Introduction

Parasitic relationships between obligate ectoparasites and their animal hosts are predicted to be influenced by ecological factors and host-specific traits (Barber and Dingemanse 2010; Kiffner et al. 2011; Perez et al. 2016). Ticks are obligate ectoparasites that feed on the blood of various vertebrate hosts, including birds (Hillyard 1996; Kurtenbach et al. 2006), and negatively influence host fitness (Norte et al. 2013). Tick burden varies among individuals as a function of individual-specific traits, such as host foraging style, personality (Barber and Dingemanse 2010; Newman et al. 2015; Sih et al. 2018), body size, or body condition (Norte et al. 2013; Newman et al. 2015). For instance, ground-foraging (compared to canopy-foraging) bird species have higher tick burdens (Newman et al. 2015; Loss et al. 2016; Kocianová et al. 2017), as do larger (compared to smaller) species (Newman et al. 2015). Furthermore, individual-specific differences in foraging or social behaviour can also predict tick burdens within populations of the same species (Sih et al. 2018). For example, individuals that are more active and explorative in their (foraging) behaviour may encounter more parasites (e.g. ticks), and consequently have higher tick burdens (Barber and Dingemanse 2010; Sih et al. 2018; Taggart et al. 2018). The link between exploration behaviour and ectoparasite (i.e. tick) infestation has been investigated in few species (Barber and Dingemanse 2010; Sih et al. 2018); further studies are needed to determine whether reported relationships are general versus specific to species or their ecological conditions. Here, we investigated whether tick infestation varied as a function of two individual-specific traits (exploratory behaviour and body condition), using wild great tits (Parus major) as a model. Both exploration behaviour and body condition are repeatable in great tits (Stuber et al. 2013; Moiron et al. 2018, 2019).

The likelihood and number of ticks carried by an individual bird can vary also as a function of ecological factors, particularly those affecting tick abundance and questing behaviour (Mejlon and Jaenson 1997). Key factors include temperature and humidity (Lindgren et al. 2000; Kiffner et al. 2011; Titcomb et al. 2017), host breeding density (Oorebeek and Kleindorfer 2008; Takumi et al. 2019), and vegetation type and coverage (Kiffner et al. 2011; Tack et al. 2012a, b, 2013). These factors vary spatiotemporally and, consequently, tick abundance will vary throughout the environment and affect host tick burden depending on how hosts are distributed over tick habitats. Tick burden generally varies with tick and host density (Randolph 1998, 2008); any factor that affects their ratio thus influences tick burden. Both the percentage of infested birds and tick burden are, consequently, predicted to change with habitat modifications and climate change (Lindgren et al. 2000; Randolph 2004; Akimov and Nebogatkin 2016). For example, city-dwelling birds may carry lower tick burdens (Evans et al. 2009; Hamer et al. 2012) as urbanisation reduces tick densities (Akimov and Nebogatkin 2016; Heylen et al. 2019). To date, most research has focused on the effects of urbanisation on bird tick infestation due to habitat modifications rather than susceptibility to tick infestation (Hamer et al. 2012; Akimov and Nebogatkin 2016). Human presence affects bird reproductive success and bird habitat choice (Hutfluss and Dingemanse 2019), and may therefore influence susceptibility of birds to tick infestation. Studies documenting effects of human disturbance on tick burden in bird populations are required to identify factors with general versus study-specific effects (Kelly 2006; Nakagawa and Parker 2015).

We tested the link between two phenotypic traits (exploration behaviour and body condition) and two environmental factors (breeding density and human disturbance) on tick infestation of great tits inhabiting 12 forest plots varying in breeding density and human disturbance levels (Araya-Ajoy and Dingemanse 2017; Hutfluss and Dingemanse 2019). Our repeated measures design spans over 3 years and 12 plots and enables the quantification of the amount of variance in tick burden attributable to among-year (temporal variation), among-plot (spatial variation), and among-bird (individual variation) variation. This variance partitioning approach thereby resulted in quantitative estimates of the magnitude of variation attributable to multiple key biological levels of variation.

Material and methods

Study sites and data collection

Our study was performed in 12 nest box plots within a 10 × 15 km2 area in southern Munich (47° 57′ N, 11° 21′ E) established in autumn 2009. Each plot was fitted with 50 nest boxes in a regular grid covering approximately 9 hectares. Breeding parameters of great tits (detailed below) were monitored for 3 years (2017–2019) within all plots except one plot that was monitored only in 2017. Each nest box was inspected biweekly during the breeding season (April–July) for nesting activity. During plot inspections, the number of recreationists was counted as a measure of human disturbance (detailed by Hutfluss and Dingemanse 2019).

