Introduction

Many plants and animals breed seasonally during periods that are likely optimal for fertilisation success and the early development of offspring. In addition to reproductive seasonality, many species exhibit marked reproductive synchrony, i.e. reproduction occurring in tighter temporal clusters than would be expected by seasonality alone (Ims 1990a). Some examples include species of mast-seeding bamboos, which will grow during a species-specific period of between 3 and 120 years, before reproducing synchronously (Janzen 1976), and the Pacific Ridley turtle, which lays eggs during synchronous mass nestings called “arribadas” involving tens of thousands of individuals (Hughes and Richard 1974).

In addition to high reproductive synchrony within species, spawning events involving multiple species occur in fishes (Whaylen et al. 2004; Heyman and Kjerfve 2008), polychaetes (Hardege 1999), echinoderms (Himmelman et al. 2008) and frogs (Wilczynski et al. 1993). Multi-species synchronous spawning is also a feature of most species-rich coral assemblages (Babcock et al. 1986; Guest et al. 2005; Baird et al. 2009a). Intra-specific synchrony is likely highly adaptive for sessile, broadcast spawning marine invertebrates as a way to maximise fertilisation success (Ims 1990a). The adaptive significance of numerous species spawning at the same time is less clear. While several hypotheses have been proposed (e.g. predator satiation), the most parsimonious explanation is that species have responded similarly, but independently, to environmental signalling cues (e.g. sea surface temperatures (SSTs), lunar cycles, diel cycles) (Oliver et al. 1988).

One of the early predictions relating to spawning in corals was that multi-species spawning synchrony was correlated with latitude, with a breakdown in spawning synchrony near the equator, due to a lack of suitable cues related to the annual amplitude of environmental variables or lack of selective pressures to spawn at an optimal time of the year (Oliver et al. 1988; Olive 1995). While later work revealed that multi-species spawning synchrony was a feature of most diverse coral assemblages, including those on equatorial reefs (Guest et al. 2005), the original hypothesis that synchrony is correlated with latitude was never quantitatively tested on a global scale (but see Baird et al. 2009a). Much of the confusion with respect to geographical variation in coral spawning synchrony exists because synchrony is rarely quantified (Baird and Guest 2009). Quantitative estimates of synchrony are available and have been widely used to quantify flowering synchrony in plant assemblages (Post 2003; Elzinga et al. 2007; Freitas and Bolmgren 2008).

In this study, we use data from Acropora assemblages on 90 Indo-Pacific sites between the latitudes of 31.5°S and 27.2°N over a 20-year period and the Marquis index (Marquis 1988) to examine synchrony at the monthly scale within multi-species assemblages, within six individual species, and explore patterns in synchrony across multiple years at specific locations.

Materials and methods

The dataset

To explore patterns of coral spawning synchrony, we compiled a dataset of coral reproductive condition for Acropora (Fig. 1) from both published (i.e. Guest et al. 2005; Baird et al. 2009b, 2010, 2011, 2015; Rosser and Baird 2009; Hanafy et al. 2010; Raj and Edward 2010) and original data (see Online Resource 1). Coral reproductive condition was defined following Baird et al. (2002). To enable a rigorous comparison of synchrony, we only included surveys that, for each site, randomly sampled the entire Acropora assemblage, sampled at least 30 colonies in each month and we did not include sites with less than 50% cumulative spawning predicted in any one year, because it was assumed that the full spawning season had not been adequately captured. Synchrony was also examined in six species that were surveyed on at least 25 sites (n = 25 was chosen as a minimum for statistical robustness of correlation analyses (David 1938)), with at least 20 colonies sampled each month, and with greater than 50% cumulative spawning.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Location of sites used in this study. Nearby sites may not be distinguishable due to overlap

