Introduction

Problematic impulse control behaviours (ICBs), incorporating impulse control disorders and other related behaviours, can develop in Parkinson’s disease (PD) patients. These behaviours often manifest as compulsive gambling, binge eating, hypersexuality, compulsive shopping, punding, hobbyism and compulsive medication use (Weintraub 2008). Previous research has identified factors associated with increased likelihood of developing ICBs in PD, including dopamine agonist (DA) medication (use, dose and duration), being male, unmarried, previous personal or family impulsive behaviour, higher Unified Parkinson’s disease rating scale (UPDRS) score and younger age of PD onset (Voon et al. 2011; Nombela et al. 2014; Kraemmer et al. 2016; Antonini et al. 2017; Cormier-Dequaire et al. 2018; Corvol et al. 2018; Gatto and Aldinio 2019). One of the most significant risk factors for ICBs in PD is DA medication, where 14–40% of patients taking this form of dopamine replacement therapy develop destructive ICBs (Bastiaens et al. 2013; Kraemmer et al. 2016; Erga et al. 2018). Clinically prescribed DAs predominantly act upon D2/D3 receptors (Gasser et al. 2015; Seeman et al. 2015), which are abundant in regions of the mesocorticolimbic (MCL) system (Ko et al. 2013; Seeman 2015). The MCL system is largely responsible for impulse control and is relatively spared during the early, unmedicated stages of PD (Cools 2006; Weintraub 2008; Smith et al. 2016; Caminiti et al. 2017; Claassen et. al. 2017; Gatto and Aldinio 2019), compared to the decrease of dopamine in the nigrostriatal system (Dauer and Przedborski 2003; Weintraub 2008; Vaillancourt et al. 2013). It is therefore possible that the addition of DA medication causes a tonic hyperdopaminergic state in the MCL network, which hinders phasic dopamine modulation, and subsequent problems with impulsivity (Weintraub 2008; Sinha et al. 2013; Vaillancourt et al. 2013; Gatto and Aldinio 2019; Meder et al. 2019). This state has been termed the overdose-hypothesis (Cools et al. 2001a; Vaillancourt et al. 2013; Ruitenberg et al. 2021). Moreover, increases in DA dose and the use of DA medication over time are often associated with ICBs in PD, due to higher concentrations of dopamine activating D2 receptors to a greater extent compared to lower concentrations (Trantham-Davidson et al. 2004). The working hypothesis being that increased and/or prolonged receptor activation may reduce D2 auto-receptor sensitivity (Gasser, Wichmann and DeLong 2015), leading to a blunted post-synaptic D2-mediated inhibitory effect, increased overall dopamine release and resultant impulsive behaviour (Ray et al. 2012; Ford 2014). The two possible mechanisms of effect are not mutually exclusive, and may well act in concert, though both offer explanations as to why DA medication leads to dysfunctional levels of dopamine and ICB development in some patients.

Another factor which can influence ICB development is genetic. Previous literature has identified specific genetic polymorphisms associated with ICBs in PD patients, either individually (Lee et al. 2009; Kraemmer et al. 2016; Erga et al. 2018) or collectively as a very large polygenic risk score (Ihle et al. 2020; Faouzi et al. 2021). The first dopaminergic genetic score quantifying the influence of a small number of genes was developed by Nikolova and colleagues (2011). This method was subsequently expanded by Pearson-Fuhrhop and colleagues (2013, 2014) to produce a polygenic dopamine genetic risk score (DGRS) incorporating five specific genes selected a-priori for each being known to modify dopamine signalling within MCL regions (Vriend et al. 2014; Smith et al. 2016; Caminiti et al. 2017) and influence impulse control (Congdon et al. 2009; Lee et al. 2009; Vriend et al. 2014; Abidin et al. 2015; Smith et al. 2016; Erga et al. 2018). These genes include: DRD1 rs4532, DRD2 rs1800497, DRD3 rs6280 (encoding D1, D2, D3 receptors, respectively), catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) rs4680 and dopamine transporter (DAT) rs28363170. The quantitative aspect of the DGRS weights the influence of each polymorphism on widespread tonic dopamine neurotransmission, where a higher score is equal to higher dopamine neurotransmission. It stands to reason that a PD patient’s genetically determined levels of MCL dopamine neurotransmission will affect how they respond, and whether they develop ICBs, when dopamine tone is further increased with DA medication. Indeed, our previous work utilising the DGRS for the first time in PD (Hall et al. 2021) demonstrated that patients with a low DGRS had more ICBs identified via the QUIP-S, which decreased with time on DA medication. Conversely, patients with a higher DGRS had fewer ICBs, but this number increased with time on DA medication. We were unable to discern whether increasing dosage over time or time of exposure to DA medication per se were causing these changes in ICBs.

