Introduction

Studying seabird foraging ecology is important not only because it explains their extreme life histories (Dobson and Jouventin 2007), but also to understand their key role as ecosystem engineers (Graham et al. 2018; Otero et al. 2018), and their value as bioindicators of prey abundance and marine productivity (Bost et al. 2008; Velarde et al. 2013). However, seabird populations are under intense pressure on land and at-sea, making them one of the most threatened groups of birds worldwide (Croxall et al. 2012; Dias et al. 2019).

The establishment of Marine Protected Areas (MPAs) and Birdlife International’s Important Bird and Biodiversity Areas (IBAs) have become two primary methods for the conservation of seabird foraging habitats. Studies on seabird distributions, foraging behaviour, and habitat use are common approaches for identifying potential MPA locations (Grecian et al. 2012; Lascelles et al. 2012; Le Corre et al. 2012; Thaxter et al. 2012). The creation of large-scale MPAs has surged in recent years with the aim to protect wide-ranging species, including seabirds, but are still constrained in their effectiveness due to various management challenges (O’Leary et al. 2018). Advances in seabird tracking technologies have promoted greater and more comprehensive scientific understanding of species behaviours at sea, which has in turn aided in both the establishment of MPAs (Wakefield et al. 2009; Hays et al. 2019) and the evaluation of their effectiveness (Harris et al. 2007). These behaviours, which are informed by measures such as trip duration and distance, dive depth, and habitat use, are broadly referred to as foraging strategies. Furthermore, data collected from tracking devices can reveal utilisation distributions, indicating the probability density of an animal’s location at any given time (Powell and Mitchell 2012). Nevertheless, both foraging strategies and utilisation distributions can vary considerably within and among species, making the identification of foraging hotspots for conservation prioritisation, including MPA designation, more challenging.

Behavioural variation within and among species is central to ecology, and has implications for scaling of conservation measures (Brakes and Dall 2016; Cleasby et al. 2023). For example, for breeding seabirds, central-place foraging strategies can scale among colonies at local scales (Warwick-Evans et al. 2018). However, variability in behaviour is common both within- (Votier et al. 2017; Trevail et al. 2021) and among colonies at regional scales (> 200 km; Mannocci et al. 2017; Miller et al. 2023; Cleasby et al. 2023). Divergent foraging strategies often arise through local adaptation to environmental conditions such as wind (Raymond et al. 2010; Nourani et al. 2023), and oceanographic features such as bathymetry, sea surface temperature, and heterogeneity (e.g., Weimerskirch et al. 2005; Waggitt et al. 2018; Trevail et al. 2019), and can therefore vary both spatially and temporally (Mendez et al. 2017). Lastly, foraging strategies are dependent on interactions with other marine foragers; in tropical regions, sub-surface predators drive prey closer to the surface in reach of flying predators (Au and Pitman 1986; Vaughn et al. 2008; Correia et al. 2019), meanwhile resource competition can drive segregation within and among species (Almeida et al. 2021; Trevail et al. 2023). These sources of variation in individual and population behaviour could limit the ability to infer priority sites for at-sea protection from limited tracking data (Soanes et al. 2013). As such, understanding foraging strategies among populations is a key step for maximising use of existing tracking data and guiding future studies in conservation and management.

The Chagos Archipelago is a group of five above-surface atolls and home to some of the world’s most pristine coral reefs (Sheppard et al. 2012). It was declared a no-take MPA in April 2010, covering circa 640,000 km2 of the Western Indian Ocean. The MPA was established to protect marine biodiversity, including seabirds, their breeding and foraging habitats (Sheppard et al. 2012). However, because of its remoteness, access limitations, and expanse, seabird research has been limited. The brown booby, Sula leucogaster, is a pantropical oceanic species and one of the 18 breeding seabird species found in the Chagos Archipelago (Carr et al. 2021b). Brown boobies’ diet mostly consists of pelagic fish (Harrison et al. 1984; Mellink et al. 2001) but can be variable in response to environmental conditions (Castillo-Guerrero et al. 2016). Foraging trip duration for the species is short compared to more pelagic sulid species (Lewis et al. 2004; Weimerskirch et al. 2009; Soanes et al. 2015). Furthermore, there is evidence of sex differences in foraging behaviour of brown boobies, larger females often undertake longer foraging trips and can forage at different trophic levels (Lewis et al. 2005; Weimerskirch et al. 2009; Young et al. 2010; Miller et al. 2018). Foraging areas may also vary depending on season and breeding stage; seabirds typically travel further during incubation (Soanes et al. 2015). Whilst previous studies have highlighted how foraging strategies vary for the species within colonies (Weimerskirch et al. 2009; Miller et al. 2018; Austin et al. 2021) and when compared with colonies in different oceans (Soanes et al. 2015, 2016; Correia et al. 2021), there is limited evidence for foraging variability among colonies within in the same archipelago or atoll (Soanes et al. 2016). Understanding inter-colony variations in the same geographic areas can be used to better address the establishment and assess the effectiveness of MPAs and IBAs.

