Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Translational motion perception and vestiboocular responses in the absence of non-inertial cues

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Path integration studies in humans show that we have the ability to accurately reproduce our path in the absence of visual information. It has been suggested that this ability is supported by acceleration signals, as transduced by the otolith organs, which may then be integrated twice to produce path excursion. Vestibuloocular responses to linear translations (LVOR), however, show considerable frequency dependence, with substantial attenuation in response to low frequency translational motion. If otolith information were processed similarly by path integration mechanisms, the resulting signal would not be sufficient to account for robust path integration for stimuli typically used in such studies. We hypothesized that such behavior relies upon cognitive skill and transient otolith cues, typically combined with non-directional cues of motion, such as vibration and noise produced by the mechanics apparatus used to produce linear motion. Continuous motion estimation tasks were used to assess translation perception, while eye movement recordings revealed LVOR responses, in 12 normal and 2 vestibulopathic human subjects while riding on a sled designed to specifically minimize non-directional motion cues. In the near absence of such cues, perceptual responses, like the LVOR, showed high-pass characteristics. This implies that otolith signals are not sufficient to support previously observed path integration behaviors, which must be supplemented by non-directional motion cues.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Angelaki DE, McHenry MQ, Dickman JD, Newlands SD, Hess BJ (1999) Computation of inertial motion: neural strategies to resolve ambiguous otolith information. J Neurosci 19(1):316–327

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • AuYong N, Paige GD, Seidman SH (2007) Multiple sensory cues underlying the perception of translation and path. J Neurophysiol 97:1100–1113

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnes GR (1988) Head-eye co-ordination: visual and nonvisual mechanisms of vestibulo-ocular reflex slow-phase modification. Prog Brain Res 76:319–328

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bendat JS, Piersol AG (1986) Random data: analysis and measurement procedures. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Benson AJ, Spencer MB, Stott JR (1986) Thresholds for the detection of the direction of whole-body, linear movement in the horizontal plane. Aviation Space Environ Med 57(11):1088–1096

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Berthoz A, Pavard B, Young LR (1975) Perception of linear horizontal self-motion induced by peripheral vision (linearvection) basic characteristics and visual-vestibular interactions. Exp Brain Res 23(5):471–489

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Berthoz A, Israël I, Vieville T, Zee D (1987) Linear head displacement measured by the otoliths can be reproduced through the saccadic system. Neurosci Lett 82(3):285–290

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Berthoz A, Israël I, Georges-Francois P, Grasso R, Tsuzuku T (1995) Spatial memory of body linear displacement: What is being stored? Science 269:95–98

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Buizza A, Leger A, Berthoz A, Schmid R (1979) Otolithic-acoustic interaction in the control of eye movement. Exp Brain Res 36(3):509–522

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Busettini C, Miles FA, Schwarz U, Carl JR (1994) Human ocular responses to translation of the observer and of the scene: dependence on viewing distance. Exp Brain Res 100:484–494

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Calkins DS (1998) Examination of two methods for statistical analysis of data with magnitude and direction emphasizing vestibular research applications. J Vestibular Res 8(4):335–340

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen MM (1973) Elevator illusion: influences of otolith organ activity and neck proprioception. Percept Psychophys 14(3):401–406

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen MM, Crosbie RJ, Blackburn LH (1973) Disorienting effects of aircraft catapult launchings. Aerospace Med 44(1):37–39

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Crane BT, Tian J, Wiest G, Demer JL (2003) Initiation of the human heave linear vestibulo-ocular reflex. Exp Brain Res 148(2):247–255

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fernández C, Goldberg JM (1976a) Physiology of peripheral neurons innervating otolith organs of the squirrel monkey. I. Response to static tilts and to long-duration centrifugal force. J Neurophysiol 39(5):970–984

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fernández C, Goldberg JM (1976b) Physiology of peripheral neurons innervating otolith organs of the squirrel monkey. III. Response dynamics. J Neurophysiol 39(5):996–1008

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gauthier GM, Hofferer JM (1976) Eye tracking of self-moved targets in the absence of vision. Exp Brain Res 26(2):121–139

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Glasauer S (1995) Linear acceleration perception: frequency dependence of the hilltop illusion. Acta Otolaryngol (Stockh) Supp 520:37–40

