Introduction

In vision, binocular fusion is facilitated by vergence. Various different types of horizontal vergence have been distinguished based on inputs used by the vergence system, e.g. binocular disparity and accommodation. One of those, fusional vergence, uses binocular disparity as input. And another, proximal vergence, is associated with “knowledge of nearness” (Howard, 2002). Recently, the influence of depth perception on vergence has regained interest, because of the possibility to probe perception through vergence. In several studies, various depth cues were used to induce depth and the influence of perception on vergence was studied both without and with binocular disparity present. In this study, we investigate whether depth perception per se contributes to vergence.

Enright (1987b) measured vergence movements corresponding to the depth relations implied by perspective foreshortening, under monocular viewing conditions. He concluded that the perceived depth suggested by perspective foreshortening could elicit vergence. Ringach et al. (1996) used the kinetic depth effect to induce perception of depth. They measured vergence, also under monocular viewing conditions, and found that vergence corresponded to depth perceived by the subject. This led them to conclude that perceived depth can elicit vergence.

More recently, several studies investigated the influence of perceived depth on vergence in the presence of disparity, i.e. under binocular viewing conditions. Sheliga and Miles (2003) used Ogle’s induced effect in which vertical disparities give rise to depth. The authors reported that depending on the condition, a maximum of 41% of the vergence could be attributed to perceived depth. Both et al. (2003) used Werner’s illusion to induce depth. They reported slight vergence responses corresponding to perceived depth. These results led to the conclusion that perceived depth contributed to the vergence responses.Footnote 1

However, in all aforementioned studies, depth perception was correlated with stimulus properties, i.e. the depth cues giving rise to the perceived depth. Therefore, the distinction between an effect on vergence caused by perception and an effect due to depth cues could not be made. Thus, it may be possible that signals related to the depth cues contributed to vergence rather than perceived depth itself.

To investigate whether perceived depth itself is sufficient to influence vergence, a condition is required in which depth perception is not correlated with both monocular and binocular cues that give rise to the perceived depth (Allison et al. 1998; Gillam 1968; Gillam and Ryan 1992; Gillam and Cook 2001). The slant rivalry stimulus offers the possibility to dissociate vergence effects due to depth cues from those caused by perceived depth (van Ee et al. 2002). To create a slant rivalry stimulus, perspective foreshortening and binocular disparity are used to define slanted surfaces. If the cues specify opposite slant orientations, subjects report alternations between perceiving a slanted rectangle and perceiving an oppositely slanted trapezoid (van Ee et al. 2002). Thus, perceived slant alternates, whereas the depth cues remain constant.

Using these slant stimuli, the contribution to vergence predicted by perception is different from that predicted by the depth cues. If perceived depth is sufficient to influence vergence, a difference in vergence should be observed while alternations occur between the two possibly perceived surface slants. However, if perceived slant does not influence vergence, vergence should remain stable regardless of the perceived surface slant orientation.

To study the effect of perceived surface slant on vergence, we analyzed saccades made under various stimulus and perceptual conditions. Binocular saccades usually contain a conjugate component (version) and a disjunctive component (vergence) (Erkelens et al. 1989). Both components are preprogrammed, thus if perceived depth influences vergence, the disjunctive component should be related to the depth direction specified by the perceived slant. Hence, measured vergence at saccade offset can give insight into the input that drives the vergence system.

Methods

Experimental setup

Stimuli were displayed using a conventional Wheatstone stereoscope consisting of two TFT displays (20″ LaCie Photon20Vision II, 1,600 × 1,200, 75 Hz) and two small mirrors, see Fig. 1. The mirrors were slanted about the vertical axis at an angle of 45° with respect to the display. The virtual intersection point of the orthogonal mirrors was aligned with the center of the displays. Subjects were seated close to the mirrors so that the left eye could not see the right mirror and vice versa. The straight-ahead viewing distance (eye-mirror-display) was 57 cm. Subjects’ heads were fixated using a bite-board.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Wheatstone stereoscope. Subjects viewed one TFT display with the corresponding eye via one of the mirrors. The viewing distance (eye-mirror-display) was 57 cm. Note that the subjects were in reality much closer to the mirrors than depicted here and that there was no crossover, i.e. each eye could only see via one mirror

Eye movements were measured using the head-mounted Eyelink I system at 250 Hz. The cameras were positioned beneath the mirrors. The whole setup and experimental room were painted black matte and the room was darkened.

Stimuli

A stimulus consisted of two images displayed, one on each of the displays. The displayed images were generated using custom Open GL based software. The images used were trapezoidal or rectangular shapes composed of lines and a sparse random dot texture, as shown in Fig. 2. The images were surrounded by a fronto-parallel sparse random dot background (not shown).

Fig. 2
figure 2

An image as shown on one display. Perspective foreshortening indicates a slant of 70°. The disparity gradient was produced by horizontally scaling the two eyes’ half images. The red fixation cross is positioned in the center of the stimulus

Perspective foreshortening of both images corresponded with a 2D projection (cyclopean viewpoint) of a rectangular surface slanted (±) 70° about the vertical axis. A horizontal (binocular) disparity gradient defined a (∓) 50° slant about the vertical axis, which was produced by scaling the two eyes’ half images horizontally. After scaling, the horizontal angular width of the stimulus was 20° for all stimuli independent of the imposed slant angles, see Fig. 3a. The same figure also shows that a counter clockwise rotation (CCW) about the vertical axis was defined as a positive slant angle and a clockwise rotation (CW) as a negative slant angle.

Fig. 3
figure 3

a The subtended angle from center to left or right side of the stimulus was 10° (γ) irrespective of the imposed surface slants defined by disparity (D) and perspective (P). A counter clockwise rotation of the surface about the vertical axis is defined as a positive surface slant angle (+). b Examples of different stimulus conditions. The surface slant angle as defined by disparity was ±50° (α) and the perspective defined surface slant angle (β) was ± 70°. In the unambiguous stimulus, disparity and perspective defined the same surface orientation yielding a stable perception of surface slant (1). Whereas in the ambiguous stimulus condition (2), the orientations of the defined surfaces were opposite, giving a bistable perception of surface slant

Stimulus parameters could define any of the following possibilities: (1) a pair of trapezoids in which the signs of slant defined by perspective foreshortening (P) and disparity (D) were equal [unambiguous stimulus, e.g. P = 70° and D = 50°, see Fig. 3b(1)] or (2) a pair of trapezoids in which the signs of slant defined by perspective foreshortening and disparity were opposite [ambiguous stimulus, e.g. P = 70° and D = −50°, see Fig. 3b(2)]. These physical parameters led to the following perceived slanted surfaces: ad (1) a slanted rectangle (stable slant) or ad (2) alternations of a slanted rectangle and an oppositely slanted trapezoid (bistable slant). Note that the slant angle defined by the monocular cues is larger than the angle defined by the binocular cues, to produce a sufficient degree of bistability for all subjects. The disparity gradient could not be diminished, as this would deteriorate the signal-to-noise ratio of vergence (see Sect. 2.5 as well).

Furthermore, a standing disparity was added to all stimuli, which made all surfaces appear to be positioned in front of the display. A fixation cross was presented in the center of the stimulus.

Procedure and tasks

Experimental trials consisted of a sequence of five different displayed items. Subjects were first presented with a fixation dot (used for offline drift correction) in the center of the display at display depth for 1.5 s (1). This dot was replaced by a fixation cross at the location and depth corresponding to the center of the stimulus (2). After 1.5 s, the stimulus images were added (3). A beep was sounded 2.0 s after stimulus onset. A monocular arrow appeared 2 s thereafter in the left eye’s image (4). The screen was blanked after 4 s (5).

During the trial, subjects were instructed to fixate the cross. When the monocular arrow had appeared, subjects were instructed to make one single saccade towards the side of the surface indicated by the arrow. The arrow always pointed towards the near side of the slanted surface defined by disparity. By doing this the vergence response (disjunctive part of saccade) was maximized, because some subjects showed considerably less vergence change when saccades were made towards the uncrossed disparity side of the stimulus. This is in agreement with the effect of version–vergence ratio on the disjunctive part of the saccade as described by Enright (1984).

Subjects had to report their percept prior to saccade onset. They were asked to report the perceived nearest side of the stimulus after the beep using a numerical keypad. By reporting which stimulus side they perceived as nearest, subjects implicitly indicated whether they perceived a slanted rectangle or a slanted trapezoid.

When viewing a slant rivalry stimulus, subjects report perceiving spontaneous alternations of their percept. In addition, they are able to voluntarily control their perception as well (van Ee et al. 2002, 2005b). To obtain equal sized data sets for each of both perceptual conditions, we used this ability to voluntarily control perception of the ambiguous stimulus. Thus, subjects were instructed to hold one of both possible perceived surfaces when viewing the bistable stimulus before the experiment started.