When hatchlings were 10–12 days old, both parents were captured in the nest box using a spring trap. Birds were given a unique, numbered band if not previously banded. They were then tested for their exploration behaviour (detailed below), weighed, measured morphologically (Moiron et al. 2019), aged (based on plumage characteristic as first year breeder or older; Dingemanse et al. 2020), and screened for ticks (protocol detailed below). Most birds were captured in May and a few in June. These captures were used to calculate breeding density, defined as the number of breeding pairs producing first clutches per hectare.

Exploration behaviour

Exploration behaviour was measured using a novel environment test adapted to the field using established protocols (see Stuber et al. 2013). Birds were transferred to a holding box attached to a cage (61 L × 39 W × 40 H cm) fitted with a mesh front and three perches, representing the novel environment. The holding box was covered with a cloth bag and the subject was allowed to acclimatise for 1 min. After acclimatisation, the focal bird was released into the novel environment without handling and recorded for 2 min with the field observer located out of sight. Videos were subsequently scored by dividing the cage into six equal sections and three floor sections as described in Stuber et al. (2013). The exploration score was calculated as the sum of movements between all sections within the first 2 min of entering the novel environment (Abbey-Lee and Dingemanse 2019; Dingemanse et al. 2020). The behavioural data used in this paper was based on videos scored in randomised order within years, and blindly with respect to individual identity and tick infestation.

Tick burden and collection

Ticks tend to concentrate around the eyes and beak of great tits (Heylen and Matthysen 2008; Fracasso et al. 2019). A regional patch examination protocol (reviewed in Lydecker et al. 2019) was developed to standardise the screening. We screened (1) around the eyes/ears and along the margin of the beak on both sides of the bird, (2) underneath the beak, (3) along the top margin of the beak, and (4) on the top of the head. We subsequently calculated the total number of ticks carried by each captured bird. In 2018–2019, all captured birds were screened; in 2017 only a sample. In 2018 and 2019, a sample of ticks were collected using fine tweezers and stored in 99% ethanol as part of another study. Those were subsequently morphologically identified to life-stage and species according to published taxonomic keys (Filippova 1977; Hillyard 1996; Estrada-Peña et al. 2014).

Recreation pressure index

During each plot inspection, all observers recorded each recreationist seen and their specific location (detailed by Hutfluss and Dingemanse 2019). We took care to count each recreationist only once by only counting the first observation of each recreationist per plot check. The probability to observe a recreationist during a plot inspection is biased by various factors (Hutfluss and Dingemanse 2019). To obtain an unbiased index of recreation pressure, the binary probability to observe recreationists was calculated using all inspections conducted from 2010 to 2019 (n = 3724 inspections). This probability was calculated using a binomial generalised linear mixed effects model (GLMM) fitting fixed effects for plot inspection duration, the number of observers, and starting time (in hours from sunrise) (Hutfluss and Dingemanse 2019). We fitted random intercepts for each unique combination of plot and year (termed plot-year, see Abbey-Lee et al. 2016; Araya-Ajoy et al. 2016; Araya-Ajoy and Dingemanse 2017) to acquire an average value of recreation pressure for each plot in each year, and for date of observation to control for date-specific environmental effects (Supplemental Table 1). We extracted best linear unbiased predictors (BLUPs) for each plot-year between 2017 and 2019 (n = 34) and used them in subsequent models as a recreation pressure index. The usage of BLUPs has been criticised when associated uncertainty is not taken forward; some have proposed to estimate a posterior distribution of possible BLUP values and thereby take forward uncertainty in subsequent analyses (Hadfield et al. 2010; Houslay and Wilson 2017). However, taking forward uncertainty in BLUP values can result in biased estimates, whereas utilising average BLUPs as fixed effects results in less precise but unbiased estimates (Dingemanse et al. 2020). Therefore, we here present the estimated effect of the average BLUP values.

Data preparation

The scaled mass index (Peig and Green 2009) was used as our measure of body condition. Following Peig and Green (2009), this index was calculated separately for each sex as follows:

$$ \kern0.75em {SM}_i={M}_i\times {\left(\frac{L_0}{L_i}\right)}^{b_{\mathrm{SMA}}} $$
(1)

where Mi and Li are the mass and tarsus measurements of individual i respectively; L0 is the arithmetic mean of all tarsus lengths of either all females or all males (dependent on the sex of individual i); and bSMA is the scaling exponent calculated as:

$$ \kern0.5em {b}_{\mathrm{SMA}}=\frac{b_{\mathrm{OLS}}}{R} $$
(2)

where bOLS is the slope of linear regression of log-transformed body mass as a function of log-transformed tarsus length of either all females or all males (dependent on the sex of individual i) and R is the Pearson correlation coefficient of the regression. We used all birds recorded from 2010 to 2019 (n = 4273 records) to calculate the scaling coefficient (including any repeated measures).

