Introduction

The psychological consequences of combat in World War I (WWI) were often dramatic and presented serious manpower problems for the military authorities [30, 35]. The phenomenology and natural history of these stress-induced mental disorders are of interest to today’s mental health practitioners because of the increasing awareness of psychological trauma in combat personnel, which poses considerable clinical and social challenges. The psychological traumata of WWI are also of particular theoretical interest because they sparked the first international debate on the origins and mechanisms of functional disorders. This paper will analyse case records from a cohort of traumatised German soldiers from 1915 to 1918 with regard to clinical presentation, triggering events, course and treatment response. We will also discuss the causal explanations given by clinicians and put them in the context of the contemporaneous biopsychological debates.

A key question in the medical debates of WWI was whether the behavioural and neurological symptoms found in soldiers without visible wounds were the result of organic brain damage, produced by shock waves from explosions or were psychological effects of traumatic experience; a debate which is currently being re-run on the issue of mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI). British medicine remained ambivalent about this issue and organic interpretations prevailed long into the war, epitomised by the label ‘shell shock’. In Germany, the supporters of an organic interpretation, who were led by the Berlin-based neurologist Hermann Oppenheim (1858–1919), were soon outnumbered by the much larger group of neurologists and psychiatrists who attributed functional neurological symptoms to psychological causes, using the label of ‘Hysterie’ (hysteria). Oppenheim’s main opponent, the Hamburg physician Max Nonne (1861–1959) promoted the treatment of war neurosis with suggestion under hypnosis [38]. Another crucial question was whether war was the causative factor in the genesis of the hysterical symptoms, or whether an underlying ‘psychopathic’ predisposition was the primary agency. This question was not only relevant from the perspective of psychological theory and nature-nurture debates, but also determined the practical issues of compensation and pensions.

Most modern accounts of shell shock and related phenomena are based on medical publications from WWI [30, 47] rather than clinical records of the time. Most research into British cases was based on pension files [25] and the admission records of army psychiatric units established close to the frontline [23]. On the German side, Lemke [29] reported contemporaneous diagnoses of 2,000 soldiers admitted to the Department of Psychiatry of Jena University, then headed by Otto Binswanger (1852–1929). However, no study has assessed the psychopathology and neurological symptoms of German combatants in detail and classified them according to present-day diagnostic categories. Interestingly, one of the most systematic studies of war syndromes was published in 1920 by the German psychiatrist and later Communist activist Fritz Fraenkel (1892–1944). Based on his thesis supervised by Karl Bonhoeffer (1868–1948) (Fig. 1), Fraenkel described a sample of 72 patients, mainly soldiers, who had been admitted in 1916 to the Department of Psychiatry of the Charité, the teaching hospital of Berlin University, which was then headed by Bonhoeffer [6]. Fraenkel provided a statistical analysis of the frequencies of different functional neurological syndromes and concluded that a psychopathic constitution was the causative factor in the development of a neurosis. The present paper is based on a similar sample of admissions of military personnel to the Charité from 1915 to 1918.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Karl Bonhoeffer (1968–1948), around the time of his retirement from the chair in psychiatry at the Charité in 1937. Source Bildarchiv des Instituts für Geschichte der Medizin, Charité Berlin

This study explores the distribution of functional syndromes across the whole period of the war. It also documents the views of the hospital’s clinicians on the relative roles of war trauma and predisposing factors (‘psychopathic constitution’), while a third aim is to analyse the frequency of the different treatments and assess their reported effects. Finally, we classified the cases according to modern diagnostic criteria to provide a basis for comparisons with other countries and across time.