One experimental block consisted of a total of 16 trials, of which 8 were ambiguous stimuli conditions and the other 8 were unambiguous stimuli conditions. Both conditions were counter balanced for saccade direction and trials were randomized for conditions and saccade directions. Each experimental session contained three to four experimental blocks run consecutively. A calibration was performed, at the start of each block, i.e. every 5 min.

Data analysis

Raw data provided by the Eyelink system was analyzed off line. We used a custom calibration procedure specifically designed to calibrate the raw data. This procedure consisted of a weighted least square error fit with a third order polynomial [35 measurement points, BFGS minimization technique (Press 1993)] (van Beers 2007). Offset correction was performed offline for each trial using the data obtained with the first fixation dot. On the calibrated data, saccade detection was performed based on version (mean gaze angle of left and right eye) velocity. A saccade was detected when the version velocity was higher than 120°/s. The end of the saccade was defined when version velocity dropped below 20°/s. Only saccades with an amplitude of at least 7° in the indicated direction were analyzed. After the saccade, a slower vergence movement was usually present, vergence calculated as right eye gaze data–left eye gaze data. This was assumed to be part of the initial preprogrammed vergence. We defined the preprogrammed vergence offset when vergence velocity was minimum within a timeframe of 100 ms after saccade offset and 5°/s at the most. We used the data at the point of minimum vergence velocity for further analysis. As we were interested in the vergence difference between the onset and offset of the saccade, we present the data relative to the gaze direction before onset of the saccade. Vergence data were averaged [three nearest neighbor average (24 ms interval)] to remove high-frequency noise.

Subjects

Subjects (6 females and 9 males aged between 18 and 30) had normal or corrected to normal vision. Before taking part in the experiment, it was checked whether a subject was able to comply with the instructions of the experiment. Not all potential subjects could perceive the trapezoidal interpretation of the stimulus within the timeframe of the experiment (2 s), when disparity and perspective foreshortening were in conflict. These subjects either had poor stereovision or needed more than 2 s to elicit a voluntary controlled flip. In total eight subjects were excluded from further participation, because it would have been impossible to measure the effect of depth perception on vergence.

Results

Saccades had normal main sequence characteristics (amplitudes of about 9°, durations of about 56 ms, maximum velocities of about 310°/s) (Collewijn et al. 1988). Most saccades reached an amplitude of 80–100% of the amplitude defined by the width of the stimulus. Sometimes a correction saccade was made after about 150 ms. Only a small fraction of saccades (less than 2%) was in the opposite direction as indicated by the monocular arrow presented.

Experiment 1: Contribution of perception of depth to vergence

To investigate whether perceived surface orientation contributes to vergence, we analyzed the data obtained during the presentation of the ambiguous stimulus. As mentioned in Sect. 2.3, we asked subjects to hold one of both possible orientations. In 52% of the 659 trials subjects reported perceiving a slanted rectangle and the slanted trapezoid was perceived in 48% of the trials. In 88% of all trials the percept was in agreement with the instruction given before the experiment started. The data of trials in which they failed (due to spontaneous or no (controlled) flips) are not included in the data presented here, but were not significantly different from the trials in which subjects were successful in obliging to the instruction.

Predictions

While the cues remained constant, perceived orientations alternated. Thus, we predict that if perception contributes to vergence, vergence should alternate accordingly. But, if not, the measured vergence changes should be independent of perceived slant orientation. Because we asked subjects to report only the sign of the orientation (and not the magnitude) of perceived slant, we can only predict the direction of the vergence changes, as illustrated in Fig. 4a. When subjects perceived the trapezoid (disparity-based percept), the saccades were directed towards the near side of the perceived surface, whereas saccades were directed towards the far side when subjects perceived the rectangle (perspective-based percept). Consequently, the vergence changes predicted by perception are in the converging direction (blue area) when subjects perceived a trapezoid and in the diverging direction (red area) when they perceived the rectangle. The upper and lower limits of the predicted vergence regions in Fig. 4a are defined by the vergence based on a zero depth change (grey line) and a vergence change based on the maximum slant defined by either depth cue: disparity (perception of trapezoid, blue line) or perspective (perception of rectangle, red line).

