Skip to main content
Log in

Experimental evidence of specialised phenotypic roles in a mobbing raptor

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Group living is associated with costs but also with potential benefits, such as a decrease in predation risk through, for example, higher defence efficiency. Mobbing is among the most specialised forms of anti-predator strategies involving group defence and has mainly been investigated in passerine birds and some mammals. Variation in the mobbing response has been found in several species according to phenotypic variation such as sex or age. This suggests that there are differential benefits between mobbers, which may have promoted individual specialisation in mobbing behaviour. We studied mobbing behaviour in a communal roosting raptor, the Marsh harrier (Circus aeruginosus), which shows active group defence. Our study population exhibits extreme colour polymorphism, with two colour morphs in males, as well as sexual dichromatism and colour variation with age. We used different decoys, placed at different distances from the roost, to manipulate experimentally the perceived predation risk and to elicit mobbing behaviour. Using the experimental design that maximised mobbing response in harriers, we then focused on the sequence and the specific behaviours involved in recruitment of mobbers, and whether individual investment in terms of defence was associated with phenotypic characteristics of individuals (i.e. sex, age and colour morph). We found that the main behaviour involved in successfully attracting mobbers was alarm calling. We also detected differential individual investment in relation to sex and age, but more importantly, we provide the first evidence for specialised male phenotypic roles during mobbing events, signalled by colour polymorphism: grey males tended not to be involved in mobbing and almost never behaved as recruiters or mobbers, while brown males behaved mainly as recruiter birds. These findings suggest that colour morph may signal the individual’s anti-predatory abilities through different behavioural strategies between males.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alexander RD (1974) The evolution of social behaviour. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 5:325–383

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Altmann SA (1956) Avian mobbing behavior and predator recognition. Condor 58:241–253

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arroyo B, Mougeot F, Bretagnolle V (2001) Colonial breeding and nest defence in Montagu’s harrier (Circus pygargus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 50:109–115

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bavoux C, Burneleau G, Cuisin J, Nicolau-Guillaumet P (1991) Le Busard des roseaux Circus aeruginosus en Charente-Maritime (France). IV. Variabilité du plumage juvénile. Alauda 59:248–255

    Google Scholar 

  • Bavoux C, Burneleau G, Nicolau-Guillaumet P, Picard M (1993) Le Busard des roseaux Circus aeruginosus en Charente-Maritime (France). VI. Couleur de l’iris, sexe et âge. Alauda 61:173–179

    Google Scholar 

  • Bavoux C, Burneleau G, Picard M (1997) Le gîte nocturne du Busard des roseaux Circus aeruginosus. Modalités de fréquentation en Charente-Maritime (France). Alauda 65:337–342

    Google Scholar 

  • Bavoux C, Burneleau G, Bretagnolle V (2006) Sexing European marsh harrier Circus aeruginosus using morphometrics and discriminant analyses. J Rapt Res 40:57–64

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradbury JW, Vehrencamp SL (1998) Principles of animal communication. Sinauer, Sunderland

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown GE (2003) Learning about danger: chemical alarm cues and local risk assessment in prey fishes. Fish Fish 4:227–234

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Caro TM (2005) Antipredator defences in birds and mammals. Chicago University Press, Chicago

  • Charnov EL, Krebs JR (1975) The evolution of alarm calls: altruism or manipulation? Am Nat 109:107–112

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collias NE, Collias EC (1978) Group territory, dominance, co-operative breeding in birds, and a new factor. Anim Behav 26:308–309

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conover MR (1987) Acquisition of predator information by active and passive mobbers in ringed-billed gull colonies. Behaviour 102:41–57

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cramp S, Simmons KEL (1980) The birds of the western Paleartic, volume 2. Oxford University Press, Oxford

  • Curio E (1978) The adaptive significance of avian mobbing I. Telenomic hypothesis and predictions. Z Tierpsychol 48:175–183

    Google Scholar 

  • Curio E, Klump G, Regelmann K (1983) An anti-predator response in the great tit (Parus major): is it tuned to the predator risk? Oecologia 60:83–88

