Introduction

In many species, individuals exhibit relatively consistent differences in behavior, which has been termed “animal personality” (Sih et al. 2004; Réale et al. 2007; Bell et al. 2009; Moiron et al. 2020). Personality traits have important roles in evolutionary and ecological processes, because they can correlate with fitness and can impact interactions within and among species (Réale et al. 2007; Smith and Blumstein 2008; Bolnick et al. 2011; Wolf and Weissing 2012). Understanding what causes and maintains variation in personality among individuals is therefore of considerable interest, particularly as environments are changing rapidly as a result of human influence.

Environments experienced during development can have significant and long-lasting impacts on phenotype and fitness (West-Eberhard 2003) and may be one way in which differences in personality emerge. Animal personality is quantified by repeatability, which is the ratio of between-individual variance (VID) to phenotypic variance (VID + within-individual variance [VR]). Previous studies have found impacts of developmental environments on repeatability in behavior. For example, agile frog tadpoles, Rana dalmatina, only had significant repeatability in risk-taking behavior when reared in an environment containing predator scent (Urszan et al. 2015). Similarly, DiRienzo et al. (2015) found that early exposure to pathogens resulted in lower repeatability in some boldness measures in field crickets, Gryllus integer. Changes in repeatability can be due to shifts in between- or within-individual variation, increasing when between-individual variance increases and decreasing when within-individual variance increases. For example, crickets, G. bimaculatus, reared at a high temperature had increased both within- and between-individual variation (Niemelä et al. 2019. Together, these studies demonstrate a clear role for early environments in affecting personality. However, it is largely unknown whether this is driven by changes in within- or between-individual variation, despite increasing interest in quantifying these two components of repeatability.

Like most phenotypic traits, behaviors are not expressed in isolation but are correlated with a range of traits, including other behaviors (Sih et al. 2004). Developmental environment effects may therefore extend to, or be constrained by, the stability of these correlations. Suites of behaviors correlated across time and contexts are termed “behavioral syndromes” (Sih et al. 2004) and are found in many species, including spiders (Johnson and Sih 2007), fish (Bell and Sih 2007; Norton et al. 2011), lizards (Michelangeli et al. 2019), birds (Carere et al. 2005; Dingemanse et al. 2012), and mammals (Dingemanse and Réale 2005). Whether these correlations constrain the effects of developmental environments may depend on whether they reflect fundamental genetic constraints or arise due to independent but correlated plastic responses to the environment. It is still unclear which is the predominant mechanism (Bell 2005), with evidence for both hypotheses. For example, work on Drosophila (Sokolowski 2001), zebrafish, Danio rerio (Norton et al. 2011), and bighorn ewes, Ovis canadensis (Dingemanse and Réale 2005), support a strong genetic basis for behavioral syndromes. In contrast, support for environmental effects and instability within short time periods has been seen in agile frog tadpoles, R. dalmatina (Urszan et al. 2015) and three-spined sticklebacks, Gasterosteus aculeatus (Bell and Sih 2007; Dingemanse et al. 2007). If the latter is the case, it would suggest that developmental environments could shape these between-individual correlations, although our understanding of how these early environments affect behavioral syndromes is still limited (Han and Dingemanse 2015; Royauté and Dochtermann 2017).

In our study, we investigated whether developmental environment affects animal personality using a small lizard species. Developmental temperature plays an important role in the development of many species, particularly for taxa such as reptiles, where many traits are affected by incubation temperature (Noble et al. 2018). These effects may be driven by different costs of development (Marshall et al. 2020) or expression of hormones, such as in leopard geckos, Eublepharis macularius (Rhen et al. 2005), which can then affect phenotypic plasticity (Dufty et al. 2002). Lizards from the Lampropholis genus are regularly used in studies investigating animal personality (e.g., Michelangeli et al. 2016a, 2018; Goulet et al. 2021) as well as effects of incubation temperature on physiology and behavior (e.g., Downes and Shine 1999; Llewelyn et al. 2018; Kar et al. 2022). Accordingly, we used a species of Lampropholis skink to investigate the impacts of incubation temperature on animal personality. Southern rainforest sunskinks, L. similis (also known as southern Wet Tropics sunskinks), are small diurnal skinks (adult snout-vent length (SVL) ~ 45 mm) endemic to the wet tropics of Queensland’s north-east (Wilson and Swan 2021). In this species, incubation temperature was found to affect dessication rate and short-term critical thermal minima (Llewelyn et al. 2018), but effects of incubation temperature on behavior have not yet been investigated. Furthermore, while no behavioral syndrome has yet been found in captive-reared southern rainforest sunskinks (Goulet et al. 2021), previous studies have suggested that natural environments may be necessary to generate and maintain repeatability in, and correlations between, behavioral traits (Wilson et al. 1994; Urszan et al. 2015). As such, a secondary aim of our study was to investigate if skinks that had developed in captivity differed in animal personality from those that had developed in the wild.