Fig. 4
figure 4

a Predicted vergence changes based on perceived surface slant orientation. The transitions between vergence at saccade onset (t = 0 ms) and at saccade offset are described using a sigmoid function. The upper and lower limits of the predicted vergence regions are based on the vergence step corresponding with the depth defined by a single cue. The box indicates the range of the detailed graphs in b. b Vergence changes based on perceived surface slant orientation of subject S6 (top) and subject S1 (bottom). Mean traces for each condition with SE of every fourth data point are displayed. Saccade offset occurred at 55 ms for S6 and at 46 ms for S1 conform main sequence characteristics (saccade onset at t = 0 ms). These data show that (1) saccade offset does not coincide with offset of preprogrammed vergence per se, (2) that the “perception of rectangle” condition is more converging than the “perception of trapezoid” condition from S6, which is opposite to the traces of S1

Results

Figure 4b shows representative vergence traces from the “perception of trapezoid” and “perception of rectangle” conditions. These vergence traces are means of 25 trials at the least, with standard error (SE) displayed at every fourth data point. Saccade onset (t = 0 ms) has been used to align the individual vergence traces for calculating the mean. Saccade offset occurred at t = 55 ms for S1 and at t = 46 ms for S6, which is in accordance with main sequence characteristics. Figure 4b shows that at this point in time, the preprogrammed vergence movement has not ceased. Preprogrammed vergence offset was determined for individual traces (see Sect. 2.4) and cannot be pinpointed exactly in these graphs. Preprogrammed vergence offset typically occurred at t = 120 ms (±30 ms), at which point the vergence velocity (mean of all subjects) was 0.2°/s (SD = 0.15°/s). The diverging movement present in the onset of the saccades is commonly observed in most conjugate saccades and caused by temporal/nasal eye movement differences (Collewijn et al. 1988).

As Fig. 4b shows, the vergence changed in the converging direction for both perceptual conditions. Moreover, all subjects showed converging responses. Thus, if subjects perceived a slanted trapezoid, vergence was in the same direction as the perceived orientation of the surface. And, if they perceived a slanted rectangle, the direction of vergence was opposite to the perceived orientation of the surface. This indicates that the contribution of perceived orientation to vergence, if any, is small. When comparing the data traces with the predictions in Fig. 4b, it is clear that the measured vergence changes are close to the value predicted by the disparity cue (blue dotted line). This result suggests that the largest contribution to vergence is related to the disparity cue.

To investigate the magnitude of the effect of perceived orientation on vergence, we further analyzed the data of all subjects, starting with selecting the end of the preprogrammed vergence movement as described in Sect. 2.4. We then normalized the data, using the calculated prediction based on the disparity cue, thereby effectively removing the variability caused by the interocular distance.Footnote 2 These normalized data are presented in Fig. 5a. Because these data show individual biases (e.g. S4), we used the difference between the two conditions tested for calculations on group data (see Fig. 5b). The data of leftward (bottom) and rightward (top) saccades are shown separately, because individual biases varied across these conditions (e.g. for S4). ANOVA statistics on individual and group data showed that there was no significant difference between the two investigated conditions (except for S1 leftward saccade, see Fig. 5 caption), thereby rejecting the hypothesis that perceived orientation influences vergence. Thus, vergence is independent of perceived orientation.

Fig. 5
figure 5

a Vergence (mean and SE selected as described in Sect. 2.4) relative to predicted vergence based on the disparity cue (0 on vertical axis) from leftward saccade trials (bottom) and rightward saccade trials (top). These data show that (1) there are individual biases (e.g. S4), (2) the “perception of rectangle” (red circle) is not less convergent than the “perception of trapezoid” (blue pentagon) condition for all subjects (e.g. S2, S5) and both directions (e.g. S4, S6), rebutting the predictions based on perception. b Vergence (mean and SE selected as described in Sect. 2.4) relative to “perception of a trapezoid” condition (0 on vertical axis). On the right side of these graphs the mean (and SE) of all subjects is shown. There is no systematic trend visible across subjects. All differences are nonsignificant except for S1, leftward saccade (F = 6.36, P < 0.05)

Experiment 2: Contribution of depth cues other than disparity to vergence

The results from experiment 1 show that perceived orientation does not influence vergence. Still, as stated in the introduction, cues other than disparity may contribute to vergence. We investigated this hypothesis by analyzing the data from the ambiguous stimulus and the unambiguous stimulus presentations.