This study aims to characterise brown booby foraging strategies for colonies located in the same archipelago. Breeding brown boobies from two islands situated 142 km apart on the Great Chagos Bank atoll, in the Chagos Archipelago, were fitted with GPS loggers to document foraging trip metrics and at-sea behaviours, and explore foraging habitat use. Because of logistical constraints on fieldwork, birds were tracked during different years, monsoon seasons, and breeding stages. Because these differences are known to influence foraging strategies, inter-colony variability is expected.

Materials and methods

Study site

Fieldwork was conducted on Nelson’s Island (72.3126° E, 5.6815° S) in July 2018 and Danger Island (71.2406° E, 6.3869° S) in January 2019 (Table 1). These two remote, uninhabited islands are respectively situated in the North and West of the Great Chagos Bank, the largest living coral atoll in the world (Sheppard et al. 2012). Brown boobies breed at three major colonies in the archipelago; North Brother (max 740 pairs), Danger Island (max 141 pairs, 35 recorded during study period), and Nelson’s Island (max 35 pairs, 12 during study period) (Carr et al. 2021b). Elsewhere in the Chagos Archipelago, colonies comprising fewer than 8 breeding pairs have been recorded across 5 islands (Carr et al. 2021b). Danger Island and Nelson’s Island are therefore important within the archipelago, although populations are not large enough meet IBA qualifying criteria for regional significance (Carr et al. 2021b). Coordinates of the Chagos Archipelago MPA were downloaded from the World Database on Protected Areas, managed by UNEP World Conservation Monitoring Centre (available at ProtectedPlanet.net).

Table 1 Study location and number of GPS loggers deployed and retrieved for brown boobies

GPS deployment and retrieval

To study at-sea foraging behaviour, 21 breeding adult brown boobies were fitted with a unique British Trust of Ornithology (BTO) metal leg ring, livestock marker to aid recapture, an archival GPS logger (15 g, IGotU GT-120, Mobile Action Technology Inc) attached to the tail with tesa tape, and a leg-mounted light level geolocator (3 g, Intigeo C330, Migrate Technology—data not used in this study). Handling time per individual was approximately 5–6 min. Together, GPS loggers and geolocators weighed < 3% of the individual’s body mass and there were no detectable effects of device deployment and recovery on breeding success during each expedition. On both Danger and Nelson’s Island we monitored all brown booby nests where tags were deployed, and a selection of nests where no adults were caught, and at the point when we left each island none of the nests were observed to have failed.

GPS loggers were programmed to obtain fixes every 5 min (24 h a day) and deployed for between 3–8 days. The stage of breeding and nest GPS locations for the tagged individuals were recorded, and when possible, the bird’s sex was identified by morphological differences (Nelson 1978).

Data processing

First, downloaded GPS data were trimmed to incorporate tag deployment and retrieval periods only. A 300 m buffer was then created around the islands and used to exclude short trips, typically for bathing or rafting (Bennison et al. 2018; Trevail et al. 2019). Complete trips were defined as journeys starting and finishing within 300 m of the colony including a minimum of four fixes.

To evaluate the GPS logger performance, GPS trip fix success rate was statistically analysed using a Wilcoxon Signed-Rank Test as observed and expected fix success rate data were non-normal. Despite there being a difference between observed and expected fix success rate (P < 0.05), a frequency histogram (available in the Supplementary Material, Fig. S1) revealed that the majority of trips had only one or zero missing locations, and only three trips contained recording gaps of over an hour of data. Because of this preliminary analysis, and experimental evidence suggesting high performance of this GPS logger (Morris and Conner 2017), performance of loggers was deemed suitable for further analyses. All statistical analyses were conducted in R 4.0.0 (R Core Team 2016).

Foraging trip metrics

GPS fixes from complete trips were used to estimate total trip duration (mins), total trip distance (km), and maximum distance travelled (km), using the R package trip (Sumner et al. 2009).

