Introduction

Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS) is a form of non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) that utilizes electrodes on the scalp to create a weak electrical current in order to modulate cortical excitability [1]. In the treatment of major depressive disorder (MDD), anodal tDCS is usually applied over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) [2], a brain area which contributes to frontoparietal network (FPN) function [3]. The FPN plays a central role for several cognitive domains, like attention[4], working memory [5], memory span [6] executive function [7], processing speed[8], and cognitive control [9]. Poor performance in these cognitive domains has also been associated with depressive disorders [10,11,12,13,14]. Therefore, it seems plausible that stimulation of the FPN could influence performance in these domains and that baseline cognitive performance, as a proxy of FPN functioning, could predict the clinical effects of stimulation.

Previous studies have investigated the neurocognitive effects of tDCS when applied to the DLPFC in patients with MDD reporting significant time-dependent improvements in attention/vigilance, working memory, executive functioning, processing speed, and social cognition when compared to placebo [15,16,17,18]. On the other hand, multiple studies report no statistically significant group-by-time interaction effects [19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27]. A recent meta-analysis of the cognitive effects of tDCS across multiple disorders revealed that active tDCS elicited improvements in attention/vigilance, and working memory when compared to sham tDCS [28]. This meta-analysis was based on studies that were very heterogeneous in designs, sample sizes, outcomes, and main findings. Thus, a study with a large sample size would be warranted to further test the effects of tDCS on cognition in patients with MDD. To the best of our knowledge, no studies have investigated baseline cognitive testing as a predictor of affective response to tDCS.

In this ancillary analysis of a triple-blind, randomized, sham-controlled multicenter trial, we investigated whether a standard bifrontal tDCS protocol compared to sham tDCS alters cognitive performance across the domains of memory span, working memory, selective attention, sustained attention, executive functioning, and processing speed. Additionally, we explored whether baseline cognitive performance as a proxy of FPN functioning predicts the antidepressant effects of tDCS versus sham tDCS.

Methods and materials

Study population

We analyzed data from the DepressionDC trial (trial registration number: NCT02530164); a triple-blind, randomized, sham-controlled clinical trial carried out across eight psychiatric centers in Germany [29]. The study investigated the efficacy and safety of tDCS as a treatment for MDD in patients that did not respond to conventional pharmacological treatment with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs). Patients were originally randomized to receive 24 sessions within 6 weeks of either active or sham tDCS. The montage employed in tDCS involves placing the anode over F3 and the cathode over F4. Active stimulation consisted of a constant 2 mA direct current that lasted for 30 min. The sham paradigm consisted of a ramp-up and ramp-down sequence to induce similar skin sensations as active tDCS. tDCS was applied using a DC-stimulator (‘Mobile’, neuroConn GmbH, Ilmenau, Germany). Inclusion and exclusion criteria are reported in the supplement. Local ethics committees approved the study at each study site. All participants gave their written informed consent before inclusion in the study. From an initial sample total of 150 patients (intention-to-treat sample), we analyzed the data from 101 patients that had available neuropsychological assessments. Data from 49 patients were missing due to technical errors, organizational difficulties at the local treatment sites, and refusal to participate.

Neurocognitive test battery

Neurocognitive function was assessed longitudinally during the study at baseline, post-treatment (week 6), and at the 6-month follow-up using the EmoCogMeter, a digitalized, validated cognitive test battery developed at the Charite Berlin [30,31,32]. The EmoCogMeter examines the domains of memory span, working memory, selective attention, sustained attention, executive function, and processing speed. Memory span is tested by a digit-span assessment [33]. Working memory was assessed by an n-back task [13]. A variant of the Stroop test and a working memory component were used to assess selective attention and sustained attention, respectively [34]. executive function was measured by both the Trail Making B [35] and Tower of Hanoi tests [36]. Finally, processing speed was measured using a symbol letter modalities test, a variation of the symbol digit modality test. For additional technical information about the tests, please refer to the supplement.

Further outcome measures

The severity of the depressive episode was assessed by trained clinical staff utilizing the Montgomery-Åsberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS), which was also chosen for the primary outcome of the study [37]. Severity is classified as an absence of symptoms (0–6 points), mild depressive episode (7–19 points), moderate depressive episode (20–34 points), or severe depressive episode (35–60 points). State and trait anxiety were measured utilizing The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) [38], with a threshold of 39–40 for identifying clinically significant anxiety symptoms [39].