Predictions

For both conditions the disparity defined slant was identical. In the unambiguous stimulus, the orientation of slant angle defined by perspective foreshortening was identical to the one defined by disparity, whereas it was opposite in the ambiguous stimulus. Thus, if perspective contributes to vergence, we predict that the vergence change of the ambiguous condition should be less convergent than that of the unambiguous condition. But, if not, vergence should be independent of the stimulus presented. Predictions of vergence changes based on slant cues are illustrated in Fig. 6a. For both conditions, the disparity cue signaled a slant of 50° in magnitude and as saccades were directed towards the crossed disparity side of the stimulus, the corresponding predicted vergence change converges (blue dotted line). The perspective cue signaled a slant of 70° in magnitude in both conditions. In the unambiguous condition, the vergence change predicted by perspective converges (green dashed line) and consequently, the predicted vergence change in the ambiguous condition diverges (red line).

Fig. 6
figure 6

a Predicted vergence changes based on the slant cues of the stimulus. The predicted values are based on the depth defined by a single cue. Details similar as in Fig. 4. b Vergence changes from ambiguous and unambiguous stimulus trials of subject S6 (top) and subject S1 (bottom). Mean traces of each condition with SE of every fourth data point are displayed. Saccade offset occurred at 56 ms for S6 and 46 ms for S1 (saccade onset at t = 0 ms). These data show that the vergence step of the ambiguous stimulus condition is less convergent than the vergence step of the unambiguous stimulus condition for both subjects

Results

Figure 6b shows representative traces of the unambiguous and ambiguous stimulus conditions. The vergence traces are means of 25 trials at the least with the SE displayed at every fourth data point. The characteristics of these vergence traces are similar to the traces in Fig. 4b. The vergence traces in Fig. 6b both converge towards the predicted value based on the disparity cue, see Fig. 6a (blue dotted line). Thus, the contribution to vergence that can be related to disparity is relatively large, consistent with the results described before.

We further analyzed the data to investigate whether there was a significant effect of the perspective cue on vergence, using the same method as described in the previous subsection. Figure 7a shows the normalized data, whereas Fig. 7b displays the data from the ambiguous condition relative to the data from the unambiguous condition including the group means. Leftward (bottom) and rightward (top) saccades are shown separately. ANOVA statistics on group and individual data show that there is a significant difference between the ambiguous and unambiguous condition. The statistics for group data are: leftward saccades (lw) (difference = 0.14°, F = 13.04, P < 0.05) and rightward saccades (rw) (difference = 0.16°, F = 62.89, P < 0.001). And for individual subjects: S0 lw (F = 11.29, P < 0.01), S1 lw (F = 14.50, P < 0.001) and rw (F = 4.04, P < 0.05), S2 lw (F = 25.22, P < 0.0001) and rw (F = 24.14, P < 0.0001), S4 lw (F = 24.84, P < 0.0001), S6 lw (F = 5.22, P < 0.05) and rw (F = 11.02, P < 0.01).

Fig. 7
figure 7

a Vergence (mean and SE selected as described in Sect. 2.4) relative to predicted vergence based on the disparity cue (0 on vertical axis) from leftward saccade trials (bottom) and rightward saccade trials (top). Note that (1) there are some large individual biases, (2) that the ambiguous condition (purple triangle) is consistently less convergent than the unambiguous condition (cyan square), supporting the predictions based on slant cues. b Vergence (mean and SE) relative to unambiguous condition. On the right side of these graphs the mean of all subjects is shown. The systematic trend visible between and across subjects is significant for the whole group (leftward saccades: F = 13.04, P < 0.05 and rightward: F = 62.89, P < 0.001). All significant differences are denoted by a star (values are stated in text)

Thus, perspective, or signals related to it, significantly contributed to vergence.

Discussion

Our findings show that depth cues rather than depth perception itself contribute to vergence accompanying saccadic movements. Perspective being congruent or incongruent with disparity caused a difference of about 14% in vergence changes predicted by disparity alone. Although our results show that vergence remained constant in the ambiguous stimulus condition, subjects perceived alternations of surface orientation. Collectively, these results show that perspective and disparity are each weighted differently for perception and vergence.

Binocular studies

Our result on the influence of perception on vergence refines the conclusions of previous studies relating perception and vergence (Sheliga and Miles 2003; Both et al. 2003). These studies investigated the influence of perception on vergence under binocular viewing conditions, similar as in our study. However, contributions of perception of depth to vergence could not be dissociated from those of depth-inducing cues. This dissociation was possible in our study. We found that, perspective contributes to vergence, independent of its perceptual effect. This conclusion suggests that vertical disparities (Sheliga and Miles 2003), global horizontal disparities (Both et al. 2003) and perspective (present study) contribute to vergence similarly as local horizontal disparities do, albeit to a lesser extent.

