Introduction

Schizophrenia is a debilitating psychiatric disorder, which is complicated substantially by comorbid drug use problems. Rates of substance use are up to five time higher in individuals with schizophrenia compared to the population at large, and this occurs across a range of drugs of abuse, including nicotine, cannabis, alcohol and psychostimulants such as cocaine and methamphetamine (Dixon 1999; Khokhar et al. 2018; Volkow 2009). Comorbid substance use significantly impairs patient welfare and quality of life, worsening symptoms, decreasing treatment compliance, increasing the risk of psychotic relapse and hospitalisation, and elevating suicide risk (Dixon 1999; Khokhar et al. 2018). It is unclear why individuals with schizophrenia are at greater risk of substance use problems, and why they continue with problematic drug use despite this worsening disease symptoms and progression.

Several theories have been advanced to explain comorbidity between schizophrenia and drug use [reviews: (Green et al. 2007; Khokhar et al. 2018)]. Khokar and colleagues recently proposed a unifying theory, whereby genetic risk for schizophrenia leads to dysfunctional brain reward circuits, which can increase substance use rates in adolescence that triggers schizophrenia development and increases the likelihood of drug use problems in later life, after the development of schizophrenia (Khokhar et al. 2018). Considering the significant role that genetic risk for schizophrenia is hypothesised to play in risk for drug use problems, it is of considerable interest to untangle this relationship.

Investigations into how schizophrenia genetic risk leads to drug abuse susceptibility are still in their infancy. Clinical studies have linked generalised genetic and environmental risk for schizophrenia (e.g. polygenic risk scores) with susceptibility to the abuse of alcohol [(Reginsson et al. 2018) but see conflicting results: (Miller et al. 2019)], nicotine/tobacco (Chen et al. 2016; Miller et al. 2019; Reginsson et al. 2018) and cannabis (Martin et al. 2014; Pasman et al. 2018). A limited number of gene mutations which increase risk for developing schizophrenia appear associated with cannabis abuse [e.g. Neuregulin 1 (NRG1) (Han et al. 2012) see also (Aukst Margetic et al. 2019)] and alcohol abuse [e.g. brain derived neurotrophic factor (Cheah et al. 2014; Zai et al. 2018), Neuregulin 1 (NRG1) (Vaht et al. 2017)]; however, no association has been shown for other genetic variants [e.g. cocaine and amphetamine regulated transcript with nicotine and alcohol abuse (Busto et al. 2010)]. Together, these clinical findings provide some initial support for genes which increase risk for developing schizophrenia also increasing susceptibility for drug use problems. However, examining this research question in clinical cohorts can be confounded by issues such as polydrug use, the polygenetic nature of schizophrenia, and use of antipsychotic medications in patient cohorts. Rodent models provide an excellent tool to investigate causes of comorbidity between substance use and schizophrenia in a controlled manner without putting patients at risk (e.g. symptom intensification, increased risk of relapse). They provide insights into how genetic or environmental factors or combinations of these factors, which can increase the risk of developing schizophrenia, may also increase drug abuse liability and the mechanisms driving increased addiction susceptibility (e.g. increased cue reactivity, susceptibility to relapse, elevated drug reinforcement) (Brown et al. 2012; Menne and Chesworth 2020; Ng et al. 2013). However, to date there has been very limited investigation of how risk genes for schizophrenia affect behaviours relevant for substance use disorders (e.g. Gancarz et al. 2016; Hikida et al. 2020).

Neuregulin 1 (NRG1) is well-established risk gene for schizophrenia [review: (Mostaid et al. 2016)], and a missense mutation in the transmembrane domain of NRG1 is associated with schizophrenia (Walss-Bass et al. 2006). There is some clinical data linking NRG1 with substance use disorders (Aukst Margetic et al. 2019; Han et al. 2012; Vaht et al. 2017), but this is yet to be systematically investigated. A mouse model for mutant NRG1, the Nrg1 transmembrane domain heterozygous (Nrg1 TM HET) mouse, exhibits behavioural and neurochemical sensitivity to the main psychoactive component of cannabis, ∆9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) (Boucher et al. 2007a, 2007b; Long et al. 2010, 2013) and other cannabinoid compounds (Boucher et al. 2011), and increased sensitivity to methamphetamine in adolescence (Spencer et al. 2012). Despite Nrg1 TM HET mice showing increased susceptibility to the effects of cannabinoids and methamphetamine on schizophrenia-relevant behaviours (e.g. locomotion, social behaviours, sensorimotor gating), behaviours relevant to substance use problems have not been examined in these mice (e.g. drug reward and self-administration, extinction and reinstatement, locomotor sensitization).

Thus, we examined substance use-relevant behaviours for cocaine as well as a natural reinforcer, sucrose in Nrg1 TM HET mice. We used cocaine because rates of cocaine use in schizophrenia patients are more than 5 × higher than the general population (Sara et al. 2014; Volkow 2009), and while Nrg1 TM HET mice show increased susceptibility to the locomotor stimulating effects of the psychostimulant methamphetamine (Spencer et al. 2012), the response of these mice to cocaine is completely unknown. We used adult male mice in this study, as the schizophrenia-relevant phenotype of Nrg1 mutants, as well as their susceptibility to cannabinoids is not as pronounced in female mice of this line (Chesworth et al. 2012a; Long et al. 2010). We hypothesised that Nrg1 TM HET mice might exhibit increased sensitivity to the rewarding and reinforcing, as well as the locomotor stimulating properties of cocaine. To thoroughly address this question and evaluate if changes to cocaine-relevant behaviours extended to other reinforcers, we examined if changes to cocaine-induced behaviours were also evident using a natural reinforcer, sucrose.

Materials and methods

Animals

Male heterozygous Nrg1 TM+/− mice (Nrg1 TM HET) and control Nrg1 TM+/+ (wild-type-like, WT) littermates were bred as described previously (Karl et al. 2007). Mice were bred and housed at Australian BioResources (Moss Vale, Australia). Mice were housed with their dams in litters prior to weaning at PND 21, when they were group-housed in individually ventilated cages (Type Mouse Version 1; Airlaw, Smithfield, Australia; air change: 90–120 times per hour averaged; air speed: 0.12 m/s; passive exhaust ventilation system). Genotypes were confirmed after weaning using tail tip biopsy and polymerase chain reaction amplification (Karl et al. 2007).