    Google Scholar 

  • Graybiel A (1952) Oculogravic illusion. A M A Arch Ophthal 48:605–615

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Graybiel A, Clark B (1965) Validity of the oculogravic illusion as a specific indicator of otolith function. Aerospace Med 36:1173–1181

    Google Scholar 

  • Graybiel A, Miller EF, Newsom BD, Kennedy RS (1968) The effect of water immersion on perception of the oculogravic illusion in normal and labyrinthine-defective subjects. Acta Otolaryngol (Stockh) 65:599–610

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Grossman GE, Leigh RJ, Bruce EN, Huebner WP, Lanska DJ (1989) Performance of the human vestibuloocular reflex during locomotion. J Neurophysiol 62:264–272

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Guedry FE (1974) Psychophysics of vestibular sensation. In: Kornhuber HH (ed) Handbook of sensory physiology, vol VI/2: Vestibular system. Springer, Berlin, pp 3–154

  • Guedry FE, Harris CS (1963) Labyrinthine function related to experiments on the parallel swing. NASA Joint Report, Pensacola

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris LR, Jenkin M, Zikovitz DC (2000) Visual and non-visual cues in the perception of linear self-motion. Exp Brain Res 135(1):12–21

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hlavacka F, Mergner T, Bolha B (1996) Human self-motion perception during translatory vestibular and proprioceptive stimulation. Neurosci Lett 210:83–86

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Israël I, Berthoz A (1989) Contribution of the otoliths to the calculation of linear displacement. J Neurophysiol 62(1):247–263

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Israël I, Fetter M, Koenig E (1993) Vestibular perception of passive whole-body rotation about horizontal and vertical axes in humans: goal-directed vestibulo-ocular reflex and vestibular memory-contingent saccades. Exp Brain Res 96:335–346

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Israël I, Grasso R, Georges-Francois P, Tsuzuku T, Berthoz A (1997) Spatial memory and path integration studied by self-driven passive linear displacement. I Basic properties J Neurophysiol 77(6):3180–3192

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Israël I, Capelli A, Sable D, Laurent C, Lecoq C, Bredin J (2004) Multifactorial interactions involved in linear self-transport distance estimate: a place for time. Int J Psychophysiol 53(1):21–28

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kumar A, Krol G (1992) Binocular infrared oculography. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol 102:367–378

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews ML, Cousins LR (1980) The influence of vehicle type on the estimation of velocity while driving. Ergonomics 23:1151–1160

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayne R (1974) A systems concept of the vestibular organs. In: Kornhuber HH (ed) Handbook of sensory physiology, vol VI/2: Vestibular System. Springer, Heidelberg, pp 493–580

  • Merfeld DM, Young LR (1995) The vestibulo-ocular reflex of the squirrel monkey during eccentric rotation and roll tilt. Exp Brain Res 106(1):111–122

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Merfeld DM, Zupan LH (2002a) Neural processing of gravitoinertial cues in humans. III. Modeling tilt and translation responses. J Neurophysiol 87(2):819–833

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Merfeld DM, Zupan LH (2002b) Neural processing of gravitoinertial cues in humans. III. Modeling tilt and translation responses. J Neurophysiol 87(2):819–833

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Merfeld DM, Christie JR, Young LR (1994) Perceptual and eye movement responses elicited by linear acceleration following spaceflight. Aviation Space Environ Med 65(11):1015–1024

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Merfeld DM, Teiwes W, Clarke AH, Scherer H, Young LR (1996) The dynamic contributions of the otolith organs to human ocular torsion. Exp Brain Res 110(2):315–321

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Merfeld DM, Park S, Gianna-Poulin C, Black FO, Wood S (2005a) Vestibular perception and action employ qualitatively different mechanisms. I. Frequency response of VOR and perceptual responses during translation and tilt. J Neurophysiol 94(1):186–198

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Merfeld DM, Park S, Gianna-Poulin C, Black FO, Wood S (2005b) Vestibular perception and action employ qualitatively different mechanisms. II. VOR and perceptual responses during combined Tilt&Translation. J Neurophysiol 94(1):199–205