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curio E, Regelmann K, Zimmermann U (1985) Brood defence in the great tit (Parus major): the influence of the life-history and habitat. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 16:273–283

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Desrochers A, Bélisle M, Bourque J (2002) Do mobbing calls affect the perception of predation risk by forest birds? Anim Behav 64:709–714

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ducrest AL, Keller L, Roulin A (2008) Pleiotropy in the melanocortin system, coloration and behavioural syndromes. Trends Ecol Evol 23:502–510

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dugatkin LA, Godin JGJ (1992) Prey approaching predators: a cost–benefit perspective. Ann Zool Fenn 29:233–252

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis JMS (2008) Which call parameters signal threat to conspecifics in white-throated magpie–jay mobbing calls? Ethology 114:154–163

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fargallo JA, Laaksonen T, Korpimäki E, Wakamatsu A (2007) Melanin-based trait reflects environmental growth conditions of nestling male Eurasian kestrels. Evol Ecol 21:157–71

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson-Lees J, Christie DA (2001) Raptors of the world. Christopher Helm, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Forsman D (2008) The raptors of Europe and the Middle East. Princeton University Press, Princeton

  • Francis AM, Hailman JP, Woolfenden GE (1989) Mobbing by Florida scrub jays: behaviour, sexual asymmetry, role of helpers and ontogeny. Anim Behav 38:795–816

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frankenberg E (1981) The adaptive significance of avian mobbing. IV. “Alerting others” and “Perception advertisement” in blackbirds facing owl. Z Tierpsychol 55:97–118

    Google Scholar 

  • Graw B, Manser MB (2007) The function of mobbing in cooperative meerkats. Anim Behav 74:507–517

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griesser M (2008) Referential calls signal predator behaviour in a group-living bird species. Curr Biol 18:69–73

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griesser M (2009) Mobbing calls signal predator category in a kin group-living bird species. Proc R Soc B 276:2887–2892

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griesser M, Ekman J (2005) Nepotistic mobbing behaviour in the Siberian jay, Perisoreus infaustus. Anim Behav 69:345–352

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haase E, Ito S, Sell A, Wakamatsu K (1992) Melanin concentrations in feathers from wild and domestic pigeons. J Hered 83:64–67

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hass CC, Valenzuela D (2002) Anti-predator benefits of group living in white-nosed coatis (Nasua narica). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 51:570–578

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hasson O (1991) Pursuit deterrent signals—communication between prey and predator. Trends Ecol Evol 6:325–329

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helfman GS (1989) Threat-sensitive predator avoidance in damselfish–trumpetfish interactions. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 24:47–58

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill WL (1991) Correlates of male mating success in the ruff Philomachus pugnax, a lekking shorebird. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 29:367–372

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill RA, Dunbar RIM (1998) An evaluation of the roles of predation rate and predation risk as selective pressures on primate grouping behaviour. Behaviour 135:411–430

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurd CR (1996) Interspecific attraction to the mobbing calls of blackcapped chickadees (Parus atricapillus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38:287–292

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jawor JM, Breitwisch R (2003) Melanin ornaments, honesty, and sexual selection. Auk 120:249–265

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones K, Hill WL (2001) Auditory perception of hawks and owls for passerine alarm calls. Ethology 107:717–726

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knight RL, Temple SA (1988) Nest-defense behavior in the red-winged blackbird. Condor 90:193–200

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krams I, Krama T, Igaune K, Mand R (2007) Long-lasting mobbing of the pied flycatcher increases the risk of nest predation. Behav Ecol 18:1082–1084

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krams I, Krama T, Igaune K, Maend R (2008) Experimental evidence of reciprocal altruism in the pied flycatcher. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62:599–605

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lank DB, Smith CM, Hanotte O, Burke T, Cooke F (1995) Genetic polymorphism for alternative mating behaviour in lekking male ruff Philomachus pugnax. Nature 378:59–62

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lima SL (2009) Predators and the breeding bird: behavioural and reproductive flexibility under the risk of predation. Biol Rev 84:485–513

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lima SL, Dill LM (1990) Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Can J Zool 68:619–640