At least 2 weeks prior to testing, adult animal animals were transferred to the animal facility at Western Sydney University (School of Medicine, Campbelltown, Australia). Mice were group housed (2–4/cage) in filter top cages (Type 1284B; Tecniplast, Rydalmere, Australia) with corn cob bedding and a wire lid, providing climbing opportunities, a mouse igloo (Bioserv, Frenchtown, USA) and tissues for nesting material. Mice were kept under a 12:12-h light:dark schedule (lights on 9am–9 pm), and food and water were available ad libitum. All animals were 6–7 months of age at the commencement of behavioural testing (average age 7.1 months), in accordance with the age-dependent phenotype observed for this model after 5–6 months of age (Karl et al. 2007). Separate cohorts of experimentally naïve mice were used. Samples sizes are as follows: Experiment 1 (5 mg/kg cocaine CPP): 14 WT, 14 Nrg1 TM HET. Experiment 2 (10 mg/kg cocaine CPP): 9 WT, 9 Nrg1 TM HET. Experiment 3 (20 mg/kg cocaine CPP): 16 WT, 9 Nrg1 TM HET. Experiment 4 (30 mg/kg cocaine CPP): 15 WT, 14 Nrg1 TM HET. Experiment 5 (cocaine intravenous self-administration Dose response): 10 WT, 7 Nrg1 TM HET. Experiment 6 (cocaine intravenous self-administration extinction and reinstatement): 25 WT, 15 Nrg1 TM HET. Experiment 7 (sucrose self-administration): 14 WT, 9 Nrg1 TM HET.

Research and animal care procedures were approved by the Western Sydney University Animal Care and Ethics Committee (ACEC) in accordance with the Australian Code of Practice for the Care and Use of Animals for Scientific Purposes (ACEC approval numbers: #A12386, A12135).

Drugs

Cocaine hydrochloride (National Measurements Institute, ACT, Australia) was dissolved in 0.9% saline. Vehicle injections were 0.9% saline. All injections were given intraperitoneally (i.p.) at a volume of 10 ml/kg body weight as published previously (Chesworth et al. 2013, 2016).

Conditioned place preference (CPP) - Experiments 1–4

CPP apparatus and methods are identical to those published previously (Chesworth and Karl 2020; Chesworth et al. 2021). The CPP apparatus was a modified open field arena (43.2 × 43.2 × 30.3 cm, Med Associates Inc., VT, USA), with a black Perspex divider separating two equally sized compartments. A small gate (11 × 9 cm) allowed access to both compartments. Wall patterns were used to distinguish the two compartments, with white walls on the left side of the apparatus, and black spots on a white background on the right side of the apparatus (Chesworth and Karl 2020; Chesworth et al. 2021).

An overview of CPP testing is provided in Supplementary Table S1.

Home cages of test mice were transferred into the experimental room at least 30 min prior to testing. On day 1 (habituation), mice were placed randomly in either compartment and allowed free access to the entire apparatus for 30 min. The time spent in each compartment during habituation was used to allocate drug pairings. Mice with a neutral preference (45–55% for either side) were randomly allocated their drug-paired side (unbiased allocation). For mice preferring one side, the drug was paired with the other side (biased allocation) (Chesworth and Karl 2020; Chesworth et al. 2021).

On days 2–4 (conditioning), mice received i.p. injections of saline or cocaine and were immediately confined into one of the two conditioning compartments. Saline conditioning was conducted in the morning (30 min after light phase onset, 0930) and cocaine conditioning (1400) was conducted in the afternoon, with at least 5 h separating the start of vehicle and drug conditioning sessions. Conditioning sessions were 30 min (Chesworth and Karl 2020; Chesworth et al. 2021). This circadian bias was intentional, to ensure cocaine was no longer circulating during saline conditioning sessions. This approach also ensured that saline conditioning, which is less salient that cocaine conditioning, occurred when animals were most alert (i.e. earlier in the light phase), to limit bias toward cocaine-environment learning.

Cocaine doses used were 5 mg/kg, 10 mg/kg, 20 mg/kg and 30 mg/kg, corresponding with Experiments 1 (5 mg/kg), 2 (10 mg/kg), 3 (20 mg/kg) and 4 (30 mg/kg). These doses were chosen because they are frequently used in the literature, and represent low (5 mg/kg), mid (10–20 mg/kg) and high (30 mg/kg) doses (Alaghband et al. 2017; Burgdorf et al. 2017; Fischer et al. 2017; Khroyan et al. 2015; Malvaez et al. 2013; Nguyen et al. 2012).

Locomotor data was also collected throughout these experiments to assess the development of locomotor sensitization for cocaine (i.e. an increase in locomotor activity with repeated cocaine administration). Locomotor data was recorded via horizontal infrared beams and custom software [Activity Monitor, Med Associates Inc.-software settings for the detection of locomotion were box size: 3; ambulatory trigger: 2; resting delay: 1000 ms; resolution: 100 ms (Karl et al. 2007)].

At test, mice were initially placed in the vehicle-paired compartment, and then given free access to the CPP apparatus (i.e. both sides of the compartment accessible as the gate was open). The test sessions were 30 min. A Preference Score was calculated for habituation and test (time in drug zone–time in vehicle zone); a positive preference score indicated a preference for the drug-paired compartment (Chesworth and Karl 2020).

Intravenous self-administration (IVSA)

Self–administration of intravenous cocaine was assessed using operant chambers (model ENV-022 V, Med Associates, VT, USA) equipped with two levers, one paired with reinforcement (the active lever), the other resulted in no outcome when pressed (the inactive lever). A magazine was located between the two levers, and food pellets were delivered via the magazine during food training. A stimulus light located above the active lever was turned on for 10 s in conjunction with reinforcement (conditioned stimulus, CS). An almond-scented piece of paper (discriminative cue) was placed below the active lever prior to each session. The chambers were housed in sound attenuated boxes and ventilated with fans. During food and cocaine self-administration, mice were tested for 6 consecutive days, and then given 1 day break, before testing resumed. IVSA methods were adapted from previously published work (Chesworth et al. 2013, 2016).

Sucrose pellet training

All mice in IVSA experiments underwent identical sucrose pellet training prior to surgery. Mice were food restricted during food training and were given 3–4 g chow per mouse per day, keeping mice at 90–95% free feeding weight. Mice were taught to discriminate the active from inactive lever with 10 days of sucrose pellet training, using dustless precision pellets (20 mg, sucrose formulation, Bio-Serv, NJ, USA), to ensure differences in cocaine self-administration were not due to an inability to learn an operant task. Sucrose pellet training sessions were 1 h. Mice were first trained for 1 day to collect sucrose pellets from the magazine (magazine training). Mice were then trained for 2 days to press the active lever for sucrose pellets delivered to the magazine under a fixed ratio 1 (FR1) schedule (single lever training). The inactive lever was not present during single lever training. Mice were then moved to double lever training for 5 days, during which both levers were extended, and mice needed to discriminate between the active and inactive levers. Mice were trained for 1 day on FR1, and then moved to FR2 for 4 days. Mice were returned to ab libitum food for at least 3 days prior to surgery.

Surgery

After instrumental training, mice were anaesthetised using isoflurane mixed with air (5% induction, 2–3% maintenance) plus meloxicam (3 mg/kg i.p.) and then implanted with indwelling venous cannulae (Mouse Vascular Access Button VAB62BS/25 and mouse jugular catheter C20PU-MJV1451, Instech, PA, USA) as previously described (Chesworth et al. 2013, 2016). Mice were allowed to recover in a heated recovery chamber for 1–2 h post-surgery (Mini-thermacage, Datesand, UK). Mice were treated with neomycin antibiotic diluted in 0.9% saline following surgery and during the 3 days recovery post-surgery, prior to the commencement of behavioural experiments.