We calculated individual-mean exploration scores using a linear mixed effects model (LMM) fitting exploration behaviour as the response variable (Supplemental Table 2). We fitted test sequence as a fixed effect covariate to control for sequence effects (Dingemanse et al. 2012, 2020), and included random intercepts for individual and plot-year. The model included data from all years (2010–2019) (n = 4251) and assumed a Gaussian error distribution. We extracted an average BLUP value per individual, which we used in subsequent analyses as a measure of an individual’s average (experience-corrected) exploration score.

Statistical analysis

Tick infestation of great tits was analysed in two parts using GLMMs. First, infestation probability was estimated using all recorded captures from 2017 through 2019 (n = 784 records). Second, tick burden (i.e. the absolute number of ticks observed) was modelled using only infested individuals (n = 520 records). For both response variables, the model set up was the same. Fixed effects were fitted for recreation pressure, breeding density, body condition, sex, age, and individual-mean exploration score. Sex and age were fitted because males (versus females) and younger (versus older) birds often have higher tick burdens (Heylen and Matthysen 2008; Heylen et al. 2013). Random intercepts were fitted for year, plot, plot-year, field observer, nest box, and individual. Prior to analysis, all fixed effects were mean-centred, and variance standardised, such that the statistical intercepts of our models reflected the value for the average individual in the average environmental condition.

All statistical analyses were performed in R (version 3.5.3) (R Core Team 2019). Models were run using the functions lmer and glmer from the package lme4 (Bates et al. 2015) assuming either binomial (infestation probability) or Poisson (tick burden) error distributions. Mean estimates and their 95% credible intervals (CIs) were estimated based on 5000 simulations using the sim function from the arm package (Gelman and Su 2016). Residual errors were calculated according to Nakagawa and Schielzeth (2010). Adjusted repeatability values were calculated as the proportion of variance unexplained by the fixed effects that was explained by the focal random effect. Estimates with 95% CIs not overlapping zero were treated as statistically significant in the frequentist’s sense. Models were checked graphically for fit (Supplemental Figs. 1 and 2).

Results

On average, 65.1% of all captured adult great tits carried ticks. Infested birds were found in all study plots and all years (Fig. 1); the average tick burden of an infested bird (n = 520) was (mean ± SD) 5.69 ± 9.02 ticks (Fig. 2). Collected ticks (n = 1583) were either larvae (n = 860) or nymphs (n = 722); we found only one (male) adult tick (in 2018). All ticks belonged to the genus Ixodes; the most common species were I. ricinus (43.9%), I. frontalis (8.3%), and I. inopinatus (2.8%). The remaining ticks (45.0%) could only be identified to genus (Ixodes spp.) and not species level. Infestation probability but not tick burden varied between plots (Table 1). Both tick burden and infestation probability varied among nest boxes (Table 1). Individuals were not repeatable in infestation probability (Table 1). Analyses based on infested individuals showed that tick burden was moderately repeatable (estimate: 0.45; 95% CI: 0.40, 0.51).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Percentages of all captured great tit adults infested with ticks from the 2017 to 2019 breeding seasons. Data for plot 7 were only available for the year 2017

Fig. 2
figure 2

Average tick burden per infested great tit adult captured in each study plot during each field season (years 2017–2019). Error bars represent the standard error. Numbers above the bars are the number of great tits captured. Data for plot 7 were only available for the year 2017

Table 1 Effect size estimates (β) and 95% credible intervals (CIs) for great tit tick infestation as the probability of infestation (n = 784 records) and tick burden of infested individuals (n = 520 records)

Exploration behaviour positively correlated with infestation probability (estimate: 0.18; 95% CI: 0.01, 0.35; Fig. 3) but not with tick burden (estimate: − 0.02; 95% CI: − 0.12, 0.09). Additionally, body condition was negatively correlated with tick burden (estimate: − 0.11; 95% CI: − 0.19, − 0.02; Fig. 4) but not with infestation probability (estimate: 0.06; 95% CI: − 0.11, 0.24). A positive trend between infestation probability and recreational pressure was observed but was not statistically significant (estimate: 0.21; 95% CI: − 0.03, 0.43; Fig. 5). Breeding density, age, and sex did not affect infestation probability or tick burden (Table 1).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Infestation probability in relation to exploration score. For illustrational purposes only, the prepared raw data of infestation probability and exploration score were split into 10 quantiles and were averaged to help visualise this relationship. Each point refers to the mean infestation probability and exploration score for the given quantile. Error bars represent the standard error for the respective mean infestation probability. The numbers report the number of records in the respective quantile. The line reports the linear relationship presented in Table 1

