Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Giving to poverty relief charities: the impact of beliefs and misperceptions toward income redistribution in a real donation experiment

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Social Choice and Welfare Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Many Americans hold erroneous beliefs regarding the level of inequality in the United States and the efforts by the federal government to alleviate poverty. In general, they overestimate the extent of poverty relief undertaken by government. Given that poverty relief programs are a public good and likely underprovided, overestimation of the level of income redistribution is likely to exacerbate this under-provision by reducing giving to private charities. This paper considers if this misperception affects giving to poverty-relief charities. We report a real-donation experiment investigating links between contributions to poverty-relief charities and perceptions of federal transfers to low income households. We also ask participants to self-identify political affiliation, religiosity, race, and gender. We find that donations to our poverty relief charities are inversely related to the perceived transfers made to the poorest quintile. Donations are approximately $0.20 less for each $1000 of perceived transfers. Interestingly, we find little correlation between giving and political beliefs.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Kuklinski et al. also find that a similar number overstate the average annual welfare payment. A similar misperception applies to spending on foreign aid. See http://www.worldpublicopinion.org/pipa/pdf/nov10/ForeignAid_Nov10_quaire.pdf. Accessed 27 May 2015 and http://voices.washingtonpost.com/ezra-klein/2010/12/american_misperceptions_of_for.html. Accessed 27 May 2015.

  2. See also the literature on information avoidance (Golman et al. 2017).

  3. Participants are told market income includes labor income, business income, and capital gains.

  4. We thank one of the referees for suggesting a counterbalanced treatment, as well as the additional survey question included in Treatment 2.

  5. One could ask if students have an accurate understanding of the income distribution. On the one hand, students may lack labor market experience; on the other hand, they may have studied income inequality, and/or selected into higher education because they understand the education premium. Existing evidence suggests that in many contexts, student populations do not behave significantly different from other populations. See for example Camerer (2015) and Davis (2016).

  6. Data in Table 1 separated by treatments are reported in Appendix Table 3. For the characteristics listed in Table 1, the only statistically significant difference between treatments is the proportion of students with a grade point average of 3.0. Participants in treatment 2 were more likely to have a GPA of 3.0 or higher (t test of different proportions: \(t = 2.52,\hbox { pr} > {\vert }t{\vert } = 0.01\)). The two treatments have a similar gender and racial profiles, and have similar responses to many survey questions.

  7. Tobit models, reported in Appendix Table 4, yield qualitatively similar results, with the following notes. Significance levels are similar if we allow for censoring at the upper bound for students who would have given more than $20 if that action were feasible. Censoring at the lower bound is appropriate if some participants would have taken money from charity if that action were allowed. The extant evidence from real-donation experiments suggests that participants do not take from charity (Luccasen and Grossman 2017, Korenok et al. 2014), so censoring only at the upper bound appears most appropriate.

  8. We also ran regressions that included separate variables for Democrats and Republican/Libertarian. We could not reject the hypothesis that the coefficients were equal. Results are available upon request.

  9. Appendix Table 5 reports regression results for each treatment separately.

  10. Li et al. (2011) note that studies consistently find women to be more generous in real donation experiments.

  11. Republicans and the small number of Libertarians were combined because these groups were believed to have similar opinions on the role of government in reducing redistributing income. Excluding Libertarians from Republicans does not yield qualitatively different results.

  12. Respectively, $6.31 and $7.25 (\(t=-0.71\), p = 0.48).

  13. Our results are similar to Wilhelm (2010), who reports no differences in giving across Christian denominations.

  14. For example, in 2013, 51% of Medicaid recipients were children (see: http://www.politifact.com/punditfact/statements/2015/jan/28/terry-jeffrey/are-there-more-welfare-recipients-us-full-time-wor/).

  15. See https://www.whitehouse.gov/sites/default/files/omb/financial/memos/improving-data-quality-for-usaspending-gov.pdf, for more information.

References

  • Alesina A, La Ferrara E (2005) Preferences for redistribution in the land of opportunities. J Public Econ 89:897–931

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andreoni J (1988) Privately provided public goods in a large economy: the limits of altruism. J Public Econ 35:57–73

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andreoni J (1989) Giving with impure altruism: applications to charity and Ricardian equivalence. J Polit Econ 97:1447–1458

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andreoni J (1990) Impure altruism and donations to public goods: a theory of warm-glow giving. Econ J 100:464–477

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andreoni J (1993) An experimental test of the public-goods crowding-out hypothesis. Am Econ Rev 83(5):1317–1327

    Google Scholar 

  • Benabou R, Tirole J (2006) Incentives and prosocial behavior. Am Econ Rev 96(5):1652–1678

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks AC (2006) Who really cares: the surprising truth about compassionate conservatism. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Camerer C (2015) The promise and success of lab-field generalizability in experimental economics: a reply to Levitt and List”. In: Schotter A (ed) Frechette G. Handbook of experimental economic methodology. Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Chan K, Godby R, Mestelman S, Muller RA (2002) Crowding-out voluntary contributions to public goods. J Econ Behav Organ 48:305–317