Species were identified in the field or from field images following Veron (2000). Due to current uncertainties in coral taxonomy, in particular in the genus Acropora (Cowman et al. 2020), the main analyses were run at the assemblage level, for which correct identifications are not necessary. For the species analysis, we chose six species that were common, widespread and relatively easy to identify. Nonetheless, we accept that these are taxa in which cryptic species are possible (Ladner and Palumbi 2012; Richards et al. 2016), suggesting these analyses might need to be revisited in the future. Uncertainties in the identifications are indicated by the use of a series of open nomenclature qualifiers (Bengtson 1988; Sigovini et al. 2016) that allow the assignment of a specimen to a nominal species with varying degrees of certainty. Specimens that closely resemble the type of a nominal species are given the qualifier cf. (e.g. Acropora cf. nasuta). Specimens that have morphological affinities to a nominal species but appear distinct are given the qualifier aff. (e.g. Acropora aff. pulchra): these specimens are either geographical variants of species with high morphological plasticity or undescribed species. Species that could not be matched with the type material of any nominal species were labelled as sp. with the image number (e.g. Acropora sp_81-4052). These specimens are most probably undescribed species. No individuals identified using open nomenclature qualifiers were used for the spatial analyses of reproductive synchrony within common Acropora species.

Predicting the month of spawning

In order to calculate the Marquis index, it was necessary to convert the coral reproductive condition data into the number of colonies spawning in each month of the year as follows. Mature colonies observed up to 6 days after the full moon were predicted to spawn in the same lunar month as the sampling date. In the few cases where mature colonies were observed 7 days or more after the full moon, they were predicted to spawn during the subsequent lunar month. The cut-off date was set at six nights after the full moon given that date is the peak night of spawning in Indo-Pacific Acropora assemblages (see Online Resource 2 for more details). Immature colonies were assumed to spawn a month later than mature colonies. We also considered that mature and immature colonies would not spawn again for up to two months after their predicted month of spawning. Indeed, gametogenic cycles require 6 to 14 months to complete (Baird et al. 2009a) and eggs that would be sufficiently mature to be released would be visible at least two months in advance. Similarly, we considered that empty colonies would not spawn for at least 60 days.

Quantifying reproductive synchrony: the Marquis index

Reproductive synchrony can be defined at several temporal levels (e.g. month, night or time of spawning) depending on the hypothesis being tested. Here, we define synchrony as the proportion of corals within a population or assemblage that are inferred to spawn in the same month based on the presence of mature (pigmented) or immature (white) gametes. We used the following index originally developed for flowering synchrony by Marquis (Marquis 1988) to quantify coral reproductive synchrony.

$$S_{M} = \frac{{m_{1} }}{{\sum\limits_{{{\text{t}} = 1}}^{{\text{t = n}}} {{\text{m}}_{{\text{t}}} } }} \cdot p_{1} + \frac{{m_{2} }}{{\sum\limits_{{{\text{t}} = 1}}^{{\text{t = n}}} {{\text{m}}_{{\text{t}}} } }} \cdot p_{2} + \frac{{m_{3} }}{{\sum\limits_{{{\text{t}} = 1}}^{{\text{t = n}}} {{\text{m}}_{{\text{t}}} } }} \cdot p_{3} + \ldots + \frac{{m_{{\text{n}}} }}{{\sum\limits_{{{\text{t}} = 1}}^{{\text{t = n}}} {{\text{m}}_{{\text{t}}} } }} \cdot p_{{\text{n}}}$$
(1)

where t is the month of survey, n in the number of months surveyed, mt is the number of coral colonies predicted to spawn in month t, pt is the proportion of colonies examined in month t that are predicted to spawn and \(\sum\limits_{{{\text{t}} = 1}}^{{\text{t = n}}} {{\text{m}}_{{\text{t}}} }\)is the total number of coral colonies predicted to spawn during the period studied. The Marquis index of synchrony can take values from 0 (spawning spread evenly across the year) to 1 (spawning involving the entire population during each month of spawning, whether over a month or more). Marquis’ synchrony index was calculated at the assemblage level SM (A) to determine the synchrony among species and at the species level SM (S) to determine the intra-species synchrony and compare synchrony among species. Sites with multiple years of reproductive surveys were analysed per year, and yearly Marquis indices were averaged to produce one value per site unless mentioned otherwise. In regions where two spawning seasons exist, such as in Western Australia, Papua New Guinea, Indonesia and Singapore, the Marquis index was calculated based on reproductive data from the entire year, to ensure a synchrony index based on the entire Acropora assemblage and to avoid overestimating synchrony when different individuals are involved in each spawning season.

