Overview of the task and subjects’ performance
We trained three monkeys to perform the task depicted in Fig. 1. In each trial, monkeys performed series of actions (squeezing a grip six, eight, or ten times) to get a small, medium, or large fluid reward (Fig. 1a). Note that the amount of force required to complete the trial was minimal and monkeys always succeeded to complete a squeeze when they tried. In 70% of trials, we introduced a choice by presenting an alternative option before monkeys could complete the trial. This alternative option, presented on the opposite side of the monitor compared to the current option, was also characterized by a given number of squeezes and a given reward size. Thus, monkeys could either choose to continue with the current option by squeezing the same grip as before, or switch to the other grip to start completing the alternative option and obtain the corresponding reward.
Monkeys performed on average (across treatment conditions) 61, 71, and 55 trials per session (monkeys A, D, and E respectively). Note that since the amount of force required to complete the squeeze was minimal, monkeys always succeeded to complete it when they tried. Thus, a failure to squeeze the bar was always interpreted as a rejection of the current offer, either at the onset or in the middle of a sequence. Overall, when monkeys were performing the task, they engaged in respectively 90.4, 95.8, and 78.9% of the offered forced choice squeezes (all “no choice” squeezes included) (monkeys A, D, and E respectively, mean across treatment conditions, no significant effect of treatment condition: linear effect taking into account the variability across subjects, t(10) = − 0.52, p = 0.61). Note that these relatively low percentages are due to the fact that the first squeezes of the sequence were often refused. On the choice squeezes, they were also very unlikely to disengage, and they accepted 99.4, 99.6, and 99.8% of choices (monkeys A, D, and E respectively, the same as above t(10) = − 0.02, p = 0.79).
Behavior under saline
We first examined monkeys’ behavior under saline. During the sequence, monkeys’ acceptance rate increased sharply after the first squeeze: monkeys sometimes rejected the offer at the beginning of a sequence but virtually never gave up in subsequent steps (Fig. 2a). Their acceptance rate was positively modulated by the reward size (linear effect taking into account the variability across subject on all first squeeze, β = 0.27 ± 0.12, t(24) = 2.15, p = 0.04) and negatively modulated by the sequence length (β = − 0.71 ± 0.13, t(24) = − 5.67, p < 0.001). We also performed a logistic regression on the first squeeze of each sequence for each individual monkey, and both effects were also significant at the subject level except for the reward size effect on monkey D which was only marginally significant (monkey A: β(sequence length) = − 0.82 ± 0.05, t(2740) = −15.83, p < 0.001, β(reward size) = 0.89 ± 0.05, t(2740) = 13.09, p < 0.001; monkey D: β(sequence length) = − 0.12 ± 0.05, t(1553) = − 2.41, p = 0.02, β(reward size) = − 0.09 ± 0.05, t(1553) = − 1.83, p = 0.07; monkey E: β(sequence length) = − 1.21 ± 0.10, t(1491) = − 11.78, p < 0.001, β(reward size) = 0.83 ± 0.07, t(1491) = 12.66, p < 0.001). We also examined the influence of task factors on monkeys’ reaction times to the green dot (go signal). By contrast with the acceptance rate, reaction times at the group level did not show a significant modulation by either sequence length (multi-level linear regression on all successful first squeezes, t(24) = 1.71, p = 0.10) or reward size (p = 0.69). At the subject level, only monkey A showed a significant effect of sequence length (β(sequence length) = 0.10 ± 0.03, t(1042) = 3.28, p = 0.001) and the effect of reward size approached significance (t(1042) = −1.82, p = 0.07) (all others: p > 0.24). Overall, monkeys’ reaction times were not modulated by the experimental factors, whereas their willingness to work on the first trial was.