Cocaine self-administration

Mice were connected via a mouse jugular access tether (25G swivel, KVAH62T, Instech, PA, USA). The swivel was connected with 24 inch PU tubing to a 10-ml syringe filled with cocaine solution in an infusion pump. The infusion volume was 20 µl and duration of infusion 2 s. Sessions were terminated if a predetermined maximum number of drug infusions was attained (for 0.1 mg/kg/infusion: 80 infusions; for 0.5 and 1 mg/kg/infusion: 50 infusions), and no drug was administered in the 5 s immediately after each drug infusion. During this period the stimulus light remained active, and any active lever presses were recorded as ‘time out’ responses. All sessions were 1.5 h in length (maximum infusion contingency notwithstanding). Mice were flushed with 0.02 ml heparinised saline daily prior to and immediately after sessions.

For both cohorts, mice were food restricted overnight prior to the first day of cocaine self-administration, to encourage responding. Food restriction was eased over 4 days of acquisition, and mice were kept on ad libitum food for the rest of the experiment.

Cocaine dose response - Experiment 5

An overview of Experiment 5 is provided in Supplementary Table S2. Mice were tested on three cocaine doses, to determine if the Nrg1 TM HET genotype affected cocaine tolerance. Each dose was available for 6 consecutive days; there was 5 days of FR2 and this was followed the next day by 1 day of progressive ratio (PR) responding at each dose (total testing days = 18). Mice were given 1 day off in between doses. PR testing (1.5 h) was used to assess motivation for each dose, where the number of lever presses required for an infusion was progressively increased within each session [PR schedule: 1, 3, 9, 13, 16, 18, 20, 22, 24, 25, 27, 28, 29, 31, 32, 34, 35, 37, 39, 41, 44, 47, 52, 64, 76, 88, 100, 112, 124, 136 (Chesworth et al. 2013, 2016)]. Breakpoint was defined as the final ratio completed within the session (Brown et al. 2009).

Initially, mice were trained using 0.5 mg/kg/infusion cocaine, to ensure acquisition of cocaine responding occurred with an established dose from the literature (Bird et al. 2014). Mice were then tested for responding with a lower dose of cocaine for 6 days (0.1 mg/kg/infusion), and then moved a higher dose of cocaine for 6 days (1 mg/kg/infusion). We used this test order to ensure the highest dose, which could be potentially aversive, did not cause mice to limit their subsequent responding for the next dose.

Cocaine extinction and reinstatement - Experiment 6

An overview of Experiment 6 is provided in Supplementary Table S3. Following 4 days of acquisition, mice were trained for 10 days on cocaine 0.5 mg/kg/infusion using an FR2 schedule; termed ‘stable FR2’. The average active lever presses on the last 5 days of responding was calculated (‘averaged FR2’), and used to create extinction criteria for each mouse [i.e. 30% of averaged FR2 active lever presses, maintained over 2 consecutive days (Chesworth et al. 2013; Yan et al. 2006)]. Extinction training followed stable FR2, where responses on the active lever were no longer reinforced with a drug infusion. The stimulus light and almond discriminative cue were not present during extinction sessions. There was a change of experimenter at extinction, such that the experimenter conducting extinction and reinstatement was not the same experimenter which conducted sucrose pellet and cocaine self-administration. Extinction sessions ran for 1.5 h and were conducted for up to 10 days. Mice needed to reach extinction criteria to be considered extinguished. If mice did not reach extinction criteria within 10 days, the number of days to reach extinction was assigned as 12, as mice would have needed at least another 2 days to extinguish and this would permit discrimination between mice which met criteria in 10 days vs mice which would have required at least another 2 days to extinguish. If mice met extinction criteria, they did not complete any more extinction sessions and were instead put into cue-induced reinstatement the next day.

For mice which did extinguish, cue-induced reinstatement testing (1.5 h) was conducted the day after extinction criteria was met. For cue-induced reinstatement, the stimulus light and almond discriminative stimulus were reintroduced to the operant chambers, but active lever responses remained unreinforced (Venniro et al. 2016). Active lever responses were not reinforced in the reinstatement session to differentiate responding for drug-associated cues with reacquisition of cocaine self-administration (the latter would occur if cocaine was present at reinstatement) (Khoo et al. 2015). Reinstatement data is presented as cumulative lever presses across the session (Brown et al. 2009).

Sucrose self-administration - Experiment 7

In Experiment 7, a final cohort of mice was assessed for operant sucrose self-administration, extinction and cue-induced reinstatement, to determine if the extinction deficits observed for cocaine in Nrg1 TM HET mice could be a more generalised deficit and thus also be evident when using a natural reinforcer. An overview of Experiment 7 is provided in Supplementary Table S4.

For sucrose self-administration experiments, mice were food restricted 1 day prior to the start of operant training, and then throughout sucrose self-administration experiments, being kept at 90–95% free feeding weight. All sucrose self-administration, extinction and reinstatement sessions were 45 min, as time course analysis of sucrose pellet experiments indicated that lever pressing was greatest in the first 45 min of the test (data not shown). There were no timeouts during sucrose self-administration sessions. Mice were trained to self-administer 10% sucrose which was delivered to the magazine (sucrose administrations are termed ‘deliveries’), with scent and light cues present. Mice were given 1 day of magazine training, then 3 days of single active lever training under FR1. Mice were then moved to double lever training, with 1 day of FR1, to facilitate acquisition of lever discrimination, and 10 days of FR2, to mimic the 10 days of stable cocaine self-administration in Experiment 6. Mice then underwent extinction training for 10 days, and cue-induced reinstatement after meeting extinction criteria, as described above. Unlike cocaine extinction sessions, there was no change of experimenter for sucrose extinction sessions.

Statistical analysis

Behavioural data were analysed using SPSS Statistics 24 (IBM, NY, USA). One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to analyse locomotor data from habituation day. For all experiments where we assessed different cocaine doses, we initially analysed all doses together to check for dose-dependent behaviour, and then conducted further ANOVA at each dose to investigate each dose individually. Four, three- and two-way repeated measures (RM) ANOVA with within factors ‘dose’ (IVSA: 0.1, 0.5, 1 mg/kg/infusion cocaine; CPP: 5, 10, 20, 30 mg/kg), ‘days’ (conditioning days), ‘lever’ (active vs. inactive lever) and between factor ‘genotype’ (WT vs. Nrg1 TM HET) and ‘dose’ (CPP/sensitization 5, 10, 20, 30 mg/kg) were conducted. Where interactions were found, Bonferroni post hoc tests were used to identify group differences at specific time points. To assess the number of days to meet extinction criteria in IVSA, we employed a log-rank Mantel-Cox test, and a Fisher’s exact test was used to determine the proportion of each genotype which extinguished (Chesworth et al. 2013). Data is presented as mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM), and differences were regarded as statistically significant if p < 0.05. In figures, post hoc effects of ‘day’ within drug condition are indicated by ‘&’ for WT mice (&&p < 0.01, &&&p < 0.001) and ‘$’ for Nrg1 TM HET mice ($p < 0.05, $$p < 0.01). Effects of ‘genotype’ are indicated by ‘*’ (*p < 0.05). RM effects, in the absence of post hocs, are shown by ‘#’ (#p < 0.05, ##p < 0.01, ###p < 0.001).