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were conducted in R, version 4.2.1. results [40]. Results were considered significant at α =0.05. We compared baseline characteristics between treatment groups using Pearson's χ2 tests and Wilcoxon-rank-sum tests as appropriate. To reduce the effect of extreme test performances, we identified values below the 1% and above the 99% percentile on each task and set them to the respective percentile values (winsorization).

To assess potential treatment effects of active tDCS on cognitive performance, we fitted linear mixed models using the lme4 package [41] to predict change from baseline to week 6 on each cognitive test. Treatment group (active tDCS versus sham tDCS) was included as a fixed effect while controlling for the respective baseline cognitive test score (formula: change in cognitive performance ~ treatment group + baseline cognitive performance). Sensitivity analyses included additional models with sex, age, and baseline MADRS as covariates.

To assess potential predictive influences of baseline cognitive performance on antidepressant treatment effects of active tDCS, we again fitted linear mixed models to predict change from baseline to week 6 on the MADRS. Treatment group, performance on the respective cognitive domain, and their interaction were included as fixed effects while controlling for baseline MADRS scores (formula: MADRS change ~ treatment group x cognitive performance at baseline + baseline MADRS score).

All models included the treatment site as a random effect (formula: ~ 1| site). Significance of the model factors was determined using omnibus tests (Type III ANOVA) with Satterthwaite approximation to degrees of freedom. We did not use imputation since linear mixed models are able to handle missing data. Standardized effect sizes for regression coefficients were computed using the emmeans::eff_size() approach, with the sigma parameter being directly extracted from the regression model [42]. We corrected for multiple testing across predictors using the false-discovery-rate (FDR) method [43].

Results

Sample characteristics

We analyzed data from 101 patients (active tDCS, n = 50; sham tDCS, n = 51). Mean age (active tDCS 39 [SD 14]; sham tDCS 39 [SD 14]; p = 0.76). Sex: active tDCS 40% male; sham tDCS 40% male. Primary baseline and clinical features across the active and sham-tDCS groups were similar (Table 1 and Supplementary Table 1). Winsorized mean test performances and the number of winsorized measurements per cognitive test are reported in supplementary Table 5 and 6.

Table 1 Baseline patient characteristics

Treatment effects on neurocognitive test scores

We observed no significant group-by-time interactions between treatment group and memory span, working memory, selective attention, sustained attention, executive function, or processing speed. Pre- and post-treatment performance across neurocognitive tests for active tDCS and sham tDCS is shown in Fig. 1, and Table 2 provides further statistical information. Results for additional models including sex, age and baseline MADRS yielded similar results (supplementary Table 2–4).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Pre- and post-treatment performance across neurocognitive tests for active tDCS and sham tDCS. Note: Error bars indicate mean (SE). Boxplots include the IQR with whiskers indicating 1.5 times IQR. Thin lines represent patient-individual changes

Table 2 Treatment effects on neurocognitive test scores

Prediction of clinician-rated depression (MADRS)

We did not detect significant interactions, when predicting MADRS change, between treatment group and memory span, working memory, selective attention, sustained attention, executive function, or processing speed. Table 3 provides the effect size of each neurocognitive test at baseline and Fig. 2 depicts the association between baseline cognitive performance and changes in MADRS scores.

Table 3 Prediction of changes MADRS
Fig. 2
figure 2

Association between baseline cognitive performance and MADRS change across the trial. MADRS Montgomery-Åsberg Depression Rating Scale

Discussion

In this ancillary analysis of the DepressionDC trial, a randomized, sham-controlled multicenter study assessing the antidepressant efficacy of a prefrontal tDCS as acute treatment in patients with MDD and SSRI treatment, we found no statistically significant group differences between active tDCS and sham tDCS for the change of performance in FPN-associated cognitive domains (i.e. memory span, working memory, selective attention, sustained attention, executive function and processing speed) from baseline to week 6. Furthermore, baseline performance in these domains was not differentially associated with a change in depression severity for active tDCS compared to sham tDCS.

Our results are in contrast to a recent meta-analysis that found significant effects of tDCS on working memory and attention [28]. This meta-analysis was based on studies with sample sizes between n = 18 [15] and n = 127 [26] the number of treatment sessions (one [24] up to 22 [26] and tDCS dosages(0.5 mA [21, 27], 1 mA [15, 20] and 2 mA [16,17,18, 22,23,24,25,26]) was highly heterogeneous. Among single studies included in this meta-analysis, several authors reported an improvement of attention/vigilance, working memory, executive functioning, processing speed, and social cognition [15, 17], spatial working memory [18] or processing speed [16]. However, other studies in this meta-analysis are rather in line with our findings and did not show significant effects of tDCS on performance in neurocognitive domains [20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27]. The ELECT-TDCS trial, a clinical study with identical stimulation parameters and a larger sample size, did not find significant effects on cognition either [26].

There are several potential reasons for these negative findings. First, our multicenter trial tested only one set of tDCS parameters with the aim of reducing depressive symptoms. However, dose–response curves for single domains of neurocognitive performance have not been established. They may be non-linear and could theoretically vary from one domain to another [44, 45] as well as from dose–response curves of antidepressant effects. While being in line with previous studies on antidepressant tDCS, the administered dosage in our trial might have been insufficient to optimally modulate specific prefrontal cognitive functions. Second, the main trial did not show beneficial antidepressant effects of active tDCS over sham tDCS. Thus, the applied tDCS protocol might have also been not potent enough to modulate neuroplasticity changes in general. Third, high levels of arousal, estimated by using the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI), have been reported to diminish cognitive practice effects elicited by tDCS, [46] underlining the potential role of arousal in shaping responses to neuromodulation. In our study, both groups had high baseline STAI scores, and such high baseline anxiety could have reduced the effects of tDCS on neurocognitive performance. Lastly, several studies have reported that tDCS might only elicit procognitive effects when simultaneously combined with specific cognitive tasks [47,48,49,50,51,52]. Thus, passive stimulation, as administered in our trial, might not be sufficient to enhance cognition in patients with MDD.

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study that investigates whether cognition at baseline may be used to predict improvement of depression during a course of tDCS. Our study has multiple strengths. The study followed the highest possible trial design standards by being triple-blinded, placebo-controlled, and multicenter. We applied a tDCS protocol (2 mA, 30 min) established in previous studies which showed a superior antidepressant efficacy of active over sham tDCS, i.e. the SELECT-TDCS [53] and ELECT-TDCS [26] trials, and our data-set is one of the biggest samples in the field to date (n = 101). Furthermore, we used a validated digital assessment battery that has successfully been used in other previous studies [31, 32, 54]. While efforts are being made to digitize previously validated cognitive tests [55, 56], such tools which also reduce documentation errors [57, 58], are still underused.

Limitations

First, there is no uniform consensus on what neurocognitive tests are better used to evaluate the performance in domains associated with FPN function. Our battery included some of the most common tests and slight variations of them. However, other standardized tests could have a higher sensitivity and specificity for detecting neuromodulation effects on cognitive performance [59]. Second, digital tools present a few caveats such as failure of the equipment, corruption of data, and loss of information when retrieving the data. This limited the availability of data in our study. Third, the evaluation of procognitive effects of tDCS and the potential predictive effects of baseline cognition on treatment response were ancillary investigations. Though this data was well balanced across both conditions, there may be latent selection biases making the sample not representative for the whole study population. In addition, the current analysis was likely underpowered to detect small treatment and prediction effects. Lastly, all patients were on a stable SSRI medication for at least 4 weeks prior to inclusion, but not antidepressant-free. Thus, our conclusions regarding the differential effects of SSRI medication and tDCS on performance in distinct neurocognitive domains are limited.

Conclusion

In conclusion, our analysis does not support the notion that acute treatment with active tDCS compared to sham tDCS leads to an improvement in FPN-related neurocognitive functions. In addition, neurocognitive functioning at baseline did not predict the change of MADRS scores over the course of tDCS. Future research should aim at identifying tDCS protocols with optimal dose–response curves for effects on specific neurocognitive domains. Most promising candidates could then be further optimized by adjusting parameters at an individual patient's level.