardiovascular risk factors such as hypertension/hyperlipidaemia [42,43,44], obesity and T2D [46] and directly from the consumption of high energy–density foods with inherent poor macro- and micronutrient profiles [40] and harmful chemical additives that together can alter gut microbial diversity [47]. Macronutrients relevant include dietary saturated fat, trans-fat, refined carbohydrates and low dietary fibre [45]. Micronutrients relevant include high sodium salt content in UPFs [48, 49]. Finally, artificial additives to food [10], such as monosodium glutamate [51], and packaging, such as bisphenol A [52], may accelerate cognitive decline, highlighted by artificially sweetened beverages demonstrating associations with all-cause dementia [53]. Possible biological pathways include pro-inflammatory adipokine and hormone secretion that promote neurodegeneration through amyloid deposition, vascular remodelling [39], cerebral microvascular dysfunction [50] and brain insulin resistance [40, 41].

The association of UPFs with other markers of poor metabolic health has been shown for a variety of conditions supporting the biological plausibility of our findings. A strong association exists between UPFs and multiple cardiometabolic diseases such as obesity, T2D, CVD, metabolic-associated steatotic liver disease and at least thirteen hormone dependent cancers [16,17,18,19,20]. The impact of this UPF consumption on health and disease is frequently characterised by end-organ damage including the liver, heart and kidneys. The brain appears also to be a target for such end-organ damage. Included studies adjusted for a similar battery of confounding cardiometabolic variables and significant results remained when sensitivity analyses were performed using studies adjusted for BMI, CVD and SES (Supplementary Fig. S3). Of course, RCTs assessing the association between UPFs and incident cognitive impairment/dementia would be ethically unjustifiable considering the associations described above between UPFs and cardiometabolic disease [16, 18, 19]. Hence the current review synthesises best available data to provide this evidence.

Although we provide strong evidence for a high intake of UPFs being associated with dementia, between-study heterogeneity was observed in respect to classification of UPF, study design, sample size, method of diagnosing dementia and participant age. To this end, we performed sensitivity analyses to identify the impact of heterogeneity on our results (Supplementary Table S6). Classifying UPFs in the original studies lacked standardisation. One study referenced NOVA in its methodology, whilst nine assessed various foods that were retrospectively defined as ultra-processed by our research team using NOVA criteria. Ultra-processing of specific foods (i.e., processed meats [38]) may increase dementia risk more than others. Similarly, this meant there was no pre-determined cut-off to quantify exposure. Moderate intake in one population may exceed high intake in another. Despite this, significant associations remained between high intake of UPF and dementia when sensitivity analysis was performed in respect to UPF classification (Supplementary Fig. S1a). Funnel plots highlight greater effect size range in studies with small sample sizes, and results lose significance when performing sensitivity analysis based on a sample size cut-off point of 10,000 participants (Supplementary Fig. S1d & e). In addition, studies with AD/VD as the outcome used ICD or DSM codes, whereas no studies with MCI as the outcome did so despite codes existing, and all included studies assessed diet using FFQs which increases recall bias and underreporting of true intake [54]. Together, this highlights the need for more robust population-based research assessing the association between UPF and dementia using a standardised classification approach.

What are the implications of our findings? To optimise brain health with aging, we need to consider strategies to reduce global UPF consumption. Mexico, UK and South Africa have all implemented sugar-based taxes on SSBs and packaged foods [55, 56] based on the premise that taxation of UPFs could subsidise the cost of minimally processed foods. Careful political consideration would be needed to spread the distribution of fresh produce equally so as not to disproportionately favour economically advanced nations.

In conclusion, the current systematic review and meta-analysis highlighted an association between the consumption of a high intake of UPFs and dementia. Further robust population-level characterisation of UPF consumption using the standardised NOVA classification system is required to determine more precisely the magnitude of, and the temporal relationship between, UPF intake and dementia. However, our findings highlight the contributory role of UPF consumption to the development of dementia and that co-ordinated global and national public health policies and clinical guidelines are needed to displace consumption of UPFs with fresh, minimally processed, easily affordable food, to tackle the societal burden of dementia.