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maklakov AA (2002) Snake-directed mobbing in a cooperative breeder: anti-predator behaviour or self-advertisement for the formation of dispersal coalitions? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 52:372–378

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marler P (1957) Specific distinctiveness in the communication signals of birds. Behaviour 11:13–39

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGraw KJ, Safran RJ, Wakamatsu K (2005) How feather colour reflects its melanin content. Funct Ecol 19:816–821

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moholt RK, Trost C (1989) Self-advertisement: relations to dominance in black-billed magpies. Anim Behav 38:1079–1088

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan C, Thomas RE, Ma W, Novotny MV, Cone RD (2004) Melanocortin-5 receptor deficiency reduces a pheromonal signal for aggression in male mice. Chem Senses 29:111–115

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Negro JJ, Bortolotti GR, Sarasola JH (2007) Deceptive plumage signals in birds: manipulation of predators or prey? Biol J Linn Soc 90:467–477

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ostreiher R (2003) Is mobbing altruistic or selfish behaviour? Anim Behav 66:145–149

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Owings DH, Coss RG (1977) Snake mobbing by California ground squirrels—adaptive variation and ontogeny. Behaviour 62:50–69

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roulin A (2004) The evolution, maintenance and adaptive function of genetic colour polymorphism in birds. Biol Rev 79:1–34

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruxton GD, Sherratt TN, Speed MP (2004) Avoiding attack: the evolutionary ecology of crypsis, warning signals and mimicry. New York University Press, New York

  • Institute SAS (2001) SAS user's guide. SAS Institute, Cary

    Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL, Marler P (1980) Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. Science 210:801–803

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simmons RE (2000) Harriers of the world. Their behaviour and ecology. Oxford University Press, Oxford

  • Slagsvold T (1985) Mobbing behaviour of the hooded crow Corvus corone corvix in relation to age, sex, size, season, temperature and kind of enemy. Fauna Norvegica Ser C Cin 8:9–17

    Google Scholar 

  • Srivastava A (1991) Cultural transmission of snake-mobbing in free-ranging Hanuman langurs. Folia Primatol 56:117–120

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sternalski A, Bavoux C, Burneleau G, Bretagnolle V (2008) Philopatry, natal and postnatal dispersal in a sedentary population of Marsh harriers Circus aeruginosus. J Zool 274:188–197

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Templeton CN, Greene E, Davis K (2005) Allometry of alarm calls: black-capped chickadees encode information about predator size. Science 308:1934–1937

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tuttle EM (2003) Alternative reproductive strategies in the white-throated sparrow: behavioral and genetic evidence. Behav Ecol 14:425–432

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welbergen JA, Davies NB (2008) Reed warblers discriminate cuckoos from sparrowhawks with graded alarm signals that attract mates and neighbours. Anim Behav 76:811–822

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson EO (1980) Sociobiology. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolff GL (2003) Regulation of yellow pigment formation in mice: a historical perspective. Pig Cell Res 16:2–15

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Woodland DJ, Jaafar Z, Knight ML (1980) The “pursuit deterrent” function of alarm signals. Am Nat 115:748–753

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright J (1997) Helping-at-nest in Arabian babblers: signalling social status or sensible investment in chicks? Anim Behav 54:1439–1448

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi A (1995) Altruism as a handicap—limitations of kin selection and reciprocity. J Avian Biol 26:1–3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi A, Zahavi A (1997) The handicap principle. Oxford University Press, Oxford

Download references

Acknowledgements

Particular thanks are due to F. Picaud and W. Huin for their participation in field work and to C. Bavoux and several trainees and volunteers of Le Marais aux Oiseaux for their dedicated help during communal roost counts. We are grateful to David Carslake for commenting and improving the English of a first draft. We thank Beatriz Arroyo, Alexandre Roulin and in particular Gary Bortolotti for their very useful comments on a previous draft. We also wish to thank Mickael Griesser for his very helpful comments and insights on our manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Audrey Sternalski.

Additional information

Communicated by E. Korpimäki

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Sternalski, A., Bretagnolle, V. Experimental evidence of specialised phenotypic roles in a mobbing raptor. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 64, 1351–1361 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-010-0950-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-010-0950-z

Keywords

Navigation