Statistical analyses

Pearson’s correlation tests were conducted to examine the relationships between latitude and reproductive synchrony at both the assemblage level and at the species level. A one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to test for any difference in reproductive synchrony among Acropora species. Data were log-transformed where necessary to meet normality assumptions. Normality of residuals was verified visually or with a Shapiro–Wilk normality test. All statistical analyses were conducted in R (R Core Team 2019), with the R packages car (Fox and Weisberg 2019), dplyr (Wickam et al. 2019), ggplot2 (Wickham 2016), rcompanion (Mangiafico 2019), rnaturalearth (South 2017) and sf (Pebesma 2018).

Results

Reproductive synchrony of the Acropora assemblages was not correlated with latitude (Pearson’s correlation, r = 0.02, t(75) = 0.22, p = 0.83; Fig. 2), even when examined per year (Online Resource 3). High synchrony was found at both high latitudes (e.g. SM(A) = 0.89 at Ndigoro Reef in New Caledonia, 22.11°S) and low latitudes (e.g. SM(A) = 0.75 at Ahus 8 Reef, in Papua New Guinea, 1.91°S). Similarly, low synchrony was found at both latitude ranges (e.g. SM(A) = 0.28 at North Molle Reef, Whitsundays, Australia, 20°S, and SM(A) = 0.31 at Pulau Weh, Indonesia, 5.87°N).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Reproductive synchrony in 77 Acropora assemblages in the Indo-Pacific as a function of latitude. Pearson’s correlation, r = 0.025, p = 0.827

Reproductive synchrony was not correlated with latitude in any of the six species examined (Fig. 3, Table 1). Both high and low reproductive synchrony was found at all latitudes, and within a given latitudinal range, there was high variability in reproductive synchrony (Fig. 3). For example, reproductive synchrony ranged between 0.36 and 0.85 in A. digitifera at 20°S (± 0.5°) and between 0.33 and 0.69 in A. hyacinthus at 20°S (± 0.5°). Average synchrony was similar across species (one-way ANOVA, F(5,197) = 1.55, p = 0.18, Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Left: Reproductive synchrony in six Acropora species. Pearson’s correlations, p > 0.05 for all species. Right: Boxplot of reproductive across all latitudes, showing the median and distribution range of reproductive synchrony in the six Acropora species examined

Table 1 Summary of Pearson correlation tests between reproductive synchrony within Acropora assemblages/species and absolute latitude

Reproductive synchrony was also variable on a temporal scale (Fig. 4). For example, at Magnetic Island (Australia), synchrony ranged from 0.37 (2005) to 0.50 (2017), at Orpheus Island (Australia), reproductive synchrony ranged from 0.24 (2012) to 0.70 (2016), and at Raffles Lighthouse (Singapore), reproductive synchrony ranged from 0.42 (2002 and 2011) to 0.68 (2009). Nonetheless, temporal variability in reproductive synchrony (can be quantified here by the standard deviation from the mean Marquis index at each location) did not follow any latitudinal trend (Pearson’s correlation, r = 0.34, t(7) = 0.95, p = 0.37).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Annual variability in spawning synchrony. From left to right: Magnetic Island, Australia, 19.1°S, n = 6; Orpheus Island, Australia, 18.6°S, n = 5; Pelorus Island, Australia, 18.6°S, n = 4; Lizard Island, Australia, 14.7°S, n = 5; Raffles Lighthouse, Singapore, 1.2°N, n = 5; Mainland Patch Reef, India, 8.7°N, n = 5; Kariyachalli Island, India, 8.9°N, n = 5; Sesoko, Japan, 26.6°N, n = 3; Oku, Japan, 26.9°N, n = 4

Discussion

No relationship was found between latitude and Acropora coral reproductive synchrony at the lunar month level. Instead, reproductive synchrony was highly variable on both spatial and temporal scales. These results support the only quantitative study of reproductive synchrony to date (i.e. Baird et al. 2009a), which found no consistent pattern between synchrony and latitude.