To test for differences in foraging trips between colonies, trip metrics were square-root transformed to meet assumptions of normality (Young et al. 2015). Data distributions before and after transformation supported use of transformed data for colony comparisons (Supplementary material Fig. S2). Because of the hierarchical nature of the data, inter-colony variations in trip metrics were assessed with ‘individual’ (i.e. bird ID) as a random effect in a linear mixed effect model (LMM) using the function lmer in R package lme4 (Bates et al. 2015). Differences between colonies were assessed by removing colony from the model using the function drop1 in lme4. For further data exploration, correlations between trip metrics were assessed using a Spearman’s rank correlation, selected after running a Shapiro–Wilk test that indicated a non-normal distribution in trip metric data.

At-sea behaviours

At-sea movement behaviour was quantified using a Hidden Markov Model (HMM) in R package moveHMM (Michelot et al. 2016). This time series model is suitable for making inferences on animal movement behaviour through a non-observable state sequence derived from an observable series (Langrock et al. 2012). The model calculates changes in step length (i.e. distance) and turning angle between GPS points to annotate trajectories by behavioural state (Michelot et al. 2016). Firstly, GPS data were linearly interpolated to every 5 min using default parameters in the R package adehabitat (Calenge 2017); this step accounted for missing locations associated with a GPS fix success rate of < 100% and allowed these data to be run using an HMM on equally spaced locations.

The HMM with three states fitted these data better than a two-state mode; (based on Akaike Information Criteria, AIC, Supplementary material Table S1), as is the case for other booby species (Boyd et al. 2014; Oppel et al. 2015; Miller et al. 2018). The Viterbi algorithm was used to estimate the most likely sequence of movement states. The HMM revealed three distinct underlying behavioural states (Fig. S6). A short step length and narrow turning angle (step: 0.07 ± 0.04 km; turn: μ = 0.02, κ = 38) corresponded to a slow-moving state, described in the literature as a ‘resting’ state (Boyd et al. 2014; Miller et al. 2018). A greater step length and the widest turning angle (step: 0.9 ± 0.8 km; turn: μ =− 0.004, κ = 1.5) revealed a ‘foraging’ behaviour. Finally, a ‘transiting’ state was matched to a much greater step length, and the narrowest turning angle (step: 3.2 ± 0.6 km; turn: μ = − 0.01, κ = 19). Transiting would correspond to a relatively straight section of the trajectory in which GPS points are spaced out by an average of 3.2 km (Fig. S6), corresponding to a flight speed of roughly 38 km/h. Visual examination of the HMM output revealed that the proportion of time spent foraging was 100% in some of the shortest trips in distance, hence a Spearman’s rank correlation was run to further investigate a relationship between maximum trip distance from the colony and proportion of time spent in each state. The analyses for this, found in Supplementary Material Fig. S3, suggested that the output for the HMM could be used to examine foraging behaviour of the tracked individuals. To understand inter-colony differences in behavioural states, binomial GLMMs were run on the proportion of time spent in each behaviour, with ‘trip’ and ‘individual’ as nested random effects.

Habitat selection

To understand how brown booby occurrence and foraging locations were linked to environment, we used habitat-selection models based on available locations (i.e., all locations within each individuals’ home range) and known foraging locations determined by the HMM (i.e., locations that were documented by tracking devices) to test if tracked animals show preference towards particular habitats within their foraging range. To sample the available environment, a circle polygon with a radius of the furthest trip from the respective colony was drawn around the two colonies (i.e. 111.5 km around Danger Island and 67 km around Nelson’s Island). Ten GPS points were randomly generated for every foraging GPS point (as indicated by the HMM) in the tracking data within this radius using the function spsample in R package sp (Trevail et al. 2019).

We sampled static and dynamic environmental variables at all used and available points that could influence the availability and accessibility of foraging opportunities for brown boobies; (1) bathymetry, (2) bathymetric slope, (3) sea surface temperature (SST), (4) chlorophyll, (5) sea-level height anomaly (SLA), (6) wind speed, (7) wind direction, and (8) distance to the colony. NOAA ETOPO1 bathymetry (1-arc minute resolution) was downloaded using package marmap (Pante et al. 2023). Slope was obtained from bathymetry using function “terrain” in the R package raster (Hijmans and Etten 2018). Dynamic variables were downloaded using the erddap interface from NOAA oceanwatch (https://oceanwatch.pifsc.noaa.gov/erddap/): sea surface temperature (NOAA geopolar blended—monthly), chlorophyll a concentration (ESA OC CCI—monthly), sea surface height anomalies and geostrophic currents (NOAA/NESDIS/STAR), and ocean surface winds (CCMP, daily). Distance from colony was obtained using function “distGeo” in package geosphere (Hijmans et al. 2022).