Methods

During WWI, 1043 servicemen were admitted to the Department of Psychiatry of the Charité (1914: 217, 1915: 357, 1916: 212, 1917: 152, 1918: 105). We randomly selected 100 servicemen (9.6% of the whole sample; 25 cases each from 1915, 1916, 1917 and 1918). A random number sequence was generated for all admission numbers by year and 25 subjects selected for each of the 4 years. The case records provide a precise account of the soldier’s war experience, his biography, presenting symptoms and response to treatment. During WWI, the ‘Psychiatrische und Nervenklinik’ (department for psychiatric and nervous disorders) of the Charité had the status of a military hospital. The department comprised a neurological and a psychiatric wing and served as an admission unit for diagnostic assessment and initiation of treatment within the network of psychiatric hospitals in Berlin. The treatment of mentally ill servicemen was subordinated to the military authorities who regularly sent their patients directly to the Charité, which also had the status of a training unit for military doctors. The Berlin case files include detailed descriptions of symptoms, their course during admission, a thorough clinical examination (including an in-depth neurological and cognitive assessment), interviews of family members and detailed assessments by other specialists such as ENT surgeons, dermatologists, neurologists and general physicians. The structure of the case files is illustrated in the extracts in the ‘Appendix: case vignette’ (see Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Cover of a medical record from the Department of Psychiatry at the Charité from 1918. For details, see the case vignette. Source Historisches Psychiatriearchiv Charité Berlin, M8906/1918

We extracted contemporaneous diagnoses from the case notes. We also assigned the presenting symptoms to 10 syndrome categories (1: Psychotic; 2: Affective; 3: Adjustment disorders; 4: Personality disorders; 5: Sensory-motor conversions [motor: gait disorders, paralyses, contractures, speech disorders; sensory: disturbances of sensation, hearing, vision]; 6: Functional seizures; 7: Somatoform disorders; 8: Other dissociative syndromes [other than dissociative disorders of movement and sensation covered under 5 and 6]; 9: Organic disorders; 10: No mental disorder). Finally, the author S.L. diagnosed all cases according to ICD-10 criteria. The diagnosis was based on all information found in the case records including the onset of symptoms, the presenting mental state and the clinical course of the disorder. All diagnoses were reviewed by an independent consultant psychiatrist (D.L.) with high inter-rater reliability (Cohen’s k = 92.5% [4]).

Results

Description of sample

The demographics of the sample are documented in Table 1. Nearly half of the soldiers of our sample were transferred from military hospitals in Berlin or nearby, whereas 25 soldiers were directly sent by their combat units, 14 came from other hospitals (including rehabilitation units) or other specialties within the Charité (particularly the departments of medicine and neurology) and the remaining 12 were admitted under other circumstances (from custody, leave from their force or referral from a private neurologist or psychiatrist).

Table 1 Sociodemographic data of the selected 100 admissions to the Charité psychiatry department in 1915–1918 (n = 100)

The ratio of officers to men in the wartime German army was 3.6 per 100 (that is 46,622 officers to 1,281904 other ranks and non-commissioned officers [37]), which suggests that commissioned officers were slightly under-represented in our sample. This is in contrast to other accounts of breakdown rates by rank in WWI [21] and WWII [19]. However, admission biases and a tendency to care for officers with mental illness outside the mainstream mental health system in order to protect them from stigma may have contributed to their under-representation and the overall small number of officers in our sample does not allow drawing firm conclusions.

Trigger events and previous psychiatric history

In 61% of cases, a specific trigger for the development of symptoms was mentioned (see Table 2). Physical traumata, for example burial and injuries (30%), were most frequently associated with mental breakdown, followed by psychological traumata (24%), such as conflicts with superiors or comrades. The majority of cases had been involved in heavy fighting, but almost a quarter (23%) had never seen action.

Table 2 Principal event that triggered onset of symptoms (n = 100)

Of the 13 soldiers who had experienced mental health problems before the war, 4 had a family history of mental illness in first-degree relatives. Seven had presented with similar syndromes during the war as compared to their pre-war presentation (3 with psychogenic seizures before the war, 2 with depression, 1 with somatoform disorder and 1 with adjustment disorder).

Nine per cent of soldiers had a history of heavy drinking, mainly associated with battle exposure. Compared to present-day accounts of alcohol misuse in combatants exposed to threatening situations and atrocities, this seems to be a very low prevalence [53].

Diagnoses

The most common contemporaneous diagnoses were ‘psychopathic constitution’ (45 cases) and hysteria (46 cases). Three cases were diagnosed with depression, 2 with psychosis, 1 with ‘traumatic neurosis’, 1 with debility, 1 with intercostal neuritis (classified as organic) and one patient did not receive a diagnosis.