Our definition of reproductive synchrony is based on the proportion of mature colonies within each month of the year. We here define synchrony at a monthly scale given reproductive sampling efforts are currently mostly conducted on a monthly (lunar calendar) basis and because the resolution is sufficient to examine latitudinal patterns. Once reproductive patterns are better understood at the monthly level, similar indices for spawning synchrony, i.e. the proportion of individuals that spawn on any night, could also be determined. This will require extensive data on in situ spawning times (and absence of spawning) over an entire reproductive period but will allow for a deeper understanding of reproductive synchrony at a resolution that will provide insights into reproductive success or isolation among genotypes within a given species.

Recent work suggests a breakdown in spawning synchrony occurred over the last decades (Shlesinger and Loya 2019), with potential implications for fertilisation success. However, among other potential issues (see Guest et al. 2020 for more detail), no quantitative estimate of reproductive synchrony was used. The potential effects of climate change on coral reproduction are worrying (Mendes and Woodley 2002; Levitan et al. 2014; Hagedorn et al. 2016), but whether or not reproductive synchrony is affected will require the use of comparable quantitative data, such as is proposed here.

The lack of correlation between reproductive synchrony and latitude suggests that variation in synchrony is also not correlated with factors that vary predictably with latitude, such as range and amplitudes of SSTs and day length, which are marked at high latitudes and decrease towards the equator. This supports previous observations of marked reproductive synchrony in equatorial regions, where environmental variables show less variability throughout the year (Guest et al. 2005). We know that the range of change in temperature is a strong predictor in the peak month of spawning (Keith et al. 2016) but the driving forces responsible for variation in synchrony will most probably be challenging to identify, and might involve a combination of environmental constraints and adaptive strategies (Ims 1990a; Koenig et al. 2003). Similar challenges have been identified in ecological studies of large-scale masting synchrony in long-lived plant species (e.g. Koenig et al. 2003; Kelly et al. 2013; Pearse et al. 2014). For example, while the benefits of synchronising mast seeding in plant assemblages include predator satiation (Janzen 1971; Ims 1990b) and increased pollination efficiency in wind-pollinated species (Smith et al. 1990; Moreira et al. 2014), the proximate drivers remain elusive and challenging to identify (Kelly et al. 2013; Pearse et al. 2014).

Temporal variability in reproductive synchrony at any one site was considerable, and that variability was not linked with latitude. Nonetheless, that variability did not hide any latitudinal patterns in reproductive synchrony, which would have been visible when reproductive synchrony was examined in single years. The month(s) of spawning of different coral species in each region is generally reliable from one year to the next, but the exact dates of spawning vary as they are aligned with the lunar cycle. As such, in some years, instead of the peak spawning occurring within a single month, spawning can be split over two consecutive months (i.e. split spawning, Foster et al. 2018), thus decreasing spawning synchrony for that year. Some yearly variability in reproductive synchrony can be explained by the presence of a split spawning event, but other ecological factors are likely to be involved as well. For example, decreases in spawning synchrony may also occur in response to stress, such as following coral bleaching (Baird and Marshall 2002), or cyclones (Baird et al. 2018) disrupting timing cues and reducing reproductive output sometimes for years. Temporal variability in reproductive synchrony at each reef is likely further complicating the task of identifying the driving forces responsible for variation in spatial synchrony across the Indo-Pacific region. Further work is needed to identify temporal variability in other reefs and to determine how to incorporate annual variability in spatial analyses of reproductive synchrony.

Considering the importance of reproductive success to reef recovery and persistence, it is critical to examine spatial and temporal patterns in reproductive synchrony using quantitative approaches. In particular, long-term reproductive synchrony surveys are required to understand the possible long-term effects of climate change on spawning synchrony.