Exclusions

One Nrg1 TM HET mouse was excluded from the 30 mg/kg CPP test analysis (Experiment 4) because it jumped from the home cage onto the floor immediately prior to testing. One WT mouse was excluded from Experiment 5 (cocaine IVSA) because of health issues identified after the first PR test. One WT mouse from Experiment 5 and one Nrg1 TM HET mouse from Experiment 6 were excluded because their catheters became blocked during the experiment, and they were considered not patent.

Results

Experiments 1–4: habituation

There was no preference for either compartment at habituation in 3 out of 4 cocaine doses tested (Supplementary Table S5). There was a preference for the right zone in the 10 mg/kg cocaine cohort [‘zone’ F(1,16) = 5.7, p = 0.03]. However, time course data indicated only a preference for the right zone at particular time points [‘zone’ × ‘time’ interaction, F(5,80) = 4.0, p = 0.003; Supplementary Figure S1A, B]. Bonferroni post hoc tests demonstrated a significant preference in the first 5-min block of the test in WT mice, and in 2nd last 5 min block in Nrg1 TM HET mice only; no other preferences between the zones were detected. This data indicate the apparatus was very largely unbiased.

Experiments 1–4: locomotor sensitisation

Dose analysis

All cocaine doses increased locomotor activity [four-way RM ANOVA: ‘drug’ F(1,91) = 570.1, p < 0.001], and higher cocaine doses increased locomotion more than lower doses [‘drug’ × ‘dose’ F(3,91) = 38.6, p < 0.001] (Fig. 1A–D). Sensitization occurred [‘drug’ × ‘days’ F(3,273) = 24.4, p < 0.001] and was more pronounced at higher cocaine doses [‘drug’ × ‘days’ × ‘dose’ F(9,273) = 14.9, p < 0.001]. Genotype had no effect on sensitization (no main effect of ‘genotype’ and no interactions with ‘genotype’: all p’s > 0.2). To explore the interaction, we split data by ‘dose’.

Fig. 1
figure 1

AD Locomotor sensitization to cocaine. Distance travelled [cm] in WT and Nrg1 TM HET mice following acute saline or cocaine i.p. over 4 consecutive days. Data is presented for A 5 mg/kg cocaine, B 10 mg/kg cocaine, C 20 mg/kg cocaine and D 30 mg/kg cocaine. Interactions between ‘days’ × ‘drug’ were present for all doses (all p’s < .001). Data analysed by three-way RM ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post hoc tests, and presented as means ± SEM. Within each drug and genotype, significant effects of ‘day’ (vs. the same drug on day 1) are represented by ‘&’ for WT mice (&&p < .01, &&&p < .001) and ‘$’ for Nrg1 TM HET mice ($p < .05, $$p < .01)

All cocaine doses increased locomotion compared to saline (all ‘drug’ p’s < 0.001) and both genotypes sensitized to all doses of cocaine (all ‘days’ × ‘drug’ p’s < 0.001, no ‘days’ × ‘drug’ × ‘genotype’ interaction for any dose) (Fig. 1A–D). Genotype did not affect locomotor sensitization at any dose (all ‘genotype’ main effects p’s > 0.05). Locomotor sensitization was confirmed with post hoc tests, which showed changes to cocaine-induced locomotion in each genotype compared to cocaine day 1 (Fig. 1A–D).

Experiments 1–4: conditioned place preference

Dose analysis

Analysing all doses together, we found an increase in preference score from habituation to test [three-way RM ANOVA: ‘days’ F(1,91) = 143.2, p < 0.001]. The change in the preference score was more pronounced with higher cocaine doses [‘days’ × ‘dose’ F(3,91) = 3.2, p = 0.03]; this was not different between the genotypes (no ‘days’ × ‘dose’ × ‘genotype’ interaction, p > 0.05). To confirm this, we split data by ‘dose’.

At all doses tested, mice exhibited an increased preference for the cocaine-paired compartment compared to habituation (‘days’ p < 0.001 for all doses) regardless of genotype (p > 0.1 for all ‘genotype’ main effects and ‘days’ × ‘genotype’ interactions) (Fig. 2A–D).

Fig. 2
figure 2

AD CPP Preference score at habituation and test. Preference score [s] in WT and Nrg1 TM HET mice at habituation and test. Preference score is defined as (time spent in the cocaine-paired compartment–time spent in the saline-paired compartment). Preference score is presented for each cocaine dose: A 5 mg/kg cocaine, B 10 mg/kg cocaine, C 20 mg/kg cocaine, and D 30 mg/kg cocaine. Data analysed by three-way RM ANOVA, and presented as means ± SEM. A ‘days’ × ‘dose’ interaction (p = .03) indicates a greater change in preference score with higher cocaine doses. RM effects of ‘day’, showing an increase in preference score at each cocaine dose, are indicated by hash symbols (.###p < .001)

Experiment 5: sucrose pellet self-administration (prior to cocaine self-administration)

Successful acquisition of sucrose pellet self-administration was similar between the genotypes and is presented in Supplementary Figure S2A and 2B.

Experiment 5: cocaine IVSA—dose response

During the 3 days of initial acquisition, there were no genotype differences in lever responding [three-way RM ANOVA: F(1,13) = 0.9, p = 0.4] or the number of infusions [F(1,13) = 0.2, p = 0.7] and no interactions with ‘genotype’ (all p’s > 0.05; data not shown). Data during FR2 is presented as the average number of lever presses over the 5 days of FR2 training, as no significant main effects of ‘days’ were detected at each dose.

Dose analysis

Lever responding was different between the three doses [three-way RM ANOVA: ‘dose’ F(2,26) = 8.8, p = 0.001]; examination of Fig. 3A indicates lever responding was highest for the 0.1 mg/kg/infusion and lowest for 1 mg/kg/infusion. There was discrimination for the active lever at all doses [‘lever type’ F(1,13) = 20.6, p = 0.001] but this was strongest at the 0.1 and 0.5 mg/kg/infusion doses [‘lever type ‘ × ‘dose’ F(2,26) = 3.5, p = 0.045]. Genotype did not affect lever responding [‘genotype’ F(1,13) = 0.1, p = 0.7, no ‘genotype’ × ‘dose’ interactions]. We ran individual ANOVA to further analyse genotype and lever effects at each dose.