For each colony, we ran habitat selection models using the biomod2 package (Thuiller et al. 2009) to predict presence/absence based on environmental variables. To avoid the idiosyncratic effect of different model types we constructed ensemble models comprising random forests (RF), generalised linear models (GLM), generalised additive models (GAM), maximum entropy (MAXENT), boosted regression trees (GBM), and artificial neural networks (ANN). Model performance was evaluated on tenfold cross validation using relative operating characteristic (ROC) and true skills statistic (TSS) scores, where values closer to 1 indicate improved model discrimination between presence and absences (ROC) and presence prediction (TSS). Ensemble performance was evaluated using an 80/20 split. Ensemble predictions used the weighted sum of probabilities (EMwmeanByTSS). We assessed variable importance and made spatial predictions using this ensemble model.

Results

GPS data

Tag recovery success was 73% on Danger Island (11 loggers) and 100% on Nelson’s Island (6 loggers, Table 1), yielding tracking data for nine birds on Danger Island and four on Nelson’s Island (Table 2; battery failure occurred on the remaining loggers). On Danger Island, data were from three males and six females. On Nelson’s Island, data were from two males, and two birds of unknown sex. Because of low sample sizes, no formal comparisons were made between foraging strategies of different sexes. All tracks were located within the Chagos Archipelago MPA (Fig. 1). Individuals from both colonies travelled away from the central Great Chagos Bank into surrounding, deeper waters (Fig. 1).

Table 2 Summary of tracking data and trip metrics obtained for brown boobies with GPS loggers. Trip metrics were rounded to one decimal place
Fig. 1
figure 1

Trips (n = 74) performed by GPS tagged Brown boobies (n = 13) from two separate colonies (triangles). (a) Trips are coloured by individual and overlaid on bathymetry map. (b) Location of the Chagos Archipelago in the Indian Ocean with MPA boundary shown in green. (c) Tracks from both colonies occur close to the islands, well within the Chagos Archipelago MPA

Foraging trip metrics

We analysed 74 trips from the 13 individuals (Table 2), compising 2–9 trips over 3–8 days per individual. In general, birds departed for one long foraging trip per day or multiple shorter ones, and trips took place during daytime. Across both colonies, trips ranged from 0.3–12.7 h duration, covered a total distance of 2.3–252.8 km, and reached a maximum of 1.1–111.5 km from the colony. On Danger Island, foraging trips lasted, on average, 3.8 ± 0.4 h, reached a maximum of 35.6 ± 3.8 km from the colony, and covered a total distance of 88.0 ± 9.0 km. On Nelson’s Island, foraging trips lasted 3.7 ± 0.5 h, reached a maximum of 23.9 ± 3.7 km from the colony, and covered a total distance of 63.0 ± 9.0 km. There were strong positive correlations between all trip metrics (Figure S4). Trip duration for both sites combined was positively correlated with total trip distance (rs = 0.899, P < 0.001, n = 74) and maximum distance travelled from the nest (rs = 0.829, P < 0.001). Total trip distance and maximum distance travelled were also positively correlated (rs = 0.973, P < 0.001). Although trip metrics were marginally shorter on Nelson’s Island than on Danger Island (Table 2), there was no significant inter-colony variation; trip duration (F1 = 0.08, P = 0.78), total distance (F1 = 1.09, P = 0.31), maximum distance (F1 = 1.93, P = 0.19) (Fig. 2). Trip metrics are presented for individuals by sex in the Supplementary Material (Fig. S5).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Box-and-whisker plots comparing the three trip metrics; trip duration, maximum distance and total distance between the two colonies on Danger Island (DI) and Nelson’s Island (NI), for which we found no significant differences