We first compared four behavioral traits (activity, exploration, sociability, and boldness) of lizards incubated at 23 °C and 26 °C to examine how incubation temperature affected mean behavior, repeatability, within- and between-individual variation, and correlations among traits. We predicted that mean behavior would differ among treatments, and, given that Niemelä et al. 2019) found higher within- and between-individual variation in crickets developing under hotter temperatures, we predicted rainforest sunskinks incubated at the hotter temperature would also have higher estimates for both variance components. We then examined whether there were any differences in the same behavioral measures between the behavior of both incubation temperature groups to that of their fathers, who had been captured from a wild population. We focused on only males, and hence fathers rather than mothers, as there is evidence for limited sex-specific personality in Lampropholis skinks (Michelangeli et al. 2016a; Goulet et al. 2021), as well as to exclude reproductive state as a confounding factor. Repeatability was expected to be higher in the wild-reared skinks, which may be due to past exposure to factors such as predation, as in agile frog tadpoles (Urszan et al. 2015), or due to greater varation in natural environments allowing populations to express greater between-individual variation (Bell et al. 2009).

Methods

Capture, housing, and breeding

Adult skinks were hand-captured from a population in Hervey Range (19.362° S, 146.4767° E), located at the southern end of the Wet Tropics of north-east Queensland, Australia, between June 2013 and January 2014. Hand-capture of Lampropholis skink species samples a range of behavioral types (Michelangeli et al. 2016b), thus mitigating the likelihood of capture bias. Skinks were transported to James Cook University in Townsville where they were housed individually in plastic tubs (340 × 120 × 160 mm) for 6 months before mating pairs were established as part of another experiment (Llewelyn et al. 2017, 2018). Mating tubs were checked daily for eggs, and, if no fertile eggs were laid within 2 months, mating partners were swapped. Individual eggs were placed into 84-mL air-tight containers containing moist vermiculite (50:50 vermiculite to water by weight) and incubated at either 23 °C or 26 °C. The incubators were checked daily and, once hatched, the skinks were housed individually and given a unique toe clip for identification. Skinks were housed in small groups when they reached adult size (> 0.75 g). Hatching took place over the course of approximately one year, between January 2014 and March 2015. The parents were also transferred to small, single sex groups following breeding, and the males later formed the wild-reared group in our study (hereafter fathers). In 2015, the fathers, low incubation, and high incubation groups were transported to Monash University, Clayton, and housed in same-sex groups of six in plastic containers (300 × 230 × 370 mm) in temperature-controlled rooms (~ 22 °C). The containers were lined with newspaper and contained moist Sphagnum moss and a terracotta saucer with a heat lamp to create a basking site. Lights were on between 07:00 and 19:00, following a 12:12 day:night cycle, and were supplemented with ultraviolet lighting. Water was provided ad libitum, and the skinks were fed live crickets, Acheta domesticus, dusted in calcium and vitamin powders three times per week.

Experimental design

Over the 2016/2017 Austral summer, adult male skinks, including the fathers (n = 21) and those from the low incubation (23 °C: n = 11) and high incubation (26 °C: n = 10) treatments, underwent separate assays to measure activity, exploration, sociability, and boldness behaviors. At the beginning of the experiments, the youngest lab-born skink was 781 days old. As Lampropholis lizards mature after 1 year under optimal conditions, all our experimental animals were therefore mature adults. The individuals selected for the study from the two incubation temperature treatments were unrelated, i.e., no siblings or half-siblings.