Fig. 4
figure 4

Tick burden of infested birds in relation to their scaled mass index (i.e. body condition). The line reports the linear relationship presented in Table 1

Fig. 5
figure 5

Infestation probability in relation to recreation pressure. For graphical purposes only, the prepared raw data of infestation probability and recreation pressure were split into 10 quantiles and were averaged to help visualise this relationship. Each point refers to the mean infestation probability and recreational pressure index for the given quantile. Error bars report the standard error for the respective mean infestation probability. The numbers report the number of records in the respective quantile. The line reports the linear relationship presented in Table 1

Discussion

The relationship between ticks and their avian hosts is shaped by ecological factors that influence both host population dynamics and parasite (tick) distributions. Their respective effects, furthermore, vary as a function of phenotypic traits expressed by hosts, such as their behavioural (or ‘personality’) types. Our study sheds light on the relative influence of each of these biological factors. We quantified effects of human disturbance, breeding density, and phenotypic traits specific to hosts (exploration behaviour and body condition), using a cavity-breeding passerine, the great tit, as a model. We found that faster explorers were more likely to be infested with ticks. Furthermore, infested birds had higher tick burdens when in poor condition. Additionally, we found a non-significant trend suggesting that high recreation pressure increased infestation probability. Finally, both tick burden and infestation probability varied across time (years) and space (plots). Importantly, among infested birds, tick burden was moderately repeatable across years, implying that individual phenotypic traits (other than exploration behaviour and body condition) influenced tick infestation. Our findings help elucidate the importance of ecological and behavioural mediators of the relationship between ticks and their avian hosts. This observational study now awaits experimental testing to reveal causality of the reported relationships.

Explorative birds were more likely to be infested but did not carry more ticks. This very same pattern has also been reported in wild squirrels (Santicchia et al. 2019). Our birds were predominantly infested by larvae and nymphs of the genus Ixodes. As for many other vertebrate hosts in Europe (Hillyard 1996), the most common tick species was I. ricinus. This tick species quests for hosts vertically up to 1.5 m (Mejlon and Jaenson 1997). An explanation for our results that requires verification is that explorative birds thus foraged close to the ground, where nymphs and larvae are most abundant (Mejlon and Jaenson 1997), and therefore were more likely to acquire ticks. Indeed, foraging style has previously been shown to be an important predictor of tick burden in other bird species (Newman et al. 2015; Kocianová et al. 2017). Exploration behaviour is known to predict foraging style, and therefore, the observed effect of exploration behaviour could partially be a result of individual differences in foraging style (Verbeek et al. 1994; Aplin et al. 2014; Moiron et al. 2018; but see Moyers et al. 2018). The use of radio-tracking may help to elucidate whether increased infestation probability is linked to where faster explorers forage or to how much they move (Snijders et al. 2014).

Sex and age did not influence tick infestation even though previous studies found (or implied) that males (compared to females) have increased tick burdens (Hoodless et al. 2003), while older (versus younger) birds have decreased tick burdens (Heylen et al. 2013; but see Heylen and Matthysen 2008). The absence of a sex effect in this study could relate to our study design. We collected data during the breeding season, when both males and females may have been in low condition due to chick rearing (Ots and Horak 1996), which may have masked any sex differences. Even so, sex-biased parasite infestation is not universally found in all species, suggesting that our null effect is genuine (Heylen and Matthysen 2008; Kiffner et al. 2013). Additionally, we found no effect of age on tick infestation in agreement with another study on great tits (Heylen and Matthysen 2008; but see Heylen et al. 2013). The lack of an effect of bird age could stem from within- versus among-individual age effects having opposite signs (van de Pol and Wright 2009). To test if this was the case, we split age into among- and within-individual components (Supplemental Text 1). This level-specific analysis also failed to show effects of these factors (Supplemental Text 1) supporting the absence of an age effect in our data.