Monocular studies

The perspective studies of Enright (1987a, b) show that vergence occurs corresponding to the depth implied by (linear) perspective under monocular viewing conditions. Enright concluded that the perceived depth contributed to vergence. However, in light of the present study, his results show that depth cues contribute to vergence under monocular viewing conditions as well. Moreover, without the dominantly contributing factor (disparity) being present, other depth cues become the main contributors to vergence.

The Necker cube study of Enright (1987a, b) and the ambiguous rotating sphere study of Ringach et al. (1996) seem to provide similar stimulus conditions as our experiment. The single depth cue used induced alternations in depth perception. In both studies corresponding alternations in vergence were measured and both authors conclude that depth perception induced vergence.

However, each depth cue used in these studies was inherently ambiguous. Thus, in these studies, the contribution of perception of depth to vergence could again not be dissociated from the contribution of the depth cues themselves. Taking the present study into consideration, there is evidence that their results reflect the alternations of the depth cue related signals and not those related to perception. In view of this conclusion, the fact that the vergence changes measured by Enright in the Necker cube experiment were much smaller than those in the linear perspective experiment may be explained by a degradation of the signals related to the depth cues due to the ambiguity herein.

Influence of perception on other visual phenomena

Our results indicate that vergence is influenced by low-level (sensory) processes and not by high-level (voluntary control) processes. Studies on bistability (van Ee et al. 2005b, a; Toppino 2003) show that high-level mechanisms can influence perception, whereas, as shown in the present study, they do not influence vergence. A recent study by Knapen and van Ee (2006) shows that slant adaptation is not influenced by perception or by voluntary control of perception, but is solely based on depth cues in the (slant rivalry) stimulus. Thus, both adaptation and vergence are influenced by low-level mechanisms, but not by high-level mechanisms.

Different weighting of cues for vergence and perception

To explain the current results and those of Enright (1987a, b) and Ringach et al. (1996), a single 3D surface representation used for perception and binocular eye movements as suggested by Ringach et al. (1996) is inappropriate. The perceptual states and the alternations thereof are not reflected in the vergence responses, suggesting that two separate representations are used for perception and eye movements. Moreover, the present results suggest that the weights assigned to signals related to monocular and binocular depth cues are different for perception and vergence, resulting in either alternating or stable outcomes respectively, see Fig. 8. These outcomes may construct two separate 3D space representations one used for perception and the other for vergence or, as an alternative, one 3D representation for perception whereas the signals for vergence are directly wired to the oculomotor system. However, in the latter situation the monocular signals must still be disambiguated before the motor command can be given to reorient the eyes.

Fig. 8
figure 8

Perception and vergence are based on separate processing streams. Both monocular cues (such as relative motion, linear perspective, blur and looming) and binocular cues [such as horizontal disparity (global and local) and global vertical disparity] are used for perception as well as for vergence. The weights assigned to the individual cues might be different when used for perception than when used for vergence, resulting in different outcomes. In the present study, for example, the ambiguous stimulus yields a bistable perception of surface slant, whereas it yields a stable vergence angle. Cognitive factors, such as voluntary control, do not seem to exert influence on vergence, but they do influence perception

Perception and action

Our results constitute another example of a dissociation between perception and action systems as suggested by Goodale and Milner (1992). Since then evidence supporting a dissociation has been accumulating, for a review see Goodale and Westwood (2004), Carey (2001) and Bruno (2001). Haffenden and Goodale (2000) and Haffenden et al. (2001) have shown that there are conditions in which perception has no influence on action, whereas other studies (Erkelens and Collewijn 1985; Masson et al. 1997; Schreiber et al. 2001; Banks et al. 2001; van Ee and van Dam 2003) have shown that conditions exist in which eye movements have no influence on visual perception. On the other hand, some studies conclude that action is influenced by perception (Franz et al. 2000, 2001; Franz 2001; Smeets et al. 2002; Lopez-Moliner et al. 2003; de Grave et al. 2006a, b; Bernardis et al. 2005; Knox and Bruno 2007). As we have shown all depth cues are used by the vergence system. The use of pictorial depth cues by other motor systems could perhaps explain this incongruence.

Conclusion

Our findings show that depth cues rather than perceived depth govern vergence that accompanies saccades. Perspective being congruent or incongruent with disparity caused a 14% difference in vergence change. These results combined with other studies show that in addition to local horizontal disparity, also global disparity, relative motion and perspective are used as input by the vergence system. Furthermore, our findings suggest that monocular and binocular cues are weighted differently for perception and vergence.