Assays were carried out in a fixed order to reduce possible carry over effects, with those expected to have the greatest influence on subsequent behavior completed last (Bell 2012). Each behavior was measured three times before moving on to the next. Activity was measured first, followed by exploration, sociability, and then boldness. Each replicate was conducted at least four days apart to further minimize potential carryover effects (Bell 2012), with a minimum of four days also between different behavioral assays. We did not provide food in the 24 h prior to testing to standardize digestion levels, as feeding is known to alter the behavior of Lampropholis skinks (Shine 2003). To assess possible body size effects on behavior, snout-vent length (SVL; mm) and mass (g) were measured before experimentation.

Trials were carried out in opaque plastic containers (~ 55 × 32 × 24 cm) in temperature-controlled rooms (~ 22 °C). We used a video camera to record trials, rather than direct observation, to avoid observer-induced disturbance. Focal skinks were individually acclimated to the experimental area for 10 min under a clear plastic container, which was removed at the start of each trial. To prevent scent contamination between trials, following each test the equipment was washed with scentless detergent and thoroughly dried (Downes and Shine 1998). Each behavioral assay was conducted three times per individual to measure repeatability (Nakagawa and Schielzeth 2010; Wolak et al. 2012). At the conclusion of all assays, behaviors were scored from the video recordings using the program JWatcher (Blumstein et al. 2006). Scoring was performed blind to incubation temperature treatment, but not to captive or wild origin.

Activity

To measure activity, we observed the distance individual skinks moved in an open-field arena marked with 20 equal grid squares. We recorded a count of the number of times a focal skink crossed from one square into another over 45 min (Chapple et al. 2011; Michelangeli et al. 2016b).

Exploration

To measure propensity to explore, we observed individuals’ tendency to traverse an obstacle to find an essential resource, a basking site (Chapple et al. 2011; Goulet et al. 2017). We created an obstacle using an opaque barrier (10 cm high) with a polyvinyl chloride (PVC) tube (4-cm diameter, 10 cm in length) running through the middle of the barrier, located approximately 1.5 cm from the ground level. The barrier spanned the width of an arena and divided it into two compartments; one which contained a basking site constructed from a terracotta saucer and heat lamp, and the other where skinks began the experiment. The only way that skinks could reach the basking site was by finding their way through the PVC tube. We measured the time spent inspecting the barrier (a novel object as it is not present in skinks’ home cages) and latency to reach the basking site within 45 min.

Sociability

To determine sociability, we presented individual skinks with a choice between basking with conspecifics or alone (Chapple et al. 2011; Michelangeli et al. 2016a, b; Goulet et al. 2017). Two basking sites were placed at either end of the arena, each divided by clear Perspex™ barriers that spanned the width of the arena and enabled basking on both sides of the barriers. This created three areas in the arena; one containing three male southern rainforest sunskinks that had been born in captivity but were not part of any experimental treatment to simulate a social group, a central area containing the focal skink, and a third that remained empty. The central area was then further divided into three using black marker with zones designated as “social,” “no-choice,” and “asocial” with increasing distance from the area containing the social group. Both the “social” and “asocial’ zones contained basking sites as they were adjacent to the Perspex™ barriers. Heat lamps raised the temperatures at the basking sites to ~ 35 °C, prompting the focal skink to select a basking site either in the “social” or “asocial” zone. We measured the time spent basking in the ‘social’ zone over a 45 min period.

Boldness

We measured boldness by observing the latency of individual skinks to emerge from shelter and subsequently bask after a simulated predator attack (Michelangeli et al. 2016b). The arena contained a basking site at one end and shelter at the other. We simulated predator attacks by using a small paintbrush to tap the skinks at the base of their tail until they fled into the shelter. We recorded their latency to emerge from shelter, with a maximum time allowed of 25 min and then the amount of time basking over an additional 10 min after emergence. For skinks that did not emerge, an emergence time of 25 min and a basking score of 0 min was given.

Statistical analysis

Data were analyzed using the program R (R Development Core Team 2016), with statistical significance set to α = 0.05. We tested for age effects on both incubation temperature groups, as they had hatched over the course of approximately 14 months. As there was no significant effect of age on behavior (p > 0.200), we excluded age as a covariate in our models.