Fig. 3
figure 3

AC Intravenous self-administration and motivation for cocaine. A Average active and inactive lever pressing [n] for cocaine across the 5 days of testing at each dose (0.1, 0.5, 1 mg/kg/infusion), in WT and Nrg1 TM HET mice. On average across genotypes, discrimination for the active lever was present for all doses (all p’s < .05). B Average infusions [n] at each cocaine dose (0.1, 0.5, 1 mg/kg/infusion) across the 5 days of testing, in WT and Nrg1 TM HET mice. C Active and inactive lever pressing [n] during progressive ratio testing at each cocaine dose (0.1, 0.5, 1 mg/kg/infusion) in WT and Nrg1 TM HET mice. D Breakpoint (i.e. last ratio met) for each cocaine dose (0.1, 0.5, 1 mg/kg/infusion) in WT and Nrg1 TM HET mice. Data analysed by three- and two-way RM ANOVA, and presented as means ± SEM. A lever type ‘ × ‘dose’ interaction (p = .045) in A suggests discrimination is higher at lower cocaine doses (0.1 and 0.5 mg/kg/infusion). A lever type ‘ × ‘dose’ interaction (p = .01) in B suggests discrimination is higher at higher cocaine doses (0.5 and 1 mg/kg/infusion). RM effects of ‘lever type’ are indicated by hash symbols (#p < .05, ##p < .01). Abbreviations: Inf: infusion

At each cocaine dose, there was discrimination for the active lever when data were collapsed across both genotypes (all ‘lever type’ p’s < 0.02), and lever responding on both active and inactive levers was not different between the genotypes (all ‘genotype’ p’s > 0.6, all ‘genotype’ × ‘lever type’ interaction p’s > 0.3). There were no genotype differences in inactive lever responding for any dose (0.1 mg/kg/infusion: F(1,13) = 0.09, p = 0.77; 0.5 mg/kg/infusion: F(1,13) = 0.19, p = 0.68; 1 mg/kg/infusion: F(1,13) = 0.80, p = 0.39; additional ANOVA for inactive lever press only).

Both genotypes administered a greater number of infusions at lower cocaine doses than higher doses [‘dose’ F(2,26) = 6.6, p = 0.005], and the number of infusions made in each genotype was similar across doses [‘genotype’ F(1,13) = 0.1, p = 0.7; no ‘genotype’ × ‘dose’ interaction p = 0.8] (Fig. 3B). For 0.1 mg/kg/infusion cocaine, the average amount of cocaine administered per day was for WT: 0.0753 mg (i.e. 2.2 mg/kg) and for Nrg1 TM HET: 0.071 mg (i.e. 2.1 mg/kg). For 0.5 mg/kg/infusion, the average amount of cocaine administered per day was WT: 0.293 mg (i.e. 8.4 mg/kg); Nrg1 TM HET: 0.282 mg (i.e. 8.1 mg/kg). For 1 mg/kg/infusion, the average amount of cocaine administered per day was WT: 0.276 mg (i.e. 7.8 mg/kg); Nrg1 TM HET: 0.345 mg (i.e. 9.9 mg/kg).

Experiment 5: cocaine IVSA—progressive ratio

Dose analysis

Lever responding under a PR schedule changed across doses [three-way RM ANOVA: ‘dose’ F(2,26) = 12.7, p < 0.001], but was not different between genotypes [‘genotype’ F(1,13) = 0.01, p = 0.9; no ‘genotype’ × ‘dose’ interactions]. There was discrimination for the active lever [‘lever type’ F(1,13) = 8.5, p = 0.01], which was stronger at some doses than others [‘lever type’ × ‘dose’ F(2,26) = 5.3, p = 0.01] (see Fig. 3C—e.g. 0.5 and 1 mg/kg/infusion; see statistics below). To examine genotype and lever type effects at each dose, we conducted two-way ANOVA for each dose.

There were no genotype differences in PR lever presses at any dose tested (all ‘genotype’ p’s > 0.6, Fig. 3C). Discrimination for the active lever was evident at 0.5 and 1 m/kg/infusion PR doses (‘lever type’ p’s < 0.02); but not at 0.1 mg/kg/infusion [F(1,13) = 3.9, p = 0.07]. Additional ANOVA for inactive lever presses demonstrated no genotype differences for any dose (0.1 mg/kg/infusion: F(1,13) = 0.38, p = 0.55; 0.5 mg/kg/infusion: F(1,13) = 0.49, p = 0.50; 1 mg/kg/infusion: F(1,13) = 0.09, p = 0.78). Breakpoint data found no genotype differences for any dose (all ‘genotype’ p’s > 0.05; Fig. 3D).

Experiment 6: cocaine IVSA—extinction and cue-induced reinstatement

Mice were trained to self-administer 0.5 mg/kg/infusion cocaine under an FR2 schedule for 10 days prior to extinction training. During this period (i.e. stable FR2), both genotypes showed discrimination for the active lever [‘lever type’ F(1,37) = 74.0, p < 0.001; no ‘genotype’ × ‘lever type’ interaction] and responding was stable across days [‘days’ F(9,333) = 1.9, p = 0.06]. Overall lever responding was higher in Nrg1 TM HET mice than WTs [‘genotype’ F(1,37) = 6.5, p = 0.02], and examination of Fig. 4A suggests this is due to higher inactive lever pressing in Nrg1 TM HET mice. Subsequent analyses of inactive lever pressing only demonstrates elevated inactive lever pressing throughout stable FR2 [‘genotype’ F(1,37) = 11.3, p = 0.002]; this effect was consistent across stable FR2 (no interaction with ‘days’ p > 0.2). There were no genotype differences in active lever presses (p = 0.14).

Fig. 4
figure 4

AF Self-administration, extinction and cue-induced reinstatement of instrumental cocaine. A Active and inactive lever press [n] for 0.5 mg/kg/infusion cocaine in WT and Nrg1 TM HET mice, over 10 days of self-administration. Both genotypes showed discrimination for the active lever (p < .05). B Infusions [n] of 0.5 mg/kg/infusion cocaine in WT and Nrg1 TM HET mice over 10 days of self-administration. C Active and inactive lever press [n] on the first and final day of cocaine extinction for WT and Nrg1 TM HET mice. D Survival curve for WT and Nrg1 TM HET mice during extinction of cocaine self-administration. E Number of WT and Nrg1 TM HET mice which reached extinction criteria and did not reach extinction criteria. F Cumulative lever pressing [n] during cue-induced reinstatement in WT and Nrg1 TM HET mice. Data presented as means ± SEM in A, B, C and F, and analysed with three- or two-way RM ANOVA. Data in D analysed with log-rank Mantel-Cox test. Data in E analysed with Fisher’s exact test. ‘Genotype’ main effects in D and E, and post hoc effects in C and F indicated by asterisks (*p < .05). RM effects of ‘lever type’ are indicated by hash symbols (###p < .001). Abbreviations: Ext: mice which met extinction criteria; Not Ext: mice which did not meet extinction criteria

Average active and inactive lever presses across the final 5 days of self-administration (average FR2) were higher in Nrg1 TM HET mice than WTs [‘genotype’ F(1,37) = 6.4, p = 0.02] (Table 1), with similar discrimination for the active lever in both genotypes [‘lever type’ F(1,37) = 46.7, p < 0.001] (Table 1). Nrg1 TM HET mice pressed more on the inactive lever than WT mice [ANOVA for inactive lever press, ‘genotype’ F(1,37) = 9.2, p = 0.004]; this genotype difference was not evident for active lever press (p = 0.11). Importantly, despite higher lever responding in Nrg1 TM HET mice, cocaine infusions were similar between the genotypes during FR2 training [‘genotype’ F(1,37) = 1.8, p = 0.2] (Fig. 4B). The average amount of cocaine administered per day was WT: 0.268 mg (i.e. 7.7 mg/kg); Nrg1 TM HET: 0.302 mg (i.e. 8.6 mg/kg).

Table 1 Animal numbers per experiment

Mice were then put into extinction. We confirmed that extinction occurred by comparing lever pressing on the first day of extinction to the day when mice met extinction criteria (for mice which extinguished, this would be the lever presses on the day they extinguished, and for the mice which didn’t extinguish, this was the lever presses on day 10). Mice reduced their lever pressing across days [‘days’ F(1,37) = 41.5, p < 0.001], and Nrg1 TM HET mice exhibited elevated lever pressing compared to WT mice [‘genotype’ F(1,37) = 7.9, p = 0.008]. Responding decreased across days on the active lever more than the inactive lever in both genotypes [‘lever type’ × ‘days’ F(1,37) = 5.9, p = 0.02] and post hoc tests indicate that inactive lever responding was higher in Nrg1 TM HET mice than WT mice at the beginning and end of extinction, while active lever responding was only higher in Nrg1 mutants on the final day of extinction (Fig. 4C).

Despite this, not all mice met extinction criteria, and we detected an extinction deficit in Nrg1 TM HET mice, as these mice took longer to reach extinction criteria than WT mice (log-rank Mantel-Cox test, χ2 = 4.56, df = 1, p = 0.03) (Fig. 4D). In addition, fewer Nrg1 TM HET mice met extinction criteria than WT mice-27% of Nrg1 mutant mice met extinction criteria, vs 64% WT mice (Fisher’s exact test, p < 0.05) (Fig. 4E). Comparing the timecourse of lever pressing in mice which did extinguish on the first and the final days of extinction, there was a decrease in lever pressing across days [‘days’ F(1,18) = 34.8, p < 0.001], and Nrg1 TM HET mice pressed more during the first half of the session across both extinction days [‘genotype’ × ‘minutes’ F(8,144) = 2.5, p = 0.01]. Bonferroni post hoc tests confirm elevated active and inactive lever responding in Nrg1 mutant mice on the first and final day of extinction (Supplementary Figure S3A, 3B).

During cue-induced reinstatement, while there were no overall genotype differences in lever responding [‘genotype’ F(1,18) = 4.4, p = 0.05], active lever responding was greater in Nrg1 mutants than WTs as the test progressed [‘lever type’ × ‘time’ × ‘genotype’ [F(8,144) = 2.1, p = 0.04; ‘time’ × ‘genotype’ F(8,144) = 4.0, p < 0.001] (Fig. 4F). Bonferroni post hoc tests indicate active lever presses were greater in Nrg1 TM HET mice compared to WTs at 40–60 min of the reinstatement session (Fig. 4F).

Individuals analyses of active and inactive lever presses separately responding confirmed our interpretation: active lever responding was greater in Nrg1 mutants compared to WT mice [‘genotype’ F(1,18) = 4.6, p = 0.047], and this genotype effect was more prominent as the test progressed [‘genotype’ × ‘time’ F(8,144) = 3.7, p < 0.001]. While inactive lever pressing was not elevated in Nrg1 mutants uniformly throughout the reinstatement session [‘genotype’ F(1,18) = 2.6, p = 0.1], a ‘genotype’ × ‘time’ interaction suggests Nrg1 TM HET mice pressed more on the inactive lever as the test progressed [‘genotype’ × ‘time’ F(8,144) = 2.3, p = 0.02].

To control for differences in lever responding during extinction in Nrg1 TM HET mice, we compared total lever pressing during reinstatement to the final day of extinction. There was a strong trend for an effect of genotype [F(1,18) = 4.3, p = 0.05], suggesting Nrg1 TM HET mice tended to show higher reinstatement lever responding when controlling for extinction lever pressing (Supplementary Figure S4A). Drug-associated cues increased responding in both genotypes at reinstatement, particularly on the active lever [‘days’ F(1,18) = 21.3, p < 0.001; ‘days’ × ‘lever type’ F(1,18) = 27.1, p < 0.001].

Experiment 7: sucrose self-administration

Both genotypes stably self-administered sucrose during the 10 days of FR2 (Fig. 5A) [no main effect of ‘days’ F(9,189) = 1.6, p = 0.1 or ‘genotype’ F(1,21) = 0.001, p = 0.9], and discriminated for the active lever over the inactive lever [‘lever type’ F(1,21) = 89.6, p < 0.001] (no genotype differences in single lever FR1, data not shown). While there was a ‘genotype’ × ‘lever type’ × ‘days’ interaction [F(9,189) = 2.9, p = 0.003], follow up post hoc tests did not show any genotype differences in active or inactive lever presses (Fig. 5A). Analysis of active and inactive lever presses using separate ANOVA found no effects of ‘genotype’ on active [F(1,21) = 0.1, p = 0.9] or inactive lever pressing [F(1,21) = 0.4, p = 0.6], confirmed this interpretation.

Fig. 5
figure 5

AF Self-administration, extinction and cue-induced reinstatement of instrumental sucrose. A Active and inactive lever press [n] for 10% sucrose in WT and Nrg1 TM HET mice, over 10 days of FR2 self-administration. Both genotypes showed discrimination for the active lever (p < .05). B Sucrose rewards [n] in WT and Nrg1 TM HET mice over 10 days of self-administration. C Active and inactive lever press [n] on the first and final day of sucrose extinction for WT and Nrg1 TM HET mice. D Survival curve for WT and Nrg1 TM HET mice during extinction of sucrose self-administration. E Number of WT and Nrg1 TM HET mice which reached extinction criteria and did not reach extinction criteria. F Cumulative lever pressing [n] during cue-induced reinstatement for sucrose in WT and Nrg1 TM HET mice. Data presented as means + SEM in A, B, C and D, and analysed with three- or two-way RM ANOVA. Data in D analysed with log-rank Mantel-Cox test. Data in E analysed with Fisher’s exact test. RM effects of ‘lever type’ are indicated by hash symbols (###p < .001). Bonferroni post hoc effects of ‘genotype’ on both active and inactive levers in F indicated by asterisks (*p < .05). Abbreviations: Ext: mice which met extinction criteria; Not Ext: mice which did not meet extinction criteria

Extinction criteria were created based on the final 5 days of sucrose self-administration (stable FR2). Sucrose self-administration was similar between the genotypes in the last 5 days of training [no main effect of ‘genotype’ F(1,21) = 0.6, p = 0.4, no interaction] and discrimination for the active lever was evident in both groups [‘lever type’ [F(1,21) = 78.7, p < 0.001, no interaction with ‘genotype’] (Table 2). Sucrose deliveries were also similar between the genotypes during FR2 training (no main effect of ‘genotype’ F(1,21) = 0.1, p = 0.8 or ‘days’ F(9,189) = 1.7, p = 0.1; no interaction] (Fig. 5B).