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mergner T, Rumberger A, Becker W (1996) Is perceived angular displacement the time integral of perceived angular velocity? Brain Research Bulletin.40(5–6):467–70; discussion 470–1

    Google Scholar 

  • Mittelstaedt ML, Mittelstaedt H (2001) Idiothetic navigation in humans: estimation of path length. Exp Brain Res 139(3):318–332

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nashner LM (1971) A model describing vestibular detection of body sway motion. Acta Otolaryngol 72(6):429–436

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Niven JI, Hixson WC, Correia MJ (1966) Elicitation of horizontal nystagmus by periodic linear acceleration. Acta Oto-Laryngologica 62(4):429–441

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Paige GD (1989) The influence of target distance on eye movement responses during vertical linear motion. Exp Brain Res 77:585–593

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Paige GD, Tomko DL (1991) Eye movement responses to linear head motion in the squirrel monkey. II. Visual-vestibular interactions and kinematic considerations. J Neurophysiol 65:1183–1196

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Paige GD, Telford L, Seidman SH, Barnes GR (1998) Human vestibuloocular reflex and its interactions with vision and fixation distance during linear and angular head movement. J Neurophysiol 80:2391–2404

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schöne H, Mortag HG (1968) Variation of the subjective vertical on the parallel swing at different body positions. Psychol Forsch 32:124–134

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seguinot V, Maurer R, Etienne AS (1993) Dead reckoning in a small mammal: the evaluation of distance. J Comparat Physiol A Sensory Neural Behav Physiol 173(1):103–113

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Seidman SH (2005) Statistical methods for the comparison of vestibular response vectors: Post hoc testing for phase differences. Program No. 168.14. 2005 Abstract Viewer and Itinerary Planner. Society for Neuroscience, Washington, DC (Online)

  • Seidman SH, Paige GD (1996) Perception and eye movement during low-frequency centripetal acceleration. Ann N Y Acad Sci 781:693–695

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Seidman SH, Telford L, Paige GD (1998) Tilt perception during dynamic linear acceleration. Exp Brain Res 119:307–314

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Seidman SH, Bush G, Paige GD (2003) Human perceptual and oculomotor responses to translation: implications for path integration for time- and frequency-domain analysis. Program No. 593.4. 2003 Abstract Viewer and Itinerary Planner. Society for Neuroscience, Washington, DC (Online)

  • Telford L, Seidman SH, Paige GD (1997) Dynamics of squirrel monkey linear vestibuloocular reflex and interactions with fixation distance. J Neurophysiol 78:1775–1790

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wertheim AH, Mesland BS, Bles W (2001) Cognitive suppression of tilt sensations during linear horizontal self-motion in the dark. Perception 30(6):733–741

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zupan LH, Merfeld DM (2005) Human ocular torsion and perceived roll responses to linear acceleration. J Vestibular Res 15(4):173–183

    Google Scholar 

  • Zupan LH, Merfeld DM, Darlot C (2002) Using sensory weighting to model the influence of canal, otolith and visual cues on spatial orientation and eye movements. Biol Cybern 86(3):209–230

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by NIH RO1 DC04153, P30 DC005409 and NASAARC LifeSciDiv. Task 199-97-62-14. The author would like to thank Geoff Bush for his monumental aid and coordination efforts at NASA Moffett Field, as well as David Tomko, Lee Stone, the staff of the Vestibular Research Facility, to G.D. Paige for the availability and screening of vestibular patients, and Ed Freedman for his editorial assistance. This work was supported by NIH grants DC-04153, DC-01935, DC-005409, EY-01389 (to the Center for Visual Science), and NASAARC LifeSciDiv. Task 199-97-62-14.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to S. H. Seidman.

Appendix

Appendix

Post-hoc comparison of phase:

Statistical treatment of vector data, which contains both gain and phase information, is most conveniently carried out by converting to a rectangular coordinate scheme, and then employing multivariate analysis techniques (Calkins 1998). As described the text, gain (G) and phase (Θ) data are converted to the complex form X + iY (Fig. 8), where X = G·cosΘ, Y = G·sinΘ, and i is the imaginary operator (the square root of −1), and compared using MANOVA on the XY plane.