Other research has argued that tick burden positively correlates to body condition (Mysterud et al. 2015). However, correlations between parasite burden and body condition occur in some but not all species, and are thus most likely not universal (Kiffner et al. 2013). How body condition of great tits relates to their tick infestation remains to be determined (Heylen and Matthysen 2008; Heylen et al. 2013). Importantly, body condition and exploration behaviour are (sometimes) correlated (Dosmann et al. 2015; Moiron et al. 2019) but effects of exploration behaviour and body condition on tick burden have not been disentangled. Previously, no relationship had been found between tick infestation and body condition in wild great tits (Heylen and Matthysen 2008; Heylen et al. 2013). Our study only partially confirms this result as body condition did not influence infestation probability, but tick burden of infested birds was negatively correlated to body condition. This negative relationship between body condition and tick burden has been observed in other species, and has been attributed to health reductions due to blood loss (Norte et al. 2013). However, the direction of the effect remains unclear, perhaps because multiple factors are involved. High tick burdens could in fact lower body condition through blood loss (Norte et al. 2013) but, additionally, birds with lower body condition could be more susceptible to tick infestation, and consequently, have higher tick burdens (Norte et al. 2010, 2013). More research is therefore needed to disentangle the pathways by which these patterns are generated.

Individuals were not repeatable in infestation probability, but tick burden was repeatable within infested individuals. This means that phenotypic traits other than those measured here (i.e. exploratory behaviour and body condition) explain tick burdens of infested birds. For instance, grooming behaviour has been shown to shape tick burdens in mammalian species (Keesing et al. 2009) and appears to influence tick infestation patterns in great tits (Fracasso et al. 2019). Additional behaviours such as aggression or foraging behaviour (see above) may shape tick infestation (Newman et al. 2015; Loss et al. 2016; Kocianová et al. 2017; Zohdy et al. 2017). Our inability to demonstrate that infestation probability was repeatable may also stem a lack of statistical power, a common problem in repeatability analyses of binary traits.

Spatial factors, including nest location, influenced tick infestation. Great tits might choose their breeding site based on several ecological factors, such as resource availability, conspecific density (Spiegel et al. 2015, 2017; Sih et al. 2018), or human disturbance (Hutfluss and Dingemanse 2019). As ticks are heterogeneously distributed over the breeding habitat (Hillyard 1996; Perez et al. 2016), factors influencing great tit habitat choice may also affect tick infestation. Tick abundance is normally affected by micro-spatial differences in, for example, relative humidity (Lindgren et al. 2000; Oorebeek and Kleindorfer 2008; Kiffner et al. 2011), vegetation type or structure (Kiffner et al. 2011; Tack et al. 2012a, b, 2013), or host availability (i.e. rodents, roe deer, etc.) (Oorebeek and Kleindorfer 2008; Kiffner et al. 2011; Takumi et al. 2019). Importantly, our birds were captured approximately over a 1-month period (range: 25–29 days), mostly in May each year, which corresponds to the peak of tick abundance (Kurtenbach et al. 2006). Seasonality, such as in weather conditions (e.g. relative humidity), can influence overall tick abundance (Mejlon and Jaenson 1997; Perez et al. 2016) and thus tick burden (Heylen et al. 2013). High host (bird) population density might push some individuals to occupy habitats more suitable for ticks, e.g. more humid habitats with denser vegetation (Tack et al. 2012a, b), and thereby influence tick infestation. This was not the case here as tick infestation did not vary with breeding density. The absence of this effect could be due to confounding effects at different levels of biological variation (van de Pol and Wright 2009). We therefore split variation in great tit breeding density into temporal and spatial components and tested if this mattered, which was not the case (Supplemental Text 1). As great tits are site faithful (Harvey et al. 1979) and nest box identity predicts tick infestation, future work should consider why individuals nest or potentially do not nest in habitats with high tick abundances. For this, fine-scale data on questing tick densities near occupied nest boxes, and information on ecological variables (i.e. vegetation type, humidity, other host species), would be required. Furthermore, as the average tick burden for any given host will be modified based on the ratio of competent hosts and tick abundance in a given geographic area (Randolph 1998, 2008; Brunner and Ostfeld 2008), the presence of other suitable vertebrate hosts (i.e. rodents and roe deer Capreolus capreolus) may have influenced tick infestation for any particular host species. Testing for interacting effects of multiple host species on tick infestation in great tits may therefore represent a promising avenue for future research.

In conclusion, we showed covariance between exploration behaviour of great tits and their tick infestation probability, with faster explorers having increased infestation probabilities. For the subpopulation of infested birds, however, fast explorers did not carry a larger tick burden among infested birds. Our study thereby supports previous research demonstrating effects of animal personality on susceptibility to tick parasitism (Sih et al. 2018). Tick burden was not influenced by sex or age, but negatively correlated with body condition. Infested individuals were, furthermore, repeatable in tick burden, implying that phenotypic traits other than those considered here explain repeatable differences in tick burden of infested individuals. Finally, recreation pressure tended to predict infestation probability. Our results highlight the complex nature of the parasitic relationship between birds and ticks and the important moderating effect of phenotypic traits specific to individual hosts.