Assigning the presenting symptoms to one of our 10 syndrome categories, we obtained the following picture: The largest group of soldiers (43%) presented with pseudoneurological motor or sensory symptoms, corresponding to ICD-10: F44.4 (in this group, 14 patients had gait disorders, 10 had speech disorders, 17 had trembling or shaking and 8 paralysis or contractures) and F44.6 (in this group, 3 patients suffered from psychogenic deafness and 2 from sensory loss; in all 5 cases of F44.6, there were also dissociative motor symptoms). Twenty-eight per cent of patients had pseudoseizures (corresponding to ICD-10: F44.5), 20% classical psychiatric syndromes (depression, psychosis, anxiety, PD, anxiety and depression in adjustment disorders, corresponding to ICD-10: F2, F3, F43, F6), 15% other dissociative disorders (7% psychogenic confusion (coded as F44.88), 3% dissociative fugue (F44.1) and another 5% other unspecific dissociative disorders (F44.9)), and 16% somatoform disorder (corresponding to ICD-10: F45 and F48). Three per cent had a confirmed organic disorder and 1 case did not meet the criteria for any mental disorder (see Table 3). These numbers add up to over 100% because 27 patients presented with symptoms that met two diagnostic categories. Of the 15 patients presenting with dissociative symptoms (other than dissociative motor, sensory or convulsive symptoms), 5 showed a specific clinical picture of aggression, increased motor activity, re-living of traumatic war experiences, unresponsiveness to external stimuli and amnesia for the episode after regaining full consciousness.

Table 3 ICD-10 categories and their corresponding contemporaneous diagnoses and judgements on fitness for military service (not all servicemen had judgements)

The 85 patients who presented with functional disorders according to modern diagnostic criteria (with medically unexplained symptoms, comprising the ICD-10 categories F44, F45 and F48) were predominantly diagnosed with ‘psychopathic constitution’ (n = 39) or ‘hysteria’ (n = 42) by the Berlin psychiatrists.

In order to determine the impact of combat stress on the manifestation of symptoms, we divided the sample into two groups, those who had (n = 77) and those who had not seen action (n = 23). Dissociative disorders (F44.1/8/9 excluding dissociative disorders of movement and sensation F44.4/5/6) only occurred in servicemen who had been exposed to combat (this category constituted to 17% of all cases exposed to combat). This asymmetrical distribution of dissociative disorders was significant (Chi square test, P = 0.035).

Treatment and outcome of treatment

At discharge, 38% of cases were classified as in partial remission, 34% as completely recovered, 26% as unchanged and one patient died in hospital. However, outcomes were better for dissociative motor disorders (ICD-10: F44.4), where 14 out of 30 (47%) patients recovered completely, 13 were partly remitted and 3 were discharged unchanged, than for dissociative convulsions (7 out of 25 recovered completely (28%), 7 were partially recovered and 11 remained unchanged).

Eighteen patients mainly with dissociative motor disorders received treatment in form of electric shocks. These were commonly applied to the affected body part using a Faradic brush. Hypnosis and suggestion were also occasionally practised. Response to electric shock was reported as being very good, with most patients recovering after a single treatment, though no follow-up studies were reported to test the longer-term effect of treatment. Seventy-two out of the 100 soldiers were assessed regarding their fitness for military service. The largest group (n = 33) was classified as unfit for any military service. Twenty-nine soldiers were assigned to duty at home camps, 4 to garrison service and only 6 to general military service including front-line service (see Table 3).

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first study of war-related disorders in German combatants of WWI employing current diagnostic criteria. The case files of the Psychiatric Department of the Charité are unique in that they offer a detailed account of psychopathological phenomena in German servicemen with post-combat disorders. Compared with Fraenkel [6] we had the benefit of the comparison with the international literature and modern diagnostic systems and incorporated cases from four, rather than one, years of the war.

Psychopathic constitution in the discussion of the time

The increase of admissions for functional disorders during the war years, which was not matched by an increase in classical psychiatric disorders—schizophrenia, manic-depressive illness and progressive paralysis—was already documented by Bonhoeffer [2]. Whereas the pre-war admission criteria at the psychiatric unit of the Charité continued to apply during WWI [2], the proportion of male patients diagnosed with psychopathic constitution or hysteria increased dramatically from 12% in 1913 to 36% in 1915.

In our sample, only 20% of patients presented with primarily psychiatric symptoms, mainly depression and anxiety. Our data are in keeping with other studies (e.g. [45, 52]) which also found that servicemen with predominantly psychiatric presentation were relatively rare.

In our sample, the vast majority of servicemen with functional disorders (characterised by medically unexplained symptoms, comprising the ICD-10 categories of F44, F45 and F48) obtained a diagnosis of ‘psychopathic constitution’ (39 out of 85 patients, 46%) or ‘hysteria’ (42 out of 85 patients, 49%). This classification reflected the view of the Charité psychiatrists that individual predisposition, rather than the trauma of battle, was responsible for the development of symptoms and resulted in only 7 servicemen being granted compensation. This position was shared by the majority of contemporaneous psychiatrists and neurologists [44]. A notable exception was Oppenheim, who had published a monograph on traumatic neuroses in 1889 [39] and supported the view that hysteria could be triggered in anyone.

Psychogenic versus organic origin

The nature-nurture debate on the relevance of an individual’s underlying constitution was orthogonal to another contentious debate of the time, whether functional symptoms were psychogenic in origin or the result of objective pathological changes or lesions. Initially, the most prominent advocates of the organic case were Sarbo [50] and Oppenheim [40]. However, Oppenheim later modified his view and claimed that not only physical trauma but also (psychological) shock could influence physical processes (through the ‘vasomotor-secretory-trophic nervous system’, which is strikingly similar to modern concepts of neuroendocrine stress responses). Two main arguments were made against organic causation. Firstly, if functional disorders were curable through hypnosis [38] or enforcement [42], then the underlying mechanisms had to be psychological rather than physiological. Secondly, hysterical disorders were not observed in prisoners of war (POW) camps [36], suggesting that symptoms arose only when soldiers were threatened with a return to hazardous combat [36]; yet both modern [22] and contemporaneous [32] research has shown that POWs, too, developed stress disorders.

A middle way was suggested by Moerchen [36], a medical officer at a POW camp in Darmstadt. He argued that psychological trauma or shock could cause subtle organic changes and trigger abnormal biological processes to the nervous system in the same way that psychological therapies (hypnosis, suggestion) could have a positive impact on organic disorders. It took over 80 years for these views to enter mainstream psychiatry [26]. According to Moerchen, the cure or improvement of functional disorders through psychological therapies or the occasional spontaneous remission were compatible with an organic mechanism in the central or peripheral nervous system.

All functional disorders in the study were classified as ‘psychogenic’ in origin by the treating psychiatrists. This applied even to cases where functional symptoms were grafted on to objective organic pathology; for example, when a patient with a gunshot wound to the hand developed a progressive paralysis of that hand. Such grafting of functional symptoms on to organically affected limbs or organ systems was frequently observed by physicians of WWI [48]. The one Berlin case of ‘traumatic neurosis’ also falls in this category because the symptoms were explained as consequences of a severe spinal injury which may explain why Bonhoeffer, who otherwise opposed Oppenheim’s theory, authorised the use of this term for this patient.

Psychogenic seizures

An intriguing observation of our study is the high proportion of psychogenic seizures (28% of cases) among German soldiers admitted to the Charité. To our knowledge, this phenomenon has not been reported in modern accounts of mental disorders resulting from combat trauma in WWI and WWII [24, 56]. With a few exceptions mentioned below, British military doctors paid little attention to psychogenic seizures and concentrated on functional disorders concerning the heart (Disorganised Action of the Heart: DAH [9, 41, 7, 31]) and sensory-motor system (pareses, tremor, speech disorders [14]). Conversely, German psychiatrists and neurologists reported high rates of seizure disorders [10, 16, 43], and conducted studies designed to differentiate between functional seizures/hysteria and genuine epilepsy. Both Goldstein [10] and Bonhoeffer [1] thought that the psychological impact of war could not cause genuine epilepsy in an individual without a constitutional predisposition. An opposing view was expressed by Richter [43], who recognised a genuine recurrent seizure disorder (‘Kriegsepilepsie’ or ‘war epilepsy’) that could be triggered by psychological trauma even in soldiers without predisposing factors. Richter, who based his article on his research in a specialised seizure unit within a military hospital between October 1917 and May 1918, distinguished between epileptic and more common hysterical seizures by means of the psychopathological criteria established by Hoche [12], for example the duration of seizures, pupillary reactions and state of consciousness.

In Britain, White [52] recorded ‘hystero-epileptic seizures with marked clonic spasms’ in young soldiers but neither he nor Hurst [13] provided any statistical data on which to base their incidence. Dudley Carmalt Jones [17], who was in charge of a specialist treatment centre for shell shock, opened in January 1917 at No. 4 Stationary Hospital, conducted a detailed clinical study of functional disorders of the war. He assessed 1300 patients admitted under the label ‘N.Y.D., N.’ (Not yet diagnosed, nervous) a few days after the onset of symptoms but only came across 3 cases of ‘violent hysterical fits’. Yealland [55], based at the National Hospital for Nervous Diseases, claimed that clonic-like fits were ‘the only type of hysterical seizure that occurred in soldiers in the recent war’ and that the classical arc de cercle of hysteria developed only in women and was exceedingly rare. Our records paint a different picture, with a wide range of pseudoseizures, including arc de cercle, in the soldier patients. It is currently not known whether this reflected a general difference in neurological manifestations of psychological trauma between Germany and Britain. An alternative explanation would be that certain types of manifestation were overrepresented in a secondary referral centre like the Charité. We intend to analyse records from other German and British hospitals to clarify this issue.

The changing face of post-traumatic syndromes

In the Berlin case records, we did not find evidence of post-traumatic syndromes as defined by the current diagnostic manuals. However, a small group of soldiers relived their traumatic combat experiences in dream-like dissociative states, mainly when waking up from a nightmare revolving around their war deployment. They showed aggressive outbursts with increased motor activity, re-staging of battle scenes (e.g. lying on the floor and shooting with an imaginary gun), unresponsiveness to external stimuli and amnesia for this episode afterwards. All received the contemporaneous diagnosis of ‘psychopathic constitution’ or ‘hysteria’. Under the heading of ‘Schreckpsychosen’, similar symptoms were described by Karl Kleist, then professor of psychiatry in Rostock, in 1918 [27]. Following a shock or fright, Kleist’s cases of ‘anxious delirium’ (‘aengstliche Delirien’)—the most common form of ‘Schreckpsychosen’—, were in a dream-like state of consciousness, disoriented to time and place and reliving combat scenes under the influence of hallucinations. Afterwards typically hysterical symptoms became dominant again and there was amnesia for the episode. Kleist, who worked in a military hospital with a specialised psychiatric/neurological unit near the front line, also described dreams about horrifying war experiences, emotional hyperreagibility and hyperarousal as well as emotional blunting as effects of acute war trauma. These symptoms at least partly correspond to modern post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Only few case records from Berlin (in addition to the 5 cases mentioned above) make a note of combat-related dreams or nightmares. More frequently, soldiers suffered from emotional blunting, detachment from other people, anhedonia and difficulties concentrating, but these symptoms lost out against the very dominant pseudoneurological and other functional symptoms. Very few soldiers had outbursts of anxiety or aggression, hypervigilance and hyperarousal. The relative rarity of this type of stress reaction and its differences from present-day PTSD support Jones’s theory that similar traumatic triggers can have different phenomenological consequences in different cultural settings [18].

Kleist’s ‘Schreckpsychosen’

We observed that dissociative disorders (F44.1/8/9 excluding dissociative disorders of movement and sensation F44.4/5/6) only occurred in servicemen who had been exposed to combat. These cases included the acute stress reaction described above which seems to overlap with Kleist’s ‘Schreckpsychosen’. According to Kleist, ‘Schreckpsychosen’ were the most common mental disorders on the theatre of war. Unlike war neuroses which commonly developed far away from the front line and persisted for a long time, they constituted acute and short-lived reactions to combat stress and were therefore rarely seen in military hospitals at home. Although most of our soldiers had long left the combat zone (most of them had been treated in several military hospitals before being referred to the Charité), some of them showed clinical pictures resembling Kleist’s Schreckpsychosen. All soldiers retrospectively diagnosed with dissociative symptoms (F44.1/8/9 excluding dissociative disorders of movement and sensation F44.4/5/6) and one case of ‘acute stress reaction’ (F43.0) strikingly resembled Kleist’s ‘Schreckpsychosen’. In our sample, however, we could not identify any cases with catatonic symptoms, which were also described by Kleist.