MacDonald and colleagues (2016) were first to use the DGRS to explain objective measures of behavioural impulsivity in a non-PD population. These objective measures were stop signal reaction time (SSRT) from the Anticipatory Response Inhibition Task (ARIT) for impulsive action, and decision making following negative reinforcement on the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) for impulsive choice. They concluded that the administration of DA medication in healthy adults improved task measures of impulsive action and choice for those with a lower DGRS and worsened them for participants with a high DGRS. Previous literature has identified no change in impulsive behaviour for PD ICB patients after a loss on the BART, compared to non ICB patients who reduced their impulsive behaviour (Martini et al. 2018). Either shorter or no difference in SSRT has been found for ICB vs no ICB PD patients in the Stop Signal Task (Claassen et al. 2015; Ricciardi et al. 2017; Vriend et al. 2018; Hlavata et al. 2020). The ARIT and our specific measure of negative reinforcement in the BART have yet to be investigated in a PD cohort in the context of ICBs.

ICBs are routinely identified using the questionnaire for impulsive-compulsive disorders in Parkinson’s disease (QUIP) and further clinically diagnosed during an interview (Weintraub et al. 2009, 2012; Papay et al. 2011; Probst et al. 2014; Krieger et al. 2017; Marques et al. 2019; Takahashi et al. 2022). The Questionnaire for Impulsive-compulsive disorders in Parkinson’s disease short (QUIP-S) and QUIP rating scale (QUIP-RS) are two widely used self-report versions of this questionnaire. The QUIP-S involves only 13 questions with ‘yes’ or ‘no’ answers (Weintraub et al. 2009; Krieger et al. 2017), whereas the QUIP-RS includes 28 questions which are answered via a frequency rating scale with five different options and the final score is equated with ICB severity (Weintraub et al. 2012; Probst et al. 2014; Marques et al. 2019; Takahashi et al. 2022). The QUIP-RS offers a larger range of scores covering the same behaviours in more depth, which suggests the resultant ICB frequency (i.e., severity) score is capable of being a more sensitive measure of impulsivity, including changes over time (Marques et al. 2019), compared to ICB incidence from the QUIP-S (Weintraub et al. 2012; Probst et al. 2014). The Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS) is also a self-report questionnaire that measures impulsivity but as a trait or personality construct (Stanford et al. 2009), rather than a diagnostic tool for pathological ICBs directly. Nevertheless, ICBs in PD (Filip et al. 2018), including those determined by the QUIP-S (Marin-Lahoz et al. 2018) and QUIP-RS (Takahashi et al. 2022) are associated with higher impulsivity on the BIS. One particular study of note determined a positive correlation between total QUIP-RS score and BIS score (Goerlich-Dobre et al. 2014), highlighting the potential adjunct use of the BIS in ICB diagnosis.

The primary focus of the present study was to investigate whether objective, sensitive lab-based measures of impulsive behaviour, genetic and disease specific measures were associated with the severity of every day impulsive behaviour measured by the QUIP in a sample of PD patients taking DA medication. The first aim was to evaluate the validity of our objective lab-based task measures to be able to reflect the severity of subjective every day ICBs. This is a key issue to address as the ARIT and our specific measure of negative reinforcement in the BART have yet to be investigated in a PD cohort in the context of ICBs. We hypothesised that measures reflecting worse impulsivity on the tasks (higher SSRTs in the ARIT and more impulsive decision making in the BART) would be related to higher scores on the QUIP-RS. Our second aim was to identify prognostic risk factors for the severity of ICBs on dopamine agonists. We hypothesised that patients with a low DGRS would display worse task impulsivity and higher ICB frequency. Whereas those with a high DGRS would exhibit better impulsivity on the tasks and lower ICB frequency. We specifically wanted to investigate if DA medication dosage or time of exposure to DA medication could predict ICB frequency, following our previous results (Hall et al. 2021). We hypothesised that both DA medication dosage and time on DA medication would be higher for patients reporting a greater frequency of ICBs. When accounting for the influence of an individual’s genetic profile, we hypothesised that for patients with a low DGRS, longer exposure to DA medication would result in a reduction in ICBs over time. In contrast, patients with a high DGRS were expected to show increasing ICB frequency with increasing time on DA medication. We did not expect to find any comparable results for patients taking dopamine medication which did not include DAs. Finally, we wanted to examine any relationship between clinically identified ICBs and subjective trait impulsivity via the BIS.

Materials and methods

Participants

One hundred participants with PD were recruited for the current study via an advertisement on Parkinson’s UK and all participants self-identified as having a PD diagnosis. 70 recruited participants were taking DA medication and the remaining 30 were taking dopamine medication not including agonists. This target of 70 DA participants was to allow for participant drop out whilst still achieving the target sample size of 61, calculated from a-priori power calculation to achieve 80% power. Participants were included in the study if they were between the ages of 40–80, had no history of neurological illness other than PD and had normal or corrected-to normal vision. All demographic, clinical, questionnaire, behavioural and genetic data were collected remotely or online by means of online software, post, emails, video calls or phone calls.

Clinical impulsivity

ICB incidence

The QUIP-short comprised of 13 ‘yes’ or ‘no’ questions regarding current impulse control behaviors lasting at least 4 weeks. Participants would receive a score of one for ‘yes’ and zero for ‘no’. Any score greater than zero confirmed the incidence of an ICB.

ICB frequency

The QUIP-RS measured the frequency of ICBs. The questionnaire included four questions in each of the following categories: gambling, sex, buying, eating, hobbyism, punding and PD medication. Participants responded to each question with a choice from a 5-point scale (0: never, 1: rarely, 2: sometimes, 3: often, 4: very often) which represented impulsivity in the past 4 weeks or any 4-week period in a designated time frame. Total scores were calculated between 0 and 112.

Trait impulsivity

Barratt impulsiveness scale

A 4-point scale (1: rarely/never, 2: occasionally, 3: often, 4: almost always/always) questionnaire comprising of 30 questions about everyday behaviours assessing attentional, motor and non-planning trait impulsivity (Patton et al. 1995). A higher score reflects greater impulsivity. Two patients did not provide answers for 2 questions relating to the work environment as they were retired, and one patient did not answer one of the questions. Therefore, each participant’s result was normalised to a percentage where the score was divided by the total score possible from the number of questions answered and then multiplied by 100.

Impulsivity task performance

Anticipatory response inhibition task (ARIT)

The ARIT was presented on a computer screen using custom code written in Inquisit 6 Lab (Version 6.5.1, Millisecond Software) and responses were made using a keyboard. Participants completed the task on their personal computers at home. Participants initially observed an instruction video and practised 20 Go and 9 Stop trials. Subsequently, they were required to complete 10 blocks of 40 experimental trials. The experimental trials consisted of 295 Go trials and 105 Stop trials in a randomised order.

For the experimental procedure, on each trial participants were presented with a screen containing two vertical white bars (Fig. 1). The left bar was controlled with the ‘z’ key using the left index finger and the right bar was controlled with the ‘? /’ key using the right index finger. Every trial started with the participant holding down both keys which initiated a black bar rising within each of the white bars. Both black bars rose at equal rates and filled the white bars completely after 1000 ms. The black bars intercepted a horizontal target line at 800 ms. During Go trials, participants were required to intercept the horizontal target line with the rising bars by timing the removal of their fingers from both keys appropriately (successful releases were within 40 ms above the target and 30 ms below). Stop trials consisted of Non-Selective Stop Both (SB) trials and Partial Stop trials. During SB trials, participants were asked to keep both keys depressed when both bars stopped rising before reaching the target (Fig. 1). Partial Stop trials comprised of Stop Left (SL) and Stop Right (SR) trials, where one bar stopped and the other continued rising. Here, participants were required to keep the key depressed corresponding to the bar that stopped rising and intercept the target line with the alternative bar by releasing the corresponding key (Fig. 1). During Stop trials the bars initially stopped at 400 ms for SB and 300 ms for SL and SR. A staircase algorithm was utilised to generate a 50% success rate for each stop version. Following a successful Stop trial, the bar stop time increased by 25 ms on the subsequent Stop trial but decreased by 25 ms following an unsuccessful Stop trial. Stop signal reaction time from SB trials was calculated as the primary dependent measure using the integration method (Logan and Cowan 1984; Verbruggen et al. 2019).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Visual display at the start of a trial (all left panels) and during a GO, SB (Non-Selective Stop Both), SL (Stop Left) and SR (Stop Right) trial in the Anticipatory Response Inhibition Task. Green keys represent successful release of the key at the target and red keys represent successful cancellation and keeping the key depressed. On successful Go trials, both keys are released at the target line. On successful SB trials, both keys are held down. On successful SL trials, the right key is released, and the left key is held down. On successful SR trials, the left key is released, and the right key is held down

Balloon analogue risk task (BART)

The BART was displayed on the participant’s personal computer screen using custom code written in Inquisit 6 Lab (Version 6.5.1, Millisecond Software) and responses were made using the mouse. Participants initially completed 5 practise trials and then 30 experimental trials.

The experimental procedure was as follows: at the beginning of each trial, participants were presented with a new balloon and two options: ‘Pump up the balloon’ or ‘collect £££’ (Fig. 2). Participants could pump up the balloon, which incrementally increased potential earnings by £0.02 with each pump. If participants chose to collect their earnings, then the current trial would end, and the amount accumulated was added to the total winnings. However, the balloon could randomly explode on any pump and any potential earnings would be lost, followed by the end of the trial. Each trial started with a 1 in 85 probability of the balloon exploding. With every pump of the balloon, one number was randomly selected and removed without replacement from an 85-length array. When number one was selected, the balloon would pop and the trial would end with no monetary collection. The risk of balloon explosion therefore increased with each pump (1/84, 1/83 etc.), but so did the potential monetary reward. The average number of pumps on a collection trial (i.e., when the number of pumps was not artificially constrained by a balloon burst) following a successful monetary collection (average collection pumps) and following a loss (balloon explosion) were calculated for each participant. The difference between these means normalised to pumps after a loss (losses cancel) reflected positive reinforcement and normalised to pumps after a win (wins cancel) reflected negative reinforcement (Mata et al. 2012; MacDonald et al. 2016). Proportions further from zero indicated a greater change in behaviour following either a positive or negative outcome. In this context, behaviour modification reflects a change in impulsivity. Negative reinforcement was the main dependent measure in this task, given previous results (MacDonald et al. 2016).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Visual display of the Balloon Analogue Risk Task. ‘Pump up the balloon’ and ‘Collect £££’ are the two available response options. Visual feedback of ‘Balloon number’, ‘Potential earnings’, ‘Number of pumps’ and ‘Total winnings’ are displayed throughout each trial

Cognitive function

Central nervous system vital signs (cnsvs)

CNSVS is a computerised neurocognitive test battery comprising of neuropsychological tests to assess cognitive behaviour and acts as a tool, not for diagnosis, but for brief clinical evaluation of mild cognitive dysfunction (Gualtieri and Johnson 2006). Participants completed all tests on their computer and made their responses using a keyboard. The scores produced from these tests contribute to neurocognitive clinical evaluation domains. Nine tests were included within the current research which were linked to 14 cognitive domains: composite memory, verbal memory, visual memory, psychomotor speed, reaction time, complex attention, cognitive flexibility, processing speed, executive function, reasoning, working memory, sustained attention, simple attention and motor speed. Automated scoring reported raw patient test scores for each domain which were automatically normalised and age-matched to a large normative database to create standard scores. These scores were produced for the 14 domains along with the neurocognitive index (NCI) which represents a global score of neurocognition by taking an average of the domain scores for composite memory, psychomotor speed, reaction time, complex attention and cognitive flexibility. Standard scores for NCI and working memory were included in analyses.

Genetic data

Five specific genetic polymorphisms which formed the DGRS were identified for each participant. Genetic analysis was conducted by LGC Genomics, and full methodology can be found at: http://www.lgcgenomics.com/. The single nucleotide polymorphisms within four genes were determined using kompetitive allele specific polymerase change reaction (KASP PCR) genotyping: DRD1 (rs4532), DRD2 (rs1800497), DRD3 (rs6280) and COMT (rs4680). This process produced a bi-allelic score for each single nucleotide polymorphism. The variable number tandem repeat in the DAT gene (rs28363170) was analyzed using a separate PCR process. Here, the PCR was followed by PCR clean-up, sanger sequencing and genotype calling. The repeat length of DAT VNTR was determined by eye on the sequence trace files.

Dependent upon the specific mutation/number of repeats for each of the five polymorphisms, every participant received a score of 0 or 1 for each polymorphism according to whether it acts to decrease or increase dopamine transmission, respectively (Pearson-Fuhrhop et al. 2013, 2014; MacDonald et al. 2016). All gene scores were then summed for an overall DGRS between 0–5 (higher score = higher dopamine levels) (Table 1S, Online Resource). All genes were in Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (all p > 0.291), which was determined with chi-square tests. For the linear regression models discussed below relating to the DA group (N = 50), the sample size for each DGRS was as follows: DGRS 0 n = 0; DGRS 1 n = 4, DGRS 2 n = 11, DGRS 3 n = 17, DGRS 4 n = 4, DGRS 5 n = 14. The DGRS was split into two groups: DGRS low (DGRS 0–2) and DGRS high (DGRS 3–5) aiming to make as equal sample sizes as possible. The DGRS was utilised as a binary independent variable within the models.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis and modelling were performed in MATLAB (version R2020a, MathWorks). As a preliminary analysis, comparisons were made for all available clinical, demographic, genetic and cognitive variables between those with and without an ICB. Seventeen participants with unavailable data for these variables due to errors in reporting and incomplete online datasets from the CNSVS were discarded from these analyses (DA n = 12, NDA n = 5). Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests identified any violations of normality. Wilcoxon rank sum tests were used to compare any variables which violated normality, while the remaining variables were compared using unpaired t-tests. A simple linear regression looked for a correlation between ICB frequency on the QUIP-RS and BIS score in both DA and NDA groups. The following linear regression models identified the variables associated with clinical and trait impulsivity.

Clinical impulsivity model

The response variable for this model was ICBs identified via the QUIP. A participant’s score on the QUIP-S and QUIP-RS were strongly correlated (R = 0.72, p < 0.001). Therefore, we chose to predict results of the QUIP-RS because a larger scale range was likely to be more sensitive to changes in impulsivity. CNSVS NCI and WM were not included due to missing data, as their inclusion would have reduced the sample size of the model. DGRS, DA levodopa equivalent daily dose (DA LEDD), Negative Reinforcement from the BART, SSRT from Stop Both trials of the ARIT, and Years on DA were selected a-priori to be included in the model to test our hypotheses and build on previous literature (MacDonald et al. 2016; Hall et al. 2021). Univariate linear regression analyses identified any additional variables which could be included as independent predictors of ICB frequency in the full model (Table 2S, Online Resource). However, any continuous variables identified were tested for collinearity against the pre-selected variables, and resultant correlated variables were not included in the final model (Table 3S, Online Resource). Therefore, UPDRS I&II and Years Since Diagnosis were not included in the final model as they both correlated with Years on DA (both p < 0.001). Gender was also not included to not overparameterise the model. The final mixed-effects multiple linear regression model was formed with selected variables and hypothesised interactions:

$$y(ICB frequency)={\beta }_{0}\left(intercept\right)+ {\beta }_{1}DGRS + {\beta }_{2}DA\;LEDD+ {\beta }_{3}\mathrm{Years \;on\,DA}+ {\beta }_{4}\mathrm{SSRT\; SB}+ {\beta }_{5}\mathrm{Negative \,Reinforcement}+ {\beta }_{6}DGRS*Years \;on \,DA+{\beta }_{7}DGRS*SSRT\;SB+ {\beta }_{8}DGRS*Negative \,Reinforcement+ \varepsilon$$

Further linear regressions were run with this model to determine the contribution of each individual genetic polymorphism towards the response variable. This involved substituting the score (0 or 1) for each genetic polymorphism into the model in place of the full DGRS. The same model was run for the NDA group, without DA LEDD and Years on DA.

Trait impulsivity model

The same independent variables and interactions from the clinical impulsivity model were selected for inclusion in the multiple linear regression model predicting BIS percentage as the response variable:

$$y (BIS percentage)={(\beta }_{0}\left(intercept\right)+ {\beta }_{1}DGRS + {\beta }_{2}DA \; LEDD+ {\beta }_{3}\mathrm{Years \; on\,DA}+ {\beta }_{4}\mathrm{SSRT \; SB}+ {\beta }_{5}\mathrm{Negative\,Reinforcement}+ {\beta }_{6}DGRS*Years \; on\,DA+{\beta }_{7}DGRS*SSRT \;SB+ {\beta }_{8}DGRS*Negative\,Reinforcement+ \varepsilon$$

The same model was run for the NDA group, without DA LEDD and Years on DA.

Model validation

Effect sizes for all models were determined and interpreted using adjusted R2 (0.01 = small, 0.09 = medium, 0.25 = large, Foster et al., 2018) and the achieved statistical power is reported (G*Power 3.1.9.6). Validation against a constant model (i.e., goodness-of-fit) was assessed for all models and an alpha value of 0.05 was used for all analyses.

Results

Preliminary analysis

Data from 83 participants (DA: n = 58, 45–77 years, mean 64.1 ± 8.80 standard deviation, NDA: n = 25, 46–79 years, mean 64.6 ± 8.60 standard deviation) were included in the preliminary clinical, demographic, genetic and cognitive comparisons between those with (QUIP-S > 1) and without (QUIP-S = 0) an ICB (Table 1). Of these participants, in the DA group, 16 participants had a low DGRS (0–2) and 42 had a high DGRS (3–5). Moreover, in the NDA group, 9 participants had a low DGRS and the remaining 16 presented a high DGRS. In the DA group, participants with an ICB were more likely to be male (p = 0.008) and presented with a higher BIS (p = 0.002) and QUIP-RS (p = 0.010) score. Scores on the UPDRS I&II trended towards being higher for those with an ICB than those without. These results were not likely to be due to changes in general cognitive function as there were no differences between CNSVS NCI and WM between ICB groups. In the NDA group, those with an ICB reported a greater number of years since diagnosis (p = 0.039), a higher QUIP-RS (p = 0.008) score, and the increased overall medication dosage (Total LEDD) trended towards significance (p = 0.062).

Table 1 Participant clinical, demographic, genetic and cognitive variables separated by incidence of impulse control behaviours via the QUIP-short

Linear regression models

The sample sizes for the following models were reduced (DA n = 50, NDA n = 22) due to incomplete datasets for included independent variables or the inability to genotype from the DNA sample. The following results are specific to DA medication, as NDA models were unable to explain any variability in the outcome variable (goodness-of-fit: clinical model p = 0.951, trait model p = 0.662). There was therefore nothing to report for these NDA models.

Clinical impulsivity

Task performance and exposure time to DA medication were associated with the frequency of ICBs

The Clinical Impulsivity model (Table 2) was validated against a constant model (F7,41 = 3.15, p = 0.007) and explained 26% of the variance in ICB frequency scores according to the adjusted R2 value (unadjusted R2 = 0.381, i.e., large effect size). The statistical power achieved by the model was 97.2%, also indicating an appropriate sample size for the model. ICB frequency increased by 12.3 for every 1 unit increase in negative reinforcement (β = 12.3, p = 0.014). This statistic indicates that, as expected, people who made more impulsive decisions on the BART after a loss also reported a higher frequency of ICBs. The increase in ICB frequency of 0.07 for each millisecond increase in SSRT SB trended towards significance (β = 0.07, 0 = 0.056), indicating people with worse motor impulsivity tended to report a higher frequency of ICBs, as predicted. Of note, the two tasks were not correlated (Table 3S, r = − 0.13, p = 0.354) and the univariate analysis (Table 2S) showed that neither task measure in isolation could explain variance in ICB frequency (ARIT SSRT Stop Both β = 0.03, p = 0.400; r = 0.09; BART Negative Reinforcement β = 6.92, p = 0.153; r = 0.22). Therefore, it appears that when partitioning out variance amongst variables within the multiple linear regression model, each task significantly accounted for independent variance in the model, which highlights the potential contribution to two different aspects of ICB severity (e.g. via motor and cognitive impulse control).

Table 2 Multiple linear regression analysis of variables associated with the frequency of impulse control behaviours

As hypothesised, ICB frequency increased by 2.09 for every year on DA medication (β = 2.09, p < 0.001). However, contrary to our hypotheses, these associations between clinical impulsivity and task performance/time on medication did not depend on a participant’s DGRS (DGRS X Negative Reinforcement: β = 11.5, p = 0.463; DGRS X SSRT: β = 0.009, p = 0.911; DGRS X Years on DA: β = − 1.19, p = 0.305). DA dose (β = − 0.004, p = 0.862) and DGRS alone (β = 9.09, p = 0.708) were also not predictive of ICB frequency score.

Interestingly, two of the DGRS constituent genes interacted with time on DAs to effect ICB frequency. When substituting COMT into the model (F7,41 = 4.1, p = 0.001, R2 = 0.444 i.e., large effect size, 95.7% power), the increase in ICB frequency from one year on DAs was 2.49 more for a COMT score of 1 (greater dopamine neurotransmission, β = 2.12) compared to 0 (β = − 0.37, p = 0.048). Similarly for DAT (F7,41 = 4.57, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.471 i.e., large effect size, 95.6% power), the increase in ICB frequency from one year on DAs was 1.14 more for a DAT score of 1 (β = 2.56) compared to 0 (β = 1.42, p = 0.014). DAT score also interacted with Negative Reinforcement. For participants with a DAT score of 1, a single unit increase in Negative Reinforcement reduced ICB frequency by 28 (β = − 11.0) compared to participants with a score of 0 (β = 17.0, p = 026). No individual genetic polymorphism was independently associated with a change in ICB frequency (p > 0.288).

Trait impulsivity

Trait impulsivity (BIS percentage) was significantly correlated with clinical impulsivity (ICB frequency) in both DA (R = 0.56, p < 0.001, Fig. 3) and NDA (R = 0.74, p < 0.001, Fig. 4) groups. This indicates that participants who reported higher levels of everyday trait impulsivity, also reported a higher frequency of ICBs.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Linear correlation between impulse control behaviour (ICB) frequency measured via the QUIP-RS and Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS) percentage score in the dopamine agonist group. Data circles represent individual participants

Fig. 4
figure 4

Linear correlation between impulse control behaviour (ICB) frequency measured via the QUIP-RS and Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS) percentage score in the non-dopamine agonist group. Data circles represent individual participants

Long term exposure to DA medication predicted subjective, real-world trait impulsivity

For trait impulsivity (F7,41 = 1.98, p = 0.074, R2 = 0.28 i.e., large effect size, 83.5% power, Table 3), a participant’s BIS increased by 1.14% with every year on DA medication (β = 1.14, p = 0.003). No other independent variables or interactions significantly predicted BIS percentage (p > 0.358).

Table 3 Multiple linear regression analysis of variables associated with Barratt Impulsiveness Scale percentage

Discussion

The focus of this study was to investigate the sensitivity of objective task measures, along with variation in dopamine genetics and disease specific measures, to determine the frequency of clinically identified ICBs. As such, the study produced several novel findings which were specific to DA medication. As hypothesised, task performance was associated with ICBs. Participants who made a greater number of impulsive decisions after a loss on the BART, or who tended to exhibit worse impulsivity on the ARIT, also reported a higher frequency of impulsive behaviours on the clinical screening tool. Interestingly, as performance on the two tasks were not related, the two performance measures seem to be associated with two distinct aspects of ICB severity. However, DGRS, an analogy of dopamine neurotransmission, did not interact with task performance to determine clinical impulsivity. Interestingly, the DAT polymorphism interacted with impulsive decision making on the BART to effect ICB frequency. The secondary aim of this study was to work towards identifying measures for prognostic use for ICBs on dopamine agonists, thus time on DA medication and DA dosage were incorporated into the models. Greater length of exposure to DA medication was associated with higher ICB frequency as predicted, whereas DA dosage was not. The DGRS did not interact with time on DAs, however when examining the influence of individual genes, more dopamine neurotransmission indexed via polymorphisms in COMT and DAT predicted higher ICB frequency with increasing exposure to DA medication. More time on DA medication was also associated with higher levels of trait impulsivity, which in turn was correlated with ICB frequency. The results of the current study present promising initial results highlighting the potential use of our task-derived measures of impulse control to predict ICB severity in people with Parkinson’s disease on DA medication.

A linear relationship existed between task performance and clinically identified ICBs, but only for patients taking dopamine agonist medication. Patients who made more impulsive decisions on the BART after a loss also reported a higher frequency of ICBs. Our finding aligns with other studies that show ICB patients failing to reduce their impulsive behaviour following a loss on the BART, reflecting punishment (Martini et al. 2018), although this effect has not been previously confirmed to be agonist specific. However, when negative feedback is calculated slightly differently as the difference between number of balloon pumps directly preceding and following a loss, PD patients can show reduced impulsive behaviour irrespective of ICB and DA status (Claassen et al. 2011). For performance on the ARIT in our study, worse impulsive action (a longer SSRT) tended to be associated with increased ICB frequency. To our knowledge, we are the first to investigate the relationship between ICBs and ARIT performance, whilst other studies have produced valid data utilising the ARIT in other patient groups with dopamine or basal ganglia dysfunction, namely focal hand dystonia and ADHD (Stinear & Byblow, 2004; Gilbert et al., 2019). Findings using SSRT derived from the stop signal task have been mixed. Studies have reported no differences in SSRT between PD patients with and without ICBs (Ricciardi et al. 2017; Vriend et al. 2018; Hlavata et al. 2020), as well as shorter SSRTs in ICB patients compared not only to PD patients without ICBs, but also to healthy control participants (Claassen et al. 2015). The positive relationship between SSRT and ICB frequency in our study may be due to task design, as the ARIT explores control of internally generated, rather than the externally cued responses. PD patients find internally generated responses with an anticipatory component most difficult (Jahanshahi et al. 1996), which likely reflects a sensitivity of predictive timing processes to the ongoing deterioration of the prefrontal-basal ganglia network (Cunnington et al. 1995) and therefore potentially dopaminergic MCL function. Overall, our objective task measures show promise as sensitive markers of impulsivity problems on DAs leading to real-world impulsive behaviours. A worthwhile next step will be to investigate whether impaired task performance is capable of preceding, and therefore forecasting, ICB development.

Contrary to our hypotheses, there was no association between DGRS and ICB frequency and no interaction between task measures and the full DGRS. This finding contrasts with our previous finding that the DGRS can explain the incidence of ICBs (Hall et al. 2021). However, there is a key distinction between the studies. Namely, our previous study was predicting the binary presence/absence of any ICB, whereas the current study tried to link the DGRS with a measure closer to ICB severity i.e., frequency of ICBs. The rationale for this was twofold: 1) to use the more finely grained and wider ranging responses on the QUIP-RS (compared to the QUIP-S) for maximal sensitivity to subtle changes in task measures e.g., a change in SSRT of a few milliseconds, and 2) because the smaller sample size in a binary outcome variable in the current study would have limited overall model sensitivity (23 QUIP-S ≥ 1, 27 = 0). Combined, perhaps our results speak to the DGRS being able to predict the development of an ICB, rather than determining the more subtle distinction between severity of behaviours. Interestingly, although the full DGRS did not interact with task measurers, the DAT polymorphism in isolation interacted with impulsive decision making on the BART to effect ICB frequency. A relationship between DAT and cognitive impulsivity task performance has previously been reported (Mata et al. 2012; MacDonald et al. 2016). DAT is responsible for the reuptake of dopamine into pre-synaptic neurons (Hovde et al. 2019) and predominantly removes dopamine from within the striatum, a key region for cognitive decision making (Mata et al. 2012; Vriend et al. 2014). A higher DAT score represents a less functional DAT protein, which leads to less clearance of dopamine from the synaptic cleft, and greater striatal dopamine neurotransmission (Cilia et al. 2010; Vriend et al. 2014). In our study, patients with higher striatal dopamine levels (i.e., DAT = 1) who made more impulsive decisions on the BART counterintuitively had lower, rather than higher, clinical impulsivity. There is no immediately obvious reason for this paradoxical finding, but it should be interpreted with caution, as the study was not designed to primarily investigate single gene effects.

Increased exposure to DA medication, but not increasing dose, predicted higher trait impulsivity and increased ICB frequency. The effect of purely time on DAs separate from dose has not been widely reported. Of those who did isolate time on DAs, some studies reported a positive correlation with ICBs (Giladi et al. 2007; Corvol et al. 2018), whereas others did not (Bastiaens et al. 2013). The findings for DA dose are also somewhat mixed, although a greater proportion of studies have previously determined a positive association between DA dosage and ICBs (Weintraub et al. 2006; Lee et al. 2010; Joutsa et al. 2012; Perez-Lloret et al. 2012; Bastiaens et al. 2013; Corvol et al. 2018; Markovic et al. 2020), than no relationship (Isaias et al. 2008; Housden et al. 2010; Weintraub et al. 2010; Callesen et al. 2014; Vela et al. 2016; Erga et al. 2017). The reduced D2 auto-receptor sensitivity hypothesis explained previously is one potential neural mechanisms of action underlying our effect of time on DAs. Epigenetics may also be playing a role. Dopamine medication may regulate DNA transcription over time to increase protein and therefore neurotransmitter production (Lepack et al. 2020), potentially leading to the increase in impulsive behaviour. In our study, COMT and DAT mutations resulting in greater dopamine neurotransmission were associated with higher ICB frequency with increasing time on DA medication. Again single-gene exploratory findings should be interpreted with caution but could point to future epigenetics work including these genes when investigating gene vs medication interactions in the context of ICB severity over time.

Participants who reported higher levels of everyday trait impulsivity, also reported a higher frequency of ICBs in both the DA and NDA groups. Impulsive trait behaviour is a risk factor for ICBs (Leeman and Potenza 2011; Weintraub and Mamikonyan 2019) and PD patients with ICBs have reported higher impulsivity on the BIS compared to those without ICBs (Isaias et al. 2008; Marin-Lahoz et al. 2018; Hlavata et al. 2020; Takahashi et al. 2022). Our positive correlation between BIS and QUIP-RS in both DA and NDA groups has previously been reported in a group of PD patients, but it is uncertain how many of these patients were on DA medication (Goerlich-Dobre et al. 2014). The presence of a comparable relationship in both groups suggests that the behavioural manifestation of ICBs in an NDA group may be similar to those on DAs. However, our clinical model was unable to account for the variability in ICB severity for this group, indicating the underlying mechanisms for ICBs may be distinct for agonist vs non-agonist medication (Kelly et al. 2020).

It is important to acknowledge some limitations of the current study. Firstly, the NDA control group had a smaller sample size than the DA group due to recruitment time constraints. The smaller sample size and reduced variability may have contributed to our clinical model being unable to account for ICB frequency in the NDA group. Although it is worth noting the ICB variability was still sufficient to reveal a correlation with BIS scores, and the NDA group reported a similar average and range of QUIP scores compared to the DA group. Nevertheless, future work should aim to replicate this lack of effect with the clinical model in a larger group of PD patients who are taking only non-agonist medication. Additionally, it is important to acknowledge that we cannot confirm that those in the NDA group did not previously take DA medication. There is therefore a possibility that some of them may have been experiencing persistent ICB effects following termination of DAs. However, the relatively short average disease duration for this group (ICB = 4.64 years, no–ICB = 2.68 years) makes this unlikely. Secondly, although we present novel findings by including time on DA medication in our models, this was a cross sectional study. A longitudinal study design is required to confirm interactions with time on an individual basis. A longitudinal design would also reveal whether task performance tracks with ICB changes over time. If this design was conducted with de novo patients, it could additionally reveal any changes to predictive variables that precede increases in ICBs, which is a crucial step towards identifying measures for prognostic use.

In summary, this study provides evidence that our objective measures from impulse control tasks and time of exposure to medication can explain ICB severity in people with PD and are specific to DA mechanisms of effect. On the other hand, the DGRS appears better suited to predicting the incidence, rather than severity, of ICBs on DAs. Future research should determine whether task performance can be used to monitor ICB changes over time within an individual on agonist medication, and crucially whether task measures can detect subtle impulsivity changes before larger changes in everyday behaviour progress to a clinically problematic level.