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coffman L, Featherstone CR, Kessler JB (2015) A model of information nudges. Working Paper

  • Congressional Budget Office (2012) The distribution of household income and federal taxes, 2008 and 2009

  • Cruces G, Perez-Truglia R, Tetaz M (2013) Biased perceptions of income distribution and preferences for redistribution: evidence from a survey experiment. J Public Econ 98:100–112

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis D (2016) Experimental methods for the general economist: five lessons from the lab. South Econ J 82(4):1046–1058

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eckel CC, Grossman PJ (1996) Altruism in anonymous dictator games. Game Econ Behav 16:181–191

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eckel CC, Grossman PJ, Johnston MR (2005) An experimental test of the crowding out hypothesis. J Public Econ 89:1543–1560

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eplay N, Gilovich T (2016) The mechanics of motivated reasoning. J Econ Perspect 30(3):133–140

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fallucchi F, Luccasen RA, Turocy TL (2017) Behavioural types in public good games: a re-analysis by hierarchical clustering. Working Paper

  • Flynn DJ, Nyhan B, Reifler J (2016) The nature and origins of misperceptions: Understanding false and unsupported beliefs about politics. Working paper

  • Golman R, Hagmann D, Loewenstein G (2017) Information avoidance. J Econ Lit 55(1):96–135

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gronberg T, Luccasen RA, Turocy TL, Van Huyck JB (2012) Are tax-financed contributions to a public good completely crowded-out? Experimental evidence. J Public Econ 96:596–603

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Korenok O, Millner E, Razzolini L (2014) Taking or giving? The price of giving: how much do individuals dislike taking? Working paper

  • Khanna J, Posnett J, Sandler T (1995) Charity donations in the UK: new evidence based on panel data. J Public Econ 56:257–272

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Khanna J, Sandler T (2000) The crowding-in effect of UK government grants. Eur Econ Rev 44:1543–1556

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kranton RE, Sanders SG (2017) Groupy versus non-groupy social preferences: personality, region, and political party. Am Econ Rev 107(5):65–69

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuklinski JH, Quirk PJ, Jerit J, Schwieder D, Rich RF (2000) Misinformation and the currency of democratic citizenship. J Polit 62:790–816

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li SX, Eckel CC, Grossman PJ, Brown TL (2011) Giving to government: voluntary taxation in the lab. J Public Econ 95:1190–1201

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luccasen RA, Grossman PJ (2017) Warm-glow giving: earned money and the option to take. Econ Inq 55(2):996–1006

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norton MI, Ariely D (2011) Building a better America–one wealth quintile at a time. Perspect Psychol Sci 6:9–12

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Payne A (2001) Measuring the effect of federal research funding on private donations at research universities: is federal research funding more than a substitute for private donations? Int Tax Public Finance 8:731–751

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piketty T (1995) Social mobility and redistributive politics. Q J Econ 110(3):551–584

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rondeau D, List J (2008) Matching and challenge gifts to charity: evidence from laboratory and natural field experiments. Exp Econ 11(3):253–267

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilhelm M (2010) Giving to organizations that help people in need: Differences across denominational identities. J Sci Stud Relig 49:389–412

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to R. Andrew Luccasen III.

Additional information

We wish to thank Mississippi University for Women for financial support of this project.

Appendices

Appendix A: Instructions and materials

1.1 A.1: Treatment 1 (for treatment 2, parts 1 and 2 are presented in reverse order)

General instructions read by the experimenter:

You are about to participate in an experiment in the economics of decision-making. Please read all instructions carefully, as the amount of money you earn will depend on how well you understand them. All money you earn will be paid to you privately in cash at the end of the session. If you have a question at any time, please feel free to ask the experimenter. We ask that you not talk with the other participants during the experiment.

This experimental session will consist of three parts. Each will be explained at the appropriate time.

One of the persons in this room has been chosen to be the monitor for today’s experiment. The monitor will be paid $25. The monitor will be in charge of various tasks to be explained below. In addition the monitor will verify that all instructions have been followed as they are outlined.

You have each been given a piece of paper with your unique five-digit code number. Your code number will be the same for all parts of this session. Keep the paper with the code number. You will use this number to collect your earnings at the end of this session.

figure a
figure b
figure c
figure d
figure e
figure f
figure g

Appendix B: Additional tables

Tables 3, 4 and 5.

Table 3 Summary statistics by treatment
Table 4 Tobit models
Table 5 Regression models for individual treatments

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Luccasen, R.A., Thomas, M.K. & Grossman, P.J. Giving to poverty relief charities: the impact of beliefs and misperceptions toward income redistribution in a real donation experiment. Soc Choice Welf 49, 387–409 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00355-017-1070-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00355-017-1070-8

Navigation