The term ‘Schreckpsychose’ is never mentioned in the Berlin case records. Bonhoeffer did not approve of this concept [27]. Whereas Kleist believed that the shock alone was sufficient to cause symptoms as described above, Bonhoeffer saw the wish to be ill (and to escape combat) as the main factor contributing to the development and consolidation of symptoms.

Trigger events

In most of our cases, specific events were identified that had triggered the soldier’s mental breakdown (see Table 2). The documented triggers were both physical (for example, injuries obtained during front-line service or burial) and/or psychological (for example, conflict with superior). The influence of perceived intimidation or bullying by a superior was noted at the time [17]. However, almost a quarter of the patients in our sample had not seen action and presumably never been exposed to any serious hazard. This phenomenon is of relevance to the ongoing debate whether genuine stress reactions require the experience of actual physical threat or whether its mere anticipation is sufficient. Whereas the PTSD definition requires exposure to a life-threatening event (criterion A, [54]), an anticipated or feared traumatic experience was sufficient to trigger a functional disorder. This phenomenon had also been observed in British servicemen awarded a war pension for a psychological disorder [20].

In most cases, symptoms started immediately after the trigger event. There were, however, a few cases of apparent latency between the perceived trigger and onset of symptoms of up to 5 months, which supports observations made by Schneider [46] and White [52]. This phenomenon was to be replicated in the 1960 s with ‘delayed stress syndrome’ or ‘post-Vietnam syndrome’ and ultimately led to the formal recognition of PTSD in the DSM-III [11]. Kurt Schneider explained the delayed symptom onset in terms of medical priorities. Close to the battlefield, physicians had to concentrate on life-threatening physical injuries while the treatment of neurotic symptoms had a lower priority so that they could develop almost undetected in the aftermath of combat. Schneider also claimed that hysterical symptoms were often masked by the application of certain medications such as hyoscine (scopolamine, an anticholinergic drug). Furthermore, he emphasised that compensation neurosis typically developed later, away from the battlefield, making it difficult for the individual to give up his symptoms. Schneider argued that neurotic soldiers should not be sent home away from their combat units because this would reduce their chances of returning to active duty. He suggested treating them with psychotherapy immediately after their traumatic experience. Schneider observed that soldiers who were not sent to distant treatment units often showed a spontaneous recovery from functional disorders. Ernst Jolowicz [15] who saw cases of functional disorders in treatment units close to the front line confirmed Schneider’s observation that neurotic symptoms consolidated in servicemen who had been evacuated from their combat unit. Because neurotic disorders appeared to have been relatively infrequent during the first few months of the war, a period of mobile warfare, he claimed that neurotic symptoms were characteristic of static trench warfare, a hypothesis that has been rephrased by modern psychiatrists [8] and historians [28, 56]. However, both our data and observations on the contagious nature of conversion disorders made at the time [44] show that exposure to trench warfare was not a requirement for the development of pseudoneurological syndromes. Jolowicz [15] also pleaded for early intervention near the battlefield to avoid pension claims, loss of manpower in combat units and unnecessary deterioration of health in the individual. At about the same time, Germany adopted the ‘forward psychiatry’ established by the British and French slightly earlier [23], by setting up dedicated treatment units close to the war zone [30].

Treatment strategies and differences in outcome

The prognosis of psychogenic seizure disorders was much worse than that of conversion disorders with sensory-motor symptoms. Commentators of the time noted that hysterical seizures and hysterical tremor were difficult to treat or treatment resistant [17, 34] whereas other functional disorders, such as hysterical deafness [3, 17], speech disorders and pareses [17], had a better prognosis even after long illness duration [5, 48].