As expected, the monkeys’ choices between the current and the alternative options were affected both by the expected costs (number of remaining squeezes) and benefits (reward size) (Fig. 2b). The effects were significant both at the group level (multi-level linear regression taking into account the variability across subject: β(sequence length difference) = − 0.003 ± 0.0002, t(69) = − 12.70, p =< 0.001, β(reward size) = 0.003 ± 0.0002, t(69) = 12.29, p < 0.001) and at the subject level (multi-level linear regression on each subjects’ choices: monkey A: β(sequence length difference) = − 0.003 ± 0.0003, t(638) = − 11.52, p < 0.001, β(reward size) = 0.003 ± 0.0003, t(638) = 11.67, P < 0.001; monkey D: β(sequence length difference) = − 0.004 ± 0.0004, t(558) = − 9.49, p < 0.001, β(reward size) = 0.004 ± 0.0004, t(558) = 9.86,p < 0.001; monkey E: β(sequence length difference) = − 0.004 ± 0.0005, t(282) = − 7.43, p < 0.001, β(reward size) = 0.003 ± 0.0005, t(282) = 7.05, p < 0.001). Thus, all monkeys had clearly understood the quantities at stake for the choice, and they were affected in a similar manner by the two factors. Note that monkeys A and D had a significant bias for the current option and the alternative option respectively (monkey A: β = − 0.0007 ± 0.0001, t(558) = 2.15, p < 0.001; monkey D: β = 0.0003 ± 0.0001, t(638) = − 5.81, p < 0.001) whereas monkey E was not biased (monkey E: t(282) = 0.80, p = 0.42) (Fig. 3b).
This analysis of monkeys’ behavior under saline showed that their performance in the task was similar. However, we could observe different profiles. Monkey A had a general bias toward staying with the current option. The other male (monkey D) had the opposite bias, and the female (monkey E) had no bias. These idiosyncratic features of the monkeys’ behavior were taken into account in all our subsequent analyses. Indeed, as for the other behavioral measures (force, reaction times, stability in choices) and parameter estimates, we fitted a different intercept for each monkey.
Effects of clonidine on behavioral flexibility: choices
We first measured the influence of clonidine on choices. We first looked at the average number of squeezes chosen and the reward size chosen (Fig. 3a), no matter whether it was a stay or switch choice, which provides a global estimate of the animals’ relative sensitivity to costs and benefits. This measure was also not reliably affected by the treatment (linear regression taking into account subject variability, t(10) = 1.85, p = 0.09 and t(10) = 0.65, P = 0.53, for squeezes and reward size respectively).
We next examined the influence of clonidine on the choice bias toward the current versus alternative option, over and above the influence of expected costs (number of squeezes) and benefits (reward sizes). Because all options were offered in equal proportions as current and alternative options, any departure of the probability to take the alternative option from 50% would represent a bias toward staying or switching. At the group level, there was no significant bias toward staying or switching across all treatment conditions (linear regression taking into account subject variability, t(10) = 0.52, p = 0.52), and the bias was not different from zero in any condition (T test, p > 0.47, for all doses) (Fig. 3b).
We then looked the stability in choices across doses. As shown in Fig. 3c, there was a clear linear increase in stability across doses of clonidine, which means that with increasing doses of clonidine, the monkeys became increasingly likely to make the same decisions when faced with the same type of choice (linear regression taking into account variability across subjects, β = 0.559 ± 0.183, t(10) = 3.06, p = 0.01).
To capture the specific influence of clonidine on distinct components of decision-making, we built a simple choice model depicted in Eqs. 1 and 2 in “Material and methods”. In this model, the value of each option corresponds to a trade-off between reward at stake and sequence length, controlled by a parameter k
cost-benefit. The probability to select a given option depends on (i) the value difference with the alternative and (ii) a fixed bias, e.g., a preference for either staying with the current option or taking the alternative, as well as (iii) the choice consistency, which determines the degree to which choices are consistent with the evaluation.
As shown in Fig. 4, we looked at the effect of the treatment on the three parameters of the choice model (k
cost-benefit, bias, and consistency) (see online resource ESM_1 for the subjects’ parameters’ estimate). The parameter k
cost-benