Skip to main content
Log in

Willingness towards cognitive engagement: a preliminary study based on a behavioural entropy approach

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Faced with a novel task some people enthusiastically embark in it and work with determination, while others soon lose interest and progressively reduce their efforts. Although cognitive neuroscience has explored the behavioural and neural features of apathy, the why’s and how’s of positive engagement are only starting to be understood. Stemming from the observation that the left hemisphere is commonly associated to a proactive (‘do something’) disposition, we run a preliminary study exploring the possibility that individual variability in eagerness to engage in cognitive tasks could reflect a preferred left- or right-hemisphere functioning mode. We adapted a task based on response-independent reinforcement and used entropy to characterize the degree of involvement, diversification, and predictability of responses. Entropy was higher in women, who were overall more active, less dependent on instructions, and never reduced their engagement during the task. Conversely, men showed lower entropy, took longer pauses, and became significantly less active by the end of the allotted time, renewing their efforts mainly in response to negative incentives. These findings are discussed in the light of neurobiological data on gender differences in behaviour.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abraham A, Thybusch K, Pieritz K, Hermann C (2014) Gender differences in creative thinking: behavioral and fMRI findings. Brain Imaging Behav 8:39–51

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ahl H (2006) Why research on women entrepreneurs needs new directions. Entrep Theory Pract 30:595–621

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andreano JM, Cahill L (2009) Sex influences on the neurobiology of learning and memory. Learn Mem 16:248–266

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA (2014) Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on. Trends Cognit Sci 18:177–185

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baylé FJ, Bourdel MC, Caci H, Gorwood P, Chignon J-M, Adés J, Lôo H (2000) [Factor analysis of french translation of the Barratt impulsivity scale (BIS-10)]. Can J Psychiat 45:156–165

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beaty RE, Silvia PJ, Nusbaum EC, Jauk E, Benedek M (2014) The roles of associative and executive processes in creative cognition. Memory Cognit 42:1186–1197

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bell EC, Willson MC, Wilman AH, Dave S, Silverstone PH (2006) Males and females differ in brain activation during cognitive tasks. NeuroImage 30:529–538

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boghi A, Rasetti R, Avidano F, Manzone C et al. (2006) The effect of gender on planning: An fMRI study using the Tower of London task. Neuroimage 33:999–1010

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bonnelle V, Manohar S, Behrens T, Husain M (2016) Individual differences in premotor brain systems underlie behavioral apathy. Cereb Cortex 26:807–819

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Braun CMJ (2007) Evolution of hemispheric specialisation of antagonistic systems of management of the body’s energy resources. Laterality 12:397–427

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo JT, Petty RE (1982) The need for cognition. J Pers Soc Psychol 42:116–131

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cahill L (2006) Why sex matters for neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 7:477–484

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carhart-Harris RL, Leech R, Hellyer PJ, Shanahan M, Feilding A, Tagliazucchi E, Chialvo DR, Nutt D (2014) The entropic brain: a theory of conscious states informed by neuroimaging research with psychedelic drugs. Front Hum Neurosci 8:1–22

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen J (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, 2nd edn. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc, Hillsdale

    Google Scholar 

  • Coyne SM, Linder JR, Rasmussen EE, Nelson DA, Birkbeck V (2016) Pretty as a princess: Longitudinal effects of engagement with Disney princesses on gender stereotypes, body esteem, and prosocial behavior in children. Child Dev 87:1909–1925

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crant JM (2000) Proactive behavior in organizations. J Manag 26:435–462

    Google Scholar 

  • De Pater IE, Van Vianen AEM, Fischer AH, Van Ginkel WP (2009) Challenging experiences: gender differences in task choice. J Manag Psychol 24:4–28

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Derntl B, Finkelmeyer A, Eickhoff S, Kellermann T et al. (2010) Multidimensional assessment of empathic abilities: neural correlates and gender differences. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35:67–82

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dien J (2008) Looking both ways through time: the Janus model of lateralized cognition. Brain Cogn 67:292–323

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dietrich A, Kanso R (2010) A review of EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies of creativity and insight. Psychol Bull 136:822–848

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duckworth AL, Peterson C, Matthews MD, Kelly DR (2007) Grit: perseverance and passion for long-term goals. J Pers Soc Psychol 92:1087–1101

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duncker K (1945) On problem solving. Psychological Monographs, 58:1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dutton JE, Ashford SJ, O’ Neill RM, Hayes E, Wierba EE (1997) Reading the wind: how middle managers assess the context for selling issues to top managers. Strateg Manag J 18:407–423

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Estes WK (1961) A descriptive approach to the dynamics of choice behavior. Behav Sci 6:177–184

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fallows D (2005) How women and men use the Internet. Pew Internet and American Life Project. http://www.pewinternet.org/2005/12/28/how-women-and-men-use-the-internet/. Accessed 30 Jan 2019

  • Fay D, Frese M (2001) The concept of personal initiative: an overview of validity studies. Hum Perform 14:97–124

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frese M, Fay D, Hilburger T, Leng K, Tag A (1997) The concept of personal initiative: Operationalization, reliability and validity in two German samples. J Occup Org Psychol 70:139–161

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga MS (1989) Organization of the human brain. Science 245:947–952

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga MS (1995) Principles of human brain organization derived from split-brain studies. Neuron 14:217–228

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga MS (2000) Cerebral specialization and interhemispheric communication: does the corpus callosum enable the human condition? Brain 123:1293–1326

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geiser C, Lehmann W, Eid M (2006) Separating “rotators” from “non- rotators” in the mental rotation test: a multigroup latent class analysis. Multivar Behav Res 41(3):261–293

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gianotti LR, Knoch D, Faber PL, Lehmann D, Pascual-Marqui RD, Diezi C, Schoch C, Eisenegger C, Fehr E (2009) Tonic activity level in the right prefrontal cortex predicts individuals' risk taking. Psychol Sci 20(1):33–38

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gilligan C (1977) In a different voice: women’s conceptions of self and of morality. Harv Educ Rev 47:481–517

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gonen-Yaacovi G, de Souza LC, Levy R, Urbanski M, Josse G, Emmanulle V (2013) Rostral and caudal prefrontal contributions to creativity: a meta-analysis of functional imaging data. Front Hum Neurosci 7:465

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gupta VK, Bhawe NM (2007) The influence of proactive personality and stereotype threat on women’s entrepreneurial intentions. J Leadersh Org Stud 13:73–85

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haaland KY, Elsinger CL, Mayer AR, Durgerian S, Rao SM (2004) Motor sequence complexity and performing hand produce differential patterns of hemispheric lateralization. J Cogn Neurosci 16:621–636

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harenski CL, Antonenko O, Shane MS, Kiehl KA (2008) Gender differences in neural mechanisms underlying moral sensitivity. Soc Cognit Affect Neurosci 3:313–321

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harmon-Jones E (2003) Clarifying the emotive functions of asym- metrical frontal cortical activity. Psychophysiology 40:838–848

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heil M, Jansen-Osmann P (2008) Sex differences in mental rotation with polygons of different complexity: do men utilize holistic processes whereas women prefer piecemeal ones? Q J Exp Psychol 61(5):683–689

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heilman KM (2016) Possible brain mechanisms of creativity. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 31:285–296

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Higgins E (1998) Promotion and prevention: regulatory focus as a motivational principle. In: Zanna MP (ed) Advances in experimental social psychology, vol 30. Elsevier Academic Press Advances in experimental social psychology, San Diego, pp 1–46

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins ET (2000) Making a good decision: value from fit. Am Psychol 55(11):1217–1230

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Higgins ET, Kruglanski AW, Pierro A (2003) Regulatory mode: locomotion and assessment as distinct orientations. In: Zanna MP (ed) Advances in experimental social psychology, vol 35. Elsevier Academic Press, San Diego, pp 293–344

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill BD, Aita SL (2018) The positive side of effort: A review of the impact of motivation and engagement on neuropsychological performance. Appl Neuropsychol Adult 25:312–317

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hinson JM, Staddon JER (1983) Matching, maximizing and hill-climbing. J Exp Anal Behav 40:321–331

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hyde JS (2014) Gender similarities and differences. Annu Rev Psychol 65:373–398

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hyde JS (2016) Sex and cognition: gender and cognitive functions. Curr Opin Neurobiol 38:53–56

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson L, Ervin K, Gardner P, Schmitt N (2001) Gender and the Internet: Women communicating and men searching. Sex Roles 44:363–379

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird PN (2010) Mental models and human reasoning. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107:18243–50

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird PN, Khemlani SS, Goodwin GP (2015) Logic, probability, and human reasoning. Trends Cognit Sci 19:201–214

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jordan K, Wüstenberg T, Heinze HJ, Peters M, Jäncke L (2002) Women and men exhibit different cortical activation patterns during mental rotation tasks. Neuropsychologia 40:2397–2408

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koellinger P, Minniti M, Schade C (2013) Gender differences in entrepreneurial propensity. Oxford Bull Econ Stat 75:213–234

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koscik T, Bechara A, Tranel D (2010) Sex-related functional asymmetry in the limbic brain. Neuropsychopharmacology 35:340–341

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krug R, Stamm U, Pietrowsky R, Fehm HL, Born J (1994) Effects of menstrual cycle on creativity. Psychoneuroendocrinology 19:21–31

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski AW, Thompson EP, Higgins ET, Atash MN, Pierro A, Shah JY, Spiegel S (2000) To “do the right thing” or to “just do it”: locomotion and assessment as distinct self-regulatory imperatives. J Pers Soc Psychol 79:793–815

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski AW, Pierro A, Mannetti L, Higgins TE (2012) The distinct psychologies of “looking” and “leaping”: assessment and locomotion as the springs of action. Soc Pers Psychol Compass 7:79–92

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kurzban R, Duckworth A, Kable JW, Myers J (2013) An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance. Behav Brain Sci 36:661–679

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lee NYL, Johnson-Laird PN (2013) Strategic changes in problem solving. J Cognit Psychol 25:165–173

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee NYL et al. (2008) The psychological problem of Sudoku. Think Reason 14:342–364

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loas G, Dardennes R, Dhee-Perot P, Leclerc V, Fremaux D (1994) Operationalization of the “locus of control” concept: translation and first validation study of the Levenson control scale (IPC: the internal powerful others and chance scale). Ann Médico-Psychol Soc Médico-Psychol 152:466–469

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Manor B, Costa MD, Hu K, Newton E, Starobinets O, Kang HG, Peng CK, Novak V, Lipsitz LA (2010) Physiological complexity and system adaptability: evidence from postural control dynamics of older adults. J Appl Physiol 109:1786–1791

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Marinsek N, Turner BO, Gazzaniga M, Miller MB (2014) Divergent hemispheric reasoning strategies: reducing uncertainty versus resolving inconsistency. Front Hum Neurosci 8:839

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe J, Funnell M, Gazzaniga MS (1995) Right-hemisphere memory superiority: studies of a split-brain patient. Psychol Sci 6:157–164

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molden DC, Higgins ET (2008) How preferences for eager versus vigilant judgment strategies affect self-serving conclusions. J Exp Soc Psychol 44:1219–1228

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Nash K, McGregor I, Inzlicht M (2010) Line bisection as a neural marker of approach motivation. Psychophysiology 47:979–983

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nederkoorn C, Vancleef L, Wilkenhöner A, Claes L, Havermans RC (2016) Self-inflicted pain out of boredom. Psychiatry Res 237:127–132

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ninness C, Ninness SK (1999) Contingencies of superstition: self-generated rules and responding during second-order response-independent schedules. Psychol Record 49:221–243

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Njemanze P (2005) Cerebral lateralization and general intelligence: Gender differences in a transcranial Doppler study. Brain Lang 92:234–239

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Olsen BI, Laeng B, Kristiansen KA, Hartvigsen G (2013) Size does matter: women mentally rotate large objects faster than men. Scand J Psychol 54:196–204

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ono K (1987) Superstitious behavior in humans. J Exp Anal Behav 47:261–271

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Orne MT (1962) On the social psychology of the psychological experiment: With particular reference to demand characteristics and their implications. Am Psychol 17:776–783

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner MI, Petersen SE (1990) The attention system of the human brain. Annu Rev Neurosci 13:25–42