The documented treatments at the Charité included eight cases of electrical stimulation of the affected body parts (with faradic brush), in two cases combined with suggestive methods, but none of the specific treatments proposed by Nonne [38] or Kaufmann [42]. A speciality of the Charité psychiatrists seems to have been therapeutic admission to the locked ‘ward for the severely ill non-quiet cases’, which was reported to lead to immediate cure of functional symptoms in some cases. This type of behavioural intervention seems to have been rare elsewhere in the German system.

Fitness for duty

The low rate of return to full military duty reflected a general belief among German military psychiatrists that individuals with psychopathic constitution were not suitable for front-line service [2, 6, 48, 44]. This was in contrast to the later practice in WWII, where the remit of psychiatric practice was to make traumatised soldiers fit for return to active duty [28].

Although the British came to similar conclusions [17, 52], their practice of returning soldiers with functional disorders to active duty seems to have been different. Jones [17] who carried out a study on 1,300 admissions to a specialist shell shock unit in a stationary hospital doubted that ‘anyone who has once developed it (shell shock) will ever be fit for front-line soldiering within the time limit of any war’. Despite this view, about 40% of his patients without complicating organic pathology were reportedly returned to active service, about 40% to light duty or prolonged rest previous to duty and about 20% to base hospitals. He justified this course of action with the high demand for men in the combatant units: ‘it has been the duty of medical officers to risk error rather in the direction of sending up possibly unfit men than in that of losing possibly fit men to their units’. With a larger standing army, the German command might have been more tolerant of medical discharge than the British. However, retrospective study of Carmalt Jones’s data has shown that his published return to duty rate was exaggerated and the actual proportion of admissions that immediately returned to active duty was only 17% [21]. Yet, because of the pervasive belief in the importance of the ‘psychopathic constitution’, unfitness for duty did by no means lead to a successful compensation claim.

Limitations

The Charité was a specialised referral centre. It is likely that the composition of its patients differed from those in the acute war hospitals. It is difficult to obtain evidence from primary sources about differences in the patient samples between the university psychiatric departments and war hospitals close to the front line because of the scarce documentation of cases from the latter. However, based on the information from the writings of contemporaneous psychiatrists on their experience in the combat zone hospitals it is likely that they dealt with the less chronic cases, whereas the Berlin cases often came to admission after a protracted course of several months up to over a year. A quantitative diagnostic study similar to ours on records from war hospitals close to the front would certainly be desirable.

Conclusion

A lack of knowledge regarding the aetiology and pathogenesis of functional disorders during WWI inspired an active debate and led to causal explanatory models that were shaped by culture-dependent beliefs, for example regarding a psychopathic constitution. In Germany, medical opinion shifted from a bio-medical to a more psychological explanatory framework early in the war. Some German psychiatrists even suggested that psychological trauma could cause changes in the brain leading to overt physical symptoms (Richter, 1919). It remains an open question whether the Berlin files were representative of the contemporaneous German attitude towards war-related functional disorders. To answer this question, comparable patient files from other military hospitals and treatment units would have to be analysed. However, the high proportion of hysterical seizures seen in our sample seems to be an all-German phenomenon according to the publications of the time. Intriguingly, psychogenic seizures seem to have been much rarer in Britain and did not feature prominently in any previous or subsequent war [18]. It has been recognised that war or other environmental stressors can lead to a wide range of neurological, somatic [24] or psychiatric syndromes [33, 49]. Clinical phenomenology is likely to have been a product of the type of trauma, the medical models available at the time and other cultural factors, but their exact interplay is still a matter for research [24].

Awareness of the time-changing nature of psychological manifestations of trauma may help today’s clinicians adjust their explanatory models and inform treatment approaches. The debate on the contribution of predisposing factors is also important for the medical approach to stress disorders and the attitude of society as a whole, including the evaluation of compensation claims. The flexibility of symptoms that can be triggered by largely similar patterns of psychological trauma should also be incorporated in psychobiological models of stress-induced disorders [51]. Today’s clinicians should be aware that the manifestation of stress-related disorders is influenced by the cultural and historical context. History has impressively shown that the face of post-traumatic reactions changes over time. Clinicians therefore have to be open to the possibility of new traumatically triggered syndromes appearing over time.