Fig. 8
figure 8

Comparison of two vector populations (crosses and circles) using zero-intercept linear fits. The vectors shown (dark and light arrows) reflect the vector means of the respective populations. Dashed lines show the lines of best fit, and the light grey lines, the 95% confidence interval for the fits. Inset: Representation of vector data on complex plane. The magnitude is expressed as the length of the vector, and the phase as the rotation from the real axis

If a difference is shown, post hoc analysis can be used to test gain and phase. Gain comparison can be carried out using simple ANOVA techniques. Phase comparison is complicated by the substantial interactions between gain and phase: as the magnitude of a vector becomes small, the phase becomes more difficult to determine, and as the magnitude approaches zero, phase estimates can become meaningless. Thus, small vectors for which phases are determined with low precision can cause artificially large variance in phase, hampering statistical comparison.

The resulting data points (still represented in Cartesian coordinates) are then submitted to a zero-intercept linear regression, and a resultant slope that is found to statistically differ from zero (equivalent to a non-zero Y component) is then used as an indication of a significant change in phase angle.

To test for differences in phase, we took advantage of the relationship between the angle of the vector population and the slope of the line fit to the equation Y = aX; a linear regression fitting the real component of the vector population to the imaginary component, and forced to pass through the origin (i.e., a zero-intercept linear regression). Two (or more) vector populations can be fit to this equation, and the resulting slopes are then statistically compared, with significant difference indicating a difference in phase (Seidman 2005).

The equation yielding the estimate of slope \( (\hat{a}) \) in a simple zero intercept regression is:

$$ {\mathop a\limits^ \wedge } = \frac{{{\sum\nolimits_{i = 1}^n {y_{i} x_{i} } }}} {{{\sum\nolimits_{i = 1}^n {x^{2}_{i} } }}}, $$

and the confidence interval on the slope is given by

$$ {\mathop a\limits^ \wedge } - t_{{\alpha /2,n - 1}} {\sqrt {\frac{{{\text{MS}}_{{\text{E}}} }} {{{\sum {x^{2}_{i} } }}}} } \le a \le {\mathop a\limits^ \wedge } + t_{{\alpha /2,n - 1}} {\sqrt {\frac{{{\text{MS}}_{{\text{E}}} }} {{{\sum {x^{2}_{i} } }}}} }, $$

where 1-α is the confidence interval and MSE is mean-square error.

Figure 8 shows the post-hoc comparison of two simulated data sets (black vs. grey) that are multivariate normally distributed in the complex plane. The mean vectors are shown as arrows extending from the origin. The zero-intercept line of best fit, shown as dashed lines, approach the angle of the vector mean. The 95% confidence intervals on slope are shown as dotted lines. For these particular simulated data sets, phase shows a significant difference, as indicated by clearly different slopes of the lines of best fit (P < 0.0001). Note that vectors that are small with respect to the remainder of the data set will not alter the slope of the lines of best fit by a substantial amount, regardless of their phase, as the residual for relatively small data points will remain small by virtue of proximity to the origin. In contrast, large-magnitude vectors of phase that is different from the remainder of the data set (i.e., phase outliers) can have dramatic effect on the estimated slope, but will also increase the size of the confidence interval because of increased mean-square error.

Testing on simulated data sets with multivariate normal distributions shows that results are quite similar to those of a direct statistical comparison of phase angles. However, as expected, the zero-intercept slope method is quite robust in the presence of phase outliers that are of small magnitude. Of course, a zero-intercept line extends in two directions from the origin, so care must be taken when comparing vector populations that approach 180° phase difference. As this method assesses differences in the slopes of vectors, and a vector has the same slope as a vector that differs in phase by 180°, this method will show no significant difference in this situation.

A complete regimen for statistical testing of vectorial data is thus:

  • Representation of data in complex plane.

  • MANOVA to determine difference between data sets.

  • ANOVA or t test on magnitude.

  • Test to see if either data set includes origin in complex plane, which would make phase meaningless.

  • Test for difference in slope of zero-intercept fit.

  • Check non-significant differences for 180° phase shift, which this method will not detect.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Seidman, S.H. Translational motion perception and vestiboocular responses in the absence of non-inertial cues. Exp Brain Res 184, 13–29 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-007-1072-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-007-1072-3

Keywords

Navigation