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rausch R (1977) Cognitive strategies in patients with unilateral temporal lobe excisions. Neuropsychologia 15:385–395

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reid G (2012) Motivation in video games: a literature review. Comput Games J 1:70–81

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roberti JW (2004) A review of behavioral and biological correlates of sensation seeking. J Res Pers 38(3):256–279

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robertson LC, Lamb MR (1991a) Neuropsychological contributions to theories of part/whole organization. Cogn Psychol 23:299–330

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robertson LC, Lamb MR, Knight RT (1991b) Normal global-local analysis in patients with dorsolateral frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia 29:959–967

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roser ME, Fugelsang JA, Dunbar KN, Corballis PM, Gazzaniga MS (2005) Dissociating processes supporting causal perception and causal inference in the brain. Neuropsychology 19:591–602

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roskes M, Sligte D, Shalvi S, De Dreu CK (2011) The right side? Under time pressure, approach motivation leads to right-oriented bias. Psychol Sci 22:1403–1407

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner BF (1948)’Superstition’ in the pigeon. J Exp Psychol 38:168–172

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sokunbi MO, Staff RT, Waiter GD, Ahearn TS, Fox HC, Deary IJ, Starr JM, Whalley LJ, Murray AD (2011) Inter-individual differences in fMRI entropy measurements in old age. IEEE Trans Bio-Medi Eng 58:3206–3214

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich KE, West RF (1998) Individual differences in rational thought. J Exp Psychol Gen 127:161–188

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanton BR, Carson A (2015) Apathy: a practical guide for neurologists. Pract Neurol 16:42–47

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steele CM (1992) Race and the schooling of black Americans. The Atlantic Monthly, April issue

  • Steele CM (1997) A threat in the air. How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance. Am Psychol 52:613–629

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens MC, Calhoun VD, Kiehl KA (2005) Hemispheric differences in hemodynamics elicited by auditory oddball stimuli. Neuroimage 26:782–792

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Stoet G (2016) Sex differences in the Simon task help to interpret sex differences in selective attention. Psychol Res 81:571–581

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tops M, Quirin M, Boksem MAS, Koole SL (2017) Large-scale neural networks and the lateralization of motivation and emotion. Int J Psychophysiol 119:41–49

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vallortigara G, Rogers LJ (2005) Survival with an asymmetrical brain: Advantages and disadvantages of cerebral lateralisation. Behav Brain Sci 28:575 589

    Google Scholar 

  • van den Bos R, Homberg J, de Visser L (2013) A critical review of sex differences in decision-making tasks: focus on the Iowa Gambling Task. Behav Brain Res 238:95–108

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vansteenkiste M, Lens W, De Witte S, De Witte H, Deci E (2004) The ‘why’ and ‘why not’ of job search behaviour: Their relation to searching, unemployment experience, and well-being. Eur J Soc Psychol 34:345–363

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vyse SA (1991) Behavioral variability and rule generation: general, restricted, and superstitious contingency. Psychol Record 41:487–506

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner GA, Morris EK (1987) “Superstitious” behavior in children. Psychological record 37:471–488

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wasserman IM, Richmond-Abbott M (2005) Gender and the Internet: Causes of variation in access, level, and scope of use. Soc Sci Q 86:252–270

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson TD, Reinhard DA, Westgate EC, Gilbert DT et al. (2014) Just think: the challenges of the disengaged mind. Science 345:75–77

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wisniewski AB (1998) Sexually-dimorphic patterns of cortical asymmetry, and the role for sex steroid hormones in determining cortical patterns of lateralization. Psychoneuroendocrinology 23:519–547

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolford G, Miller MB, Gazzaniga M (2000) The left hemisphere’s role in hypothesis formation. J Neurosci 20:RC64

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zung WWK (1965) A rating instrument for anxiety disorders. Psychosomatics 12:371–379

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors. The authors wish to thank two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Elena Daprati.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures were in accordance with the ethical standards of the local committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Daprati, E., Sirigu, A., Desmurget, M. et al. Willingness towards cognitive engagement: a preliminary study based on a behavioural entropy approach. Exp Brain Res 237, 995–1007 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-019-05482-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-019-05482-6

Keywords

Navigation