Table 2 Average FR2 responding for 0.5 mg/kg infusion cocaine (Experiment 6) and 10% sucrose (Experiment 7). Average active and inactive lever presses over the last 5 days of stable FR2 training in WT and Nrg1 TM HET mice

Comparing the first and last extinction days, we saw no overall reduction in lever pressing during sucrose extinction [‘days’ F(1,21) = 1.7, p = 0.2], although a ‘lever type’ × ‘days’ interaction suggests active lever responding decreased and inactive lever responding did not (Fig. 5C). Nrg1 TM HET mice did not make more lever responses that WTs during extinction [‘genotype’ F(1,21) = 1.4, p = 0.3], and Bonferroni post hoc tests found no genotype differences at any time point (Fig. 5C).

There were no genotype differences in the latency to reach extinction criteria (log-rank Mantel-Cox test, χ2 = 0.18, df = 1, p = 0.67) (Fig. 5D). Three mice per genotype reached extinction criteria and the proportion in each genotype was similar (21% of WT, 33% of Nrg1 TM HET; Fisher’s exact test, p = 0.64) (Fig. 5E). When we analysed timecourse data for lever pressing on the first and last days of extinction in mice which did extinguish, there were no genotype differences in lever responding [‘genotype’ F(1,4) = 1.5, p = 0.3; no interactions with ‘genotype’] (Supplementary Figure S3C, 3D), and all mice reduced their lever responding across days [‘days’ [F(1,4) = 45.4, p < 0.001], more so on the active lever [‘days’ × ‘lever type’ F(1,4) = 33.1, p < 0.001].

At cue-induced reinstatement, Nrg1 TM HET pressed more on both active and inactive levers as the test progressed [‘time’ × ‘genotype’ interaction F(8,32) = 12.8, p < 0.001; no main effect of ‘genotype’ F(1,4) = 4.2, p = 0.1] (Fig. 5F). Bonferroni post hoc tests indicated elevated lever responding on both levers was evident in Nrg1 mutant mice compared to WT controls in the last 30 min of the session. Furthering this, individual ANOVA for active and inactive lever pressing during reinstatement found no genotype differences on active lever presses [‘genotype’ F(1,4) = 0.1, p = 0.9], but a ‘time’ × ‘genotype’ interaction for inactive lever presses suggests elevated inactive lever pressing in Nrg1 TM HET mice as the reinstatement test progressed [F(8,32) = 7.8, p < 0.001; no ‘genotype’ main effect p = 0.1].

When comparing total lever pressing during reinstatement to the final extinction day, Nrg1 TM HET mice pressed more on both levers across both testing days [‘genotype’ F(1,4) = 9.7, p = 0.04] (Supplementary Figure S4B). Both genotypes increased their responding at cue-induced reinstatement [‘days’ F(1,4) = 42.9, p = 0.003].

Discussion

We found impaired extinction of operant cocaine but not sucrose in Nrg1 TM HET mice. Responding on both active and inactive levers was greater in Nrg1 TM HET mice during cocaine cue-induced reinstatement, compared to WT controls. Nrg1 mutant mice also exhibited higher levels of inactive lever pressing during operant cocaine self-administration, extinction and cue-induced reinstatement, as well as during sucrose cue-induced reinstatement. Despite this, Nrg1 TM HET mice did not show altered cocaine or sucrose self-administration, nor were there genotype differences in cocaine reward, locomotor sensitization to cocaine, or cocaine motivation. This suggests Nrg1 TM HET mice may have deficits in response inhibition in the absence of cocaine, despite similar cocaine and sucrose reward and reinforcement.

We detected operant extinction deficits in Nrg1 TM HET mice for cocaine across several measures: Nrg1 mutant mice made more lever responses during cocaine extinction, took longer to meet cocaine extinction criteria, and a greater proportion of Nrg1 mutant mice did not meet cocaine extinction criteria. We also observed higher cumulative active and inactive lever responding during the middle to later part of cocaine cue-induced reinstatement. While the reinstatement data suggests elevated responding to cocaine-associated cues in Nrg1 mutant mice, this finding may also be affected by extinction deficits in Nrg1 mutant mice. Indeed, cue-induced responding under extinction conditions occurs early in a session, after which animals learn that drug cues do not result in drug delivery. As a consequence, mice reduce their responding and thereby demonstrate within session extinction. Thus, elevated responding which only occurs later in the cocaine cue reinstatement session may represent impaired within-session extinction in Nrg1 mutant mice. Interestingly, extinction deficits were not observed for operant sucrose, and active lever responding was not elevated during sucrose cue reinstatement in Nrg1 mutant mice, suggesting extinction impairments may be cocaine-specific. Linking this to clinical data, deficits in response inhibition are evident in individuals with schizophrenia and are linked to cognitive impairment (Gotra et al. 2020); however, it is unknown if response inhibition is linked to genetic risk for schizophrenia. Our data suggest that impaired response inhibition can extend to drug reinforcers such as cocaine in the presence of Nrg1 mutation.

There has been limited investigation of how mutations in Nrg1 TM may cause dysregulation of circuits involved in inhibitory control during extinction. Nrg1 TM HET mice show elevated cortical gamma frequency power and reductions in sensory-driven gamma responses, potentially representing increased ‘noise’ in neural networks (Long et al. 2015), which may impair cognitive processes which drive extinction learning. Gamma oscillations are linked with working memory abilities necessary for within-session extinction (Yamamoto et al. 2014), and elevated hippocampal network oscillations can underpin resistance to extinction (Caliskan and Stork 2019). It is possible that the Nrg1 TM HET mutation regulates hippocampal and/or cortical extinction processes which are relevant for cocaine extinction learning.

We did not observe extinction impairments for operant sucrose, and in fact, very few mice in the operant sucrose experiment reached extinction criteria. While it is possible Nrg1 TM HET mutation specifically affects operant cocaine extinction, one methodological difference should be considered before drawing this conclusion. Unlike cocaine extinction, there was no change of experimenter at extinction for sucrose, and an experimenter may serve as a contextual cue which signals drug availability. Indeed, rodents are sensitive to changes in experimenter and this can affect subsequent behaviour (Davis et al. 1997; van Driel and Talling 2005)—see also (Sorge et al. 2014)). Thus, the protocols were not identical due to factors outside of our control, and it is unclear if we may have observed a greater proportion of animals extinguishing, and/or observed extinction deficits in Nrg1 mutants for operant sucrose with a change of experimenter at extinction. Considering Nrg1 and its main receptor ErbB4 are linked to fear extinction deficits (Cao et al. 2021; Skirzewski et al. 2020), we cannot rule out that Nrg1 TM HET mutation may affect extinction processes for both cocaine and natural reinforcers.