At-sea behaviour

On average, tracked individuals (colonies pooled) spent 57% of the time foraging, 38% of the time transiting, and 5% of the time resting. All deconstructed tracks for both colonies and a close-up individual example are respectively found in the Supplementary Material (Figure S7 & S8). Tracked individuals at Nelson’s Island spent a significantly greater proportion of time foraging (GLMM, χ2(1) = 15.4, P < 0.001) and a lower proportion of time transiting (χ2(1) = 488, P < 0.001), whereas there was no statistical difference in the proportion of time resting (χ2(1) = 150, P = 0.53) between colonies (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Colony comparison box-and-whisker plot of proportion of time spent in three behavioural states by GPS tracked brown boobies; forage, transit, rest. Behavioural states that differed significantly between colonies (P < 0.001) are indicated with three asterisks (***)

Habitat selection

Individual habitat selection models had high performance (cross-validation AUC > 0.85, TSS generally > 0.7 at both colonies; Figure S9). For predictions, we formed ensemble models combining all model types at each colony, with high AUCs (0.98 for Danger Island, 0.985 for Nelson’s Island).

Variable importance assessment (Fig. S10) highlighted that distance to colony was a strong predictor at both colonies, with individuals preferring habitat closer to their colony (Fig. 4) relative to the overall environment available within their foraging range. Sea-level height anomaly and sea-surface temperature were also important at both colonies but with different ranges likely associated with different at-sea conditions (Fig. 4). At both colonies individuals also preferred habitat with steep slopes (Fig. 4) along the shelf edge. Other variables differed in their importance at each colony. Wind-speed was a useful predictor for birds from Danger Island (perhaps as wind speed was consistently higher, Fig. 4); chlorophyll-a and bathymetric depth were important predictors at Nelson (with individuals preferring shallower, less productive waters, Fig. 4). Based on these habitat preferences, we predicted brown boobies from the two tracked colonies to forage nearby to their colonies, in areas extending outwards into deeper waters, away from the central Great Chagos Bank (Fig. 5).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Probability of habitat use along environmental gradients for GPS tracked brown boobies from Danger Island (pink) and Nelson’s Island (blue), based on ensemble models (error bars show confidence intervals based on 100 ensemble iterations). Birds at each colony experienced different ranges of dynamic variables, but all preferentially foraged closer to the colony and at steeper bathymetric slopes

Fig. 5
figure 5

Predicted occurrence of central place foraging brown boobies from Danger Island (left) and Nelson’s Island (right) during tracking periods across the Chagos Archipelago, based on habitat preferences. Islands are denoted by black outlines

Discussion

Our results reveal similar patterns of at-sea behaviour between two segregated colonies of brown boobies in the Chagos Archipelago. Despite differences in colony location, monsoon period, year, and breeding stage during tracking, trip metrics did not differ significantly between colonies. Foraging similarities could arise because of shared environmental preferences for shelf edge habitats close to the colony. Both colonies foraged in distinct at-sea areas inside the MPA.

Brown boobies used comparable foraging strategies at both tracked colonies in the Chagos Archipelago with similar trip lengths of < 12.7 h duration (Fig. 2), foraged at distal locations (Figure S7) up to a maximum of 111.5 km from the colony, and spent very little time resting at sea (Fig. 3). Nevertheless, we note that the number of tracked individuals is quite small (DI n = 9, NI n = 4) and while similarities should be treated with caution, all tracked birds had more than one trip (Table 1). Trip durations and distances were similar to those reported for this species in other oceans (Table 3), further highlighting that brown boobies generally forage in neritic zones and along coastal shelf edges (Weimerskirch et al. 2009; Young et al. 2015; Miller et al. 2018; Correia et al. 2021; Austin et al. 2021). However, given that temporal changes in the environment can influence foraging strategies (Soanes et al. 2015), and seabirds often forage further from the colony during incubation (Sommerfeld and Hennicke 2010), it is interesting that variability in foraging effort was not observed between study colonies, perhaps because of shared environmental preferences.

Table 3 Comparison of trip metrics for breeding brown booby colonies in the Pacific Ocean, Gulf of California, Caribbean Sea from prior studies, and in Indian Ocean from this study

Brown boobies are one of three sulids breeding in the Chagos Archipelago, alongside red-footed boobies, Sula sula (total 22,871 pairs), and masked boobies, Sula dactylatra (total 164 pairs) (Carr et al. 2021b). Brown boobies forage much closer to the islands than the smaller, more pelagic red-footed boobies (mean maximum distance: 112.9 ± 3.7 in Chagos Archipelago, (Trevail et al. 2023), that are resident in the archipelago year-round (Votier et al. 2024). Masked booby at-sea distributions in the Chagos Archipelago are unknown, however population sizes are smaller, so intraspecific competition is less likely.

The habitat selection analyses demonstrated that distance to the colony was the most important environmental predictor across both populations, followed by sea-level height anomaly and sea-surface temperature. Although the range of these dynamic variables differed across the two tracking periods, birds at both colonies foraged in areas with higher sea-level height anomaly than available, indicative of eddy circulation and potential nutrient entrainment (Hyrenbach et al. 2006). Sea-surface temperature was also an important driver of habitat selection at both colonies, with birds at Nelson’s Island selecting intermediate temperatures compared to those available between 27.5 and 28 °C, and birds at Danger Island selecting warmer waters > 29.5 °C (Fig. 4). Both of these temperature ranges coincide with favourable feeding habitats of skipjack tuna (Druon et al. 2017), so could suggest preference for facultative feeding opportunities, although further investigation into foraging modality is necessary (Dunn et al. 2024). At both colonies, individuals foraged over steeper slopes, away from the central Great Chagos Bank, similar to brown boobies in the Great Barrier Reef that also forage on the reef edge (Miller et al. 2018). Bathymetric slopes are associated with greater productivity as currents and topography can drive prey closer to the surface (Piatt et al. 2006; Reisinger et al. 2018), thereby attracting foraging seabirds (Piatt et al. 2006; McDuie et al. 2018; De Pascalis et al. 2020).

There were significant differences in the proportion of time spent in foraging and transiting states between the colonies. Differences may be explained by trip distances (Fig. S3); birds on Danger Island travelled marginally greater distances than birds from Nelson’s Island, and therefore may have incurred greater transit distances (Oppel et al. 2015). Alternatively, birds at Nelson’s Island experienced greater wind speeds (Fig. 4), potentially facilitating lower transit times. Female brown boobies sometimes travel further than males during foraging trips (Lewis et al. 2005; Weimerskirch et al. 2009; Soanes et al. 2015; Bunce 2015; Miller et al. 2018), although the reverse may also occur (Austin et al. 2021). Sample sizes here precluded formal testing, although we note no obvious sex differences in descriptive statistics (Fig. S5).

Foraging effort and at-sea distribution models are both used to help designate MPAs for seabirds (Lascelles et al. 2012; Le Corre et al. 2012). Our results demonstrate the value of the Chagos Archipelago large scale MPA for encompassing the foraging area and habitat preferences of brown boobies during breeding. This is encouraging for seabirds that appear to be increasing following the long-term impacts of invasive rats and vegetation (Carr et al. 2021a, b). Nevertheless, distribution of brown boobies within the Chagos Archipelago MPA does not imply complete protection of foraging resources, as illegal fishing remains a concern within the MPA (Collins et al. 2021).

Here, we attempted to understand variability in foraging strategies of two colonies of brown boobies in the Chagos Archipelago. As outlined previously, limited access to field sites meant that individuals between the two colonies were tracked during different years, monsoon periods, and breeding stages, and that sample sizes of both sexes were relatively low. Whilst similarities in foraging strategies are therefore noteworthy, future studies in the region could focus on filling these data gaps and providing greater understanding of intra-colony variability in this species.

Because of low sample sizes, at-sea tracks (Fig. 1) may under-represent true population distributions (Soanes et al. 2013). Despite the limited number of individuals with retrieved tracking data, incorporating multiple trips per individual undoubtedly improves estimation of area use (Soanes et al. 2013). Understanding the proportion of general and core foraging areas represented by tracked individuals is dependent on multiple factors, including year, and sex. Therefore, drawing conclusions on the effectiveness of the Chagos Archipelago MPA surrounding Danger Island and Nelson’s Island using brown boobies could be assessed in future studies with a particular emphasis on sample sizes.

Conclusion

To conclude, we found comparable foraging strategies by brown boobies from nearby islands (142 km apart) in the Chagos Archipelago despite differences in tracking periods and breeding stage. Brown boobies foraged in coastal areas along the shelf-edge, which contrasts the more pelagic behaviour of neighbouring red-footed boobies (Trevail et al. 2023), but is consistent with studies of this species elsewhere (Austin et al. 2021). Importantly, we found that foraging boobies avoided the shallow water of the Grand Chagos Bank atoll, but showed a preference for areas with a steeper bathymetric slope, higher sea-surface height anomalies, and ranges of SST that correspond to tuna foraging habitat. Finally, brown boobies remained within the Chagos Archipelago MPA suggesting that this large-scale MPA can offer protection to habitats and prey.