To determine whether incubation temperature affected mean activity, exploration, sociability, or boldness, and to compare these to the wild-reared fathers, we used separate mixed-effect models for each behavior. Latency (s) to emerge from shelter (boldness) and to reach the goal compartment (exploration) were both analyzed using Cox mixed-effects models (Therneau 2020), activity was analyzed with a generalized linear mixed-effects model with a Poisson distribution, and all other behavioral measures were analyzed with linear mixed models fitted with Gaussian error distributions using the lme4 package (Bates et al. 2015). Time basking in the social zone and time basking after emerging from shelter were both square root transformed to meet assumptions. Models included lizard ID as a random effect to control for repeated measures, and mass (scaled), time of day (scaled), trial number, and treatment (fathers, high incubation, low incubation) were included as fixed effects. We used a combination of Akaike information criteria (AIC) and likelihood ratio tests (LRT) to examine whether interactions among fixed effects or, as there were some father and son pairs, including family ID as a random effect, improved model fit. Interactions that improved model fit were retained in the final model, but family ID did not improve the fit for any behavioral variable and so was excluded from further analysis. Furthermore, we examined whether SVL was a better predictor of body size effects on behavior, either as a substitution for, or in addition to, mass using AIC and LRTs. Mass was the better predictor, or had equal support, compared to SVL (Table S1), and so we controlled for body size using mass. When factors with more than three levels were significant, we used the emmeans R package (Lenth 2020) to perform post hoc pairwise comparisons among groups.

Repeatability estimates (R) and variance components (VID and VR) were obtained from Bayesian univariate mixed-effects models using the brms package (Burkner 2017), with repeatability estimated as VID/(VID + VR). Models were specified with weakly informative priors and ran for 10,000 iterations (3000 warmup) with the same error distributions, transformations, and fixed and random effect structure as above. We calculated repeatability and variance components fitted separately for each group of skinks, and so treatment was not included as a fixed effect in these models. Trace plots were visually inspected to ensure convergence and proper model mixing. To estimate the effect size of treatment on the variance components and repeatability estimates, we calculated the pairwise difference between posterior distributions and used this to estimate the mean difference and 95% credible intervals (CrI) (Royauté et al. 2015; Royauté and Dochtermann 2017). Repeatable behaviors were then included in separate multivariate Bayesian mixed-effect models for each group to test for the presence of behavioral syndromes, with the same priors, error distributions, transformations, and fixed and random effects structure. Inference for all tests was based on means and where CrIs did not overlap with zero.

Results

Mean behavioral differences

Activity was affected by the interaction between developmental environment and trial number (Table 1; Fig. 1a). Mean activity did not differ among developmental environments (p > 0.325), but there were differences among groups in how activity changed across trials. All groups decreased activity from trial one to trial two, but only the wild-caught fathers further decreased activity in the third compared to second trial (Table S2; Fig. 1a). Mass and time of day also had a positive relationship with activity (Table 1).

Table 1 Results of linear mixed-effects models analyzing the effects of incubation temperature and rearing in a wild compared to captive environment (group) on mean behavior in southern Wet Tropics sunskinks. Significance is indicated by bold text
Fig. 1
figure 1

Effects of developmental environment (wild: gray; high incubation: yellow; and low incubation: teal) on activity (a) across trials 1, 2, and 3; exploration (b time to reach the goal compartment; c time inspecting the barrier); sociability (d); and boldness behaviors (e time to emerge from shelter; f time basking after emerging from shelter) in rainforest sunskinks. Boxplots show medians, interquartile ranges (IQR), and whiskers the 1.5 × IQR

We also found significant effects of developmental environment on exploratory behavior (Table 1; Fig. 1b, c). Skinks incubated in the high temperature treatment were more explorative than both the wild-caught fathers and those incubated in the low temperature treatment, as indicated by a higher proportion of individuals reaching the goal compartment (Table S2; Fig. 1b) and a greater amount of time inspecting the barrier (Table S2; Fig. 1c). In contrast, skinks incubated at the low temperature did not differ in exploratory behavior from the wild-caught fathers (Table S2; Fig. 1b, c). Trial number significantly affected time inspecting the barrier, but not latency to reach the goal compartment (Table 1). Skinks from all developmental environments spent less time inspecting the barrier in trial two compared to trial one (− 32.15 ± 12.9 SE, p = 0.039), but time spent inspecting the barrier did not significantly differ between trials two and three (− 26.12 ± 12.8, p = 0.110), or between trials one and three (6.03 ± 13.0, p = 0.888). Skinks were also more explorative later in the day and when they were heavier (Table 1).