We observed higher levels of inactive lever responding in Nrg1 TM HET mice compared to WT controls in Experiments 6 and 7. Higher inactive lever responding may be linked to the locomotor hyperactivity phenotype of Nrg1 TM HET mice (Karl et al. 2007; Visini et al. 2022); however, this explanation is unlikely as hyperactivity would theoretically increase responding on both levers, not just the inactive lever. Another possibility is that Nrg1 mutant mice incorrectly overgeneralise active lever contingencies to the inactive lever. Generalisation of active lever responding to a novel lever can occur when training for both levers occurs in the same context (Bernal-Gamboa et al. 2018), and when stimuli are perceptually similar (Bergstrom 2020). While generalisation of learning can be beneficial (e.g. prior stimulus associations can predict reinforcement from a novel stimulus), overgeneralisation can be maladaptive (Dunsmoor and Paz 2015), and may be considered a form of cognitive impairment, representing an inability to engage in sufficient pattern separation to distinguish two distinct stimuli (Abumaria et al. 2013). Nrg1 TM HET mice exhibit cognitive deficits in novel object recognition and recall of contextual fear conditioning (Duffy et al. 2010), while in clinical populations, NRG1 SNPs and serum NRG1β1 concentration are linked with poorer cognitive ability in individuals with schizophrenia (Cho et al. 2015; Yang et al. 2021). Thus, it is possible that elevated inactive lever pressing may reflect cognitive impairment in Nrg1 mutants.

We found no genotype differences in cocaine reward in CPP or cocaine reinforcement in IVSA. We hypothesised that cocaine reward and reinforcement may be enhanced by Nrg1 TM HET mutation, as Nrg1 TM HET mice exhibit reduced striatal dopamine D2 receptor density (Newell et al. 2013), and enhanced striatal D2 receptor expression can reduce cocaine self-administration in rats [(Thanos et al. 2008) see also (Nader et al. 2006)]. However, across a dose range in both CPP and IVSA, we found that mutant Nrg1 does not affect acquisition and expression of cocaine reward or cocaine operant learning. It is possible that the degree of striatal D2 receptor reduction in Nrg1 mutants was insufficient to alter cocaine reward or reinforcement. Indeed, other studies have shown a > 40% increase in striatal D2 receptors reduces cocaine self-administration in rats (Thanos et al. 2008), and striatal D2 receptors in Nrg1 TM HET mice are only reduced by ~ 22% (Newell et al. 2013). Alternatively, it is possible that stress may enhance cocaine reward or reinforcement in Nrg1 TM HET mice. Nrg1 TM HET mice are more susceptible than WT controls to effects of stress on schizophrenia-relevant behaviours (Chesworth et al. 2012b), and individuals carrying an NRG1 variant who experience stressful life events are more likely to use alcohol, tobacco and illicit drugs than to individuals who do not carry the NRG1 variant (Vaht et al. 2017). Thus, a gene × environment interaction may be necessary to demonstrate effects of Nrg1 TM HET mutation on cocaine reward or reinforcement.

Cocaine-induced locomotion and the development of cocaine locomotor sensitization was intact in Nrg1 TM HET mice. Interestingly, we observed a reduction in locomotor activity with repeated 30 mg/kg cocaine in both genotypes, which may indicate an earlier peak in locomotor activity and an increase in stereotyped behaviour with repeated cocaine (Crittenden et al. 2021). It was somewhat surprising that no genotype differences in cocaine locomotor sensitization were detected, as stereotyped behaviours following repeated amphetamine are linked to genes regulated by Nrg1 (Crittenden et al. 2021), and adolescent Nrg1 mutant mice show a dose-dependent increase in methamphetamine-induced locomotion (Spencer et al. 2012), although no genotype differences are evident for amphetamine-induced locomotion in adulthood (van den Buuse et al. 2009). It is possible that differences in cocaine sensitization may be dependent on age; indeed, our laboratory has found that Nrg1 TM HET mice are differently affected by cannabinoids such as THC and cannabidiol during adolescence compared to adulthood (Boucher et al. 2007a; Long et al. 2013, 2012; Visini et al. 2022). Future research can investigate if Nrg1 TM HET mice show altered susceptibility to cocaine locomotor sensitization in adolescence.

We observed some slight differences between Experiments 5 and 6, whereby Nrg1 TM HET mice exhibited elevated lever responding for 0.5 mg/kg/infusion cocaine (collapsed across both levers) in Experiment 6, which was not present in Experiment 5. It is possible this is due to the duration of training under the same dose. In Experiment 5, mice were kept on 0.5 mg/kg/infusion for 5 days (4 × FR2 and 1xPR), whereas in Experiment 6, they were kept at this dose for 10 days. Importantly, examination of Fig. 4A suggests that genotype differences become more prominent after the first 5 days. It is possible that if we had kept mice in Experiment 5 at 0.5 mg/kg/infusion for longer that they may have shown the genotype differences observed in Experiment 6.

There were some limitations to the present study. First, the duration of cocaine or sucrose self-administration in Experiments 5–7, or of experimenter administered cocaine in Experiments 1–4 was fairly short. While self-administration protocols of 2–3 weeks are standard in the field (Dickson et al. 2014; Lopez et al. 2021; Lujan et al. 2018; Roberts et al. 2018), they do not reflect human drug taking experiences which can occur over several years or decades (Kuhn et al. 2019; Spanagel 2017). Thus, we can only report on and interpret the data in terms of cocaine reward and reinforcement, rather than attempting to comprehensively model human cocaine use problems. Also, we conducted these experiments in male mice only. This was a conscious decision, as female Nrg1 TM HET mice exhibit a less prominent behavioural phenotype than males (Chesworth et al. 2012a; Duffy et al. 2010), and show a reduced behavioural response to THC than male Nrg1 mutants (Boucher et al. 2007a; Long et al. 2010). Thus, it is possible there are sex-specific effects of cocaine in Nrg1 mutants. Finally, the highest dose used in cocaine IVSA (1 mg/kg/infusion) was potentially too high [although it has been used previously: (Bird et al. 2014)] and it may have induced ceiling effects. Future work should examine lower cocaine IVSA doses.

In conclusion, this is the first report linking Nrg1 to behaviours which may increase susceptibility for problematic cocaine use. While there are some clinical reports linking cannabis and alcohol use problems with NRG1 mutations (Aukst Margetic et al. 2019; Han et al. 2012; Vaht et al. 2017), to date there have been no investigations into NRG1 mutations and association with cocaine use disorder. Our data extend current knowledge by not only linking Nrg1 with behaviours relevant to problematic cocaine use, but also identifying which cocaine-induced behaviours Nrg1 mutation impacts on. By demonstrating extinction deficits for cocaine in Nrg1 TM HET mice, we demonstrate a behavioural mechanism (i.e. limiting cocaine responding) which may increase susceptibility to cocaine use in individuals carrying this mutation.