Sociability was not significantly affected by developmental environment and was the only behavior not significantly affected by mass (Table 1; Fig. 1d). We also found some limited effects of developmental environment on boldness (Table 1). Developmental environment did not have a significant effect on latency to emerge from shelter (Table 1; Fig. 1e), but did have a significant effect on time basking after emerging from shelter in the main model (Table 1). However, post hoc pairwise comparisons revealed no significant differences among incubation treatments, as well as non-significant decrease in basking behavior in the wild-caught compared to lab-reared skinks (Table S2; Fig. 1f). Mass had a positive relationship with boldness, decreasing latency to emerge from shelter and increasing time basking after emerging from shelter (Table 1).

Repeatability and variation

Behavioral repeatability differed among developmental environments and behaviors. Only two behaviors were repeatable for skinks from all three developmental environments: activity and time inspecting the barrier (Table 2). For activity, repeatability was higher in the low incubation treatment compared to both the high incubation treatment and wild-caught fathers (Table S3; Fig. 2a), while for time inspecting the barrier the low incubation treatment had higher repeatability compared to only the high incubation treatment (Table S3; Fig. 2b). In contrast to these behaviors, latency to emerge from shelter and sociability were only repeatable in skinks incubated at the high temperature, and time basking after emerging from shelter was repeatable for both the low incubation treatment and wild-caught fathers (Table 2). The low incubation treatment had a higher repeatability estimate for basking time compared to the fathers (Table S3; Fig. 2f). No developmental treatment group was repeatable for latency to emerge from shelter (Table 2).

Table 2 Adjusted repeatability, between-individual variation (VID), and within-individual variation (VR) with 95% credibility intervals (CrI) for behaviors measured in southern Wet Tropics sunskinks incubated at a low or high temperature, as well as for the wild-caught fathers. Bold text indicates repeatability and variance estimates significantly different from zero
Fig. 2
figure 2

Pairwise differences in behavioral repeatability estimates between southern rainforest sunskinks that had developed in the wild (fathers) or in captivity at a low or high incubation temperature. Differences are shown for activity (a), exploration (b time inspecting barrier; c latency to reach goal), sociability (d), and boldness (e latency to emerge from shelter; f basking after emerging) behavior. Circles represent mean difference between groups, and vertical lines show 95% credibility intervals. Repeatability estimates were derived from posterior distributions of Bayesian mixed-effects models

Developmental environment only affected between-individual variation in three behaviors (Table S3). When comparing incubation treatments, skinks incubated at a low temperature had higher between-individual variation for activity and time basking after emerging from shelter (Table S3; Fig. 3a, f), while between-individual variation was higher in the high incubation treatment for sociability (Table S3; Fig. 3d). There were a greater number of differences in within-individual variation among incubation treatments (Table S3). Within-individual variation was more frequently higher in the high incubation treatment, which was the case for activity, time inspecting the barrier, and both boldness behaviors, latency to emerge from shelter and time basking after emerging from shelter (Fig. 3a, b, e, f). However, for latency to reach the goal and sociability, within-individual variation was higher in the low incubation treatment (Fig. 3c, d).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Pairwise differences in between-individual variation (VID; black circles) and within-individual variation (VR; gray triangles) of southern rainforest sunskinks that developed in the wild (fathers) or in captivity at a low or high incubation temperature. Differences are shown for activity (a), exploration (b time inspecting barrier; c latency to reach goal), sociability (d), and boldness (e latency to emerge from shelter; f basking after emerging) behavior. Values represent differences in mean variation between groups and vertical lines show 95% credibility intervals. Estimates were derived from posterior distributions of Bayesian mixed-effects models

Wild- and captive-reared skinks also differed in between-individual variation. This tended to be lower in the wild-reared fathers, although whether the difference was significant depended on incubation treatment (Table S3; Fig. 3a–f). The only exception was for time basking after emerging from shelter, where the fathers had higher between-individual variation compared to the high incubation treatment (Table S3; Fig. 3f). Similar to differences among incubation treatments, there were a greater number of differences in within-individual variation between captive- and wild-reared skinks. Within-individual variation was also lower in the fathers for most behaviors when compared to both incubation treatments (Table S3; Fig. 3a–f). There was also an exception where within-individual variation was higher for the high incubation treatment compared to the fathers for latency to reach the goal compartment (Fig. 3c).

Behavioral syndromes

The treatments differed in which behaviors were repeatable (Table 3; Fig. 2), and so the models examining between-individual correlations among behaviors differed between treatments (Table 3). We found no evidence for incubation temperature driving differences among behavioral syndromes, with no among-individual correlations between any repeatable behavior (Table 3). This did not appear to be the result of developing in captivity, as we also did not detect relationships among behaviors in the fathers (Table 3).

Table 3 Between-individual correlations among repeatable behaviors with 95% credibility intervals (CrI) for skinks incubated at a low or high incubation temperature, as well as for wild-caught fathers. NA indicates pairs of behaviors where one or more were not significantly repeatable for a group, and therefore not included in behavioral syndrome analysis

Discussion

Our results show that developmental conditions affect the behavior of southern rainforest sunskinks, including mean behavior, repeatability, and within- and between-individual variation. Skinks incubated at a higher temperature were the most explorative, compared to those that developed at a lower temperature or in the wild. Effects on repeatability and behavioral variation were complex, with incubation temperature having opposite effects on different traits. Overall, differences in repeatability because of variation in developmental environment were primarily driven by changes in within-individual variation, which were higher in a greater number of traits for the high incubation treatment and lowest in the wild-caught group. We also found no evidence for a behavioral syndrome involving these behaviors in rainforest sunskinks, similar to previous studies on this species (Goulet et al. 2021), indicating that incubation temperature, as well as rearing in a captive compared to wild environment, does not affect between-individual correlations among behavioral traits. While our results further highlight the lasting effects of developmental conditions on an organism’s phenotype via behavioral responses, the fitness and ecological consequences of these effects remain unclear.

A higher incubation temperature resulted in southern rainforest sunskinks that were more explorative. Riskier behavioral types, such as exploration, can lead to a small but significant increase in survival in the wild, likely due to higher resource acquisition despite increased predation risk (Moiron et al. 2020). Our results, therefore, suggest a potential benefit to developing at a higher temperature, but contrast with previous studies. Three-toed skinks, Saiphos equalis, for example, incubated at a higher temperature spent less time exploring (Beltran et al. 2020), while pine snakes, Pituophis melanoleucus, incubated at both high and low temperatures were slower explorers than those incubated at a medium temperature (Burger 1998). Incubation temperature effects can differ in magnitude and direction in a trait- and species-dependent way (Noble et al. 2018; While et al. 2018), which may be due to differences between species in thermal adaptation or thermal preferences (Sinervo et al. 2010; Pincheira-Donoso et al. 2013; Beltran et al. 2020) and might lead to different impacts of climate change among species. It is, therefore, not unusual to see contrasting results between studies. The other behaviors we measured in our study did not, however, differ between incubation treatments. It may be our relatively small sample sizes playing a role, or, alternatively, some incubation temperature effects may not persist into adulthood, which has been found previously for critical thermal minimum in southern rainforest sunskinks (Llewelyn et al. 2018). However, other long-term studies have found incubation temperature effects lasting to 181–365 days post-hatching (Noble et al. 2018). It is more likely, therefore, that incubation temperature does not play a large part in determining mean behavioral phenotype of rainforest sunskinks, although with some exceptions, such as exploration.

We also found few differences in average behavior when comparing the captive-reared skinks to the wild-caught fathers. This was surprising, given the vast differences in developmental environments (Foster 2013), and previous studies demonstrating behavioral differences as a result of physical and social enrichment of rearing environments and as a result of predator cues in a wide range of taxa (e.g., reptiles: Riley et al. 2017; invertebrates: Liedtke et al. 2015; and amphibians: Urszan et al. 2015), both factors which would differ between captive and natural environments. However, garden skinks, L. guichenoti, reared with predator scent were found to become less responsive to the predator cues over time, such that after 1-year activity and microhabitat use did not differ from skinks reared with a control scent (Downes 2001). Similarly, juvenile but not adult southern water skinks, Eulamprus heatwolei, avoided predator odors (Head et al. 2002). Despite the age difference between captive- and wild-reared skinks in our study, all skinks were adults, which, if rainforest sunskinks also become less responsive behaviorally to predators with age, may explain the lack of difference in activity as well as sociability, as grouping behavior can provide antipredator benefits in skinks (e.g., delicate skinks: (Downes and Hoefer 2004). However, we did find lower exploratory behavior and a non-significant trend of lower boldness behavior in the wild-reared fathers. This trend may be due to riskier behavioral types more likely to persist in captivity but be predated upon in the wild (Smith and Blumstein 2008; Archard and Braithwaite 2010; Urszan et al. 2015), although does not explain why differences in exploration depended on the incubation temperature of the captive-reared skinks. It is possible that for southern rainforest sunskinks, the embryonic thermal environment, while not playing a large role in development of mean behavior, is still more important than differences present between captive and natural environments in contributing to adult phenotypes.

While we only found differences in mean exploratory behavior among incubation treatments, as well as captive—compared to wild-reared skinks, most behaviors differed in repeatability. This highlights the importance of developmental environments in shaping behavioral consistency, alongside studies investigating impacts of diet (Careau et al. 2014), predation (Urszan et al. 2015), and immune challenges (DiRienzo et al. 2015). For the repeatable behaviors, repeatability estimates fell broadly within the range of average repeatability, 0.37, found in a meta-analysis on a wide range of taxa (Bell et al. 2009). Interestingly, despite the above studies demonstrating that factors such as diet, predation, and immune challenges can affect repeatability, which would, among others, differ between natural and captive environments, behaviors where the wild-caught fathers demonstrated personality were the same behaviors where skinks from the low incubation treatment demonstrated personality. In contrast, there were more differences between the fathers and skinks incubated at the high temperature. While we cannot rule out some differences may be due to age variation between captive- and wild-reared skinks, this suggests a relatively large role of incubation temperature, among other factors varying during development, in determining personality in southern rainforest sunskinks.

Variation in personality between incubation treatments were largely driven by differences in within-individual variation, with differences in between-individual variation inconsistent, or non-existent, among behaviors. Skinks incubated at the high temperature had higher within-individual variation in four of the six behavioral measures, despite demonstrating personality in more behaviors, compared to skinks incubated at a low temperature. The only occasions where the higher incubation treatment had lower within-individual variation were for the behaviors where the high, but not low, incubation treatment demonstrated repeatability, i.e., latency to reach the goal compartment and sociability. Niemelä et al. (2019) demonstrated a similar trend in field crickets, Gryllus bimaculutas, where a higher developmental temperature increased within-individual variance in exploration behavior. However, crickets developing at this higher temperature also had greater between-individual variation (Niemelä et al. 2019). In comparison, in our study, southern rainforest sunskinks incubated at the higher temperature had increased between-individual variation for only one behavior, sociability, while those from the low incubation treatment had greater between-individual variation for two behavioral measures. This suggests that incubation temperature in skinks may have a greater effect on personality through affecting within-individual variation, i.e., the behavioral plasticity of individuals, potentially through different responses to trial number or habituation to the experimental assays, rather than affecting variation in behavioral types within populations. However, our sample sizes were relatively low, and we may therefore have not been able to fully capture more subtle impacts of incubation temperature on between-individual variation. Regardless, more plastic individuals are thought to be better suited to cope with environmental change (Visser 2008; Sih et al. 2011; Van Buskirk 2012), provided plasticity is adaptive rather than maladaptive or neutral. Therefore, as average temperatures, as well as incubation temperatures, rise with climate change, southern rainforest sunskinks may be better able to cope with short-term environment change as juveniles or adults.

Personality differences being driven by within-individual variation may be explained by early environments signaling that greater within-individual variation may be more beneficial in future environments. As such, developmental plasticity may affect short-term phenotypic changes expressed later in life, i.e., reversible plasticity. In mosquitofish, Gambusia holbrooki, for example, fish born in summer develop in, and are more likely to experience, warmer temperatures, and therefore have a lower ability to adjust metabolism in response to cooler temperatures experienced in adulthood, resulting in developmental plasticity affecting the ability to be reversibly plastic later in life (Seebacher et al. 2014). This reduces the potential of a phenotype-environment mismatch, and, if developmental environments reliably predict the need for reversible plasticity, may also reduce the trade-off with investment in other fitness-related traits, such as reproduction, as maintaining the ability to be plastic, such as more neural tissue or flexibility in physiology, can itself be costly (Snell-Rood 2013). While our study did not measure plastic responses of rainforest sunskinks in response to a change in the physical or social environment, the differences in within-individual variation observed among developmental treatments may have been the result of differences in individual variance in response to trial number or habituation to the behavioral assays. Alternatively, it may be more costly for individuals to maintain an ability to be more behaviorally variable or plastic. Higher costs of development, which can change, for example, with ectotherm developmental temperature (Marshall et al. 2020), or restricted resources during development, may therefore also affect investment in reversible plasticity. Increased diet quality in juvenile house crickets, for example, decreased repeatability through increased within-individual variation (Royauté and Dochtermann 2017), and, similarly, zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata, reared without diet restrictions also had higher within-individual variance in activity (Careau et al. 2014). This may also explain the pattern of lower within-individual variation in the fathers in most behavioral measures compared to both incubation treatments, as conditions in captivity are generally more relaxed, for example, with consistent access to food and no predation risk, compared to what individuals experience in natural environments.

Despite repeatability in many behavioral traits, there was no evidence for a behavioral syndrome in southern rainforest sunskinks. This lack of between-individual correlations among behaviors is unsurprising, given Goulet et al. (2021) also did not find any correlations between activity, exploration, and sociability in southern rainforest sunskinks. However, our result contrasts with those from studies on the delicate skink, a closely related species, where activity, exploration, and, in some populations, boldness form a syndrome (Michelangeli et al. 2016b, 2019; Goulet et al. 2017). Many other studies have also found behavioral syndromes, suggesting they appear to be widespread among species (Wolf et al. 2007; Conrad et al. 2011; Wolf and Weissing 2012). There are many ways in which behaviors can correlate, and many ways in which these correlations might emerge (Réale et al. 2010). It is possible that these mechanisms are not present in southern rainforest skink habitats or that time in captivity has eroded or weakened relationships between behaviors (Wilson et al. 1994). Regardless, as neither incubation temperature group demonstrated significant correlations between behaviors, we can exclude developmental temperature as a factor driving the formation of behavioral syndromes, at least in southern rainforest sunskinks. However, incubation temperature could be important in the formation of behavioral syndromes in species that display such syndromes in wild populations, indicating an area requiring further research.

In summary, differences in incubation temperature affected behavioral variation and consistency of southern rainforest sunskinks, driven primarily through changes in within-individual variation. While skinks incubated at a higher temperature demonstrated personality in more behaviors, they also tended to have higher within-individual variation, specifically for behaviors where they were not the only treatment to demonstrate repeatability. We speculate that this may be due to lower costs of development, allowing them to develop a greater capacity for variation in behavior or that different incubation temperatures signal differences in future environments where higher within-individual variation may be more, or less, beneficial to individuals. Regardless, this may mean that skinks incubated at a higher temperature are more able to flexibly adjust to short-term environmental change. As environments are changing rapidly due to anthropogenic influences, such as habitat modification and climate change, this may be a mechanism for this group to better cope with these future environments. In contrast, mean behavior was not as strongly affected by developmental environment, with only differences between incubation treatments, as well as between captive- and wild-reared skinks, present in exploration. There was also no evidence that developmental temperatures affected between-individual correlations among behaviors, with no behavioral syndrome present in either incubation treatment. This did not appear to be the result of rearing in captivity, as there was also no behavioral syndrome present in the wild-caught fathers. Overall, our results demonstrate a large impact of rearing environment in shaping animal personality and behavioral variation, with incubation temperature a particularly significant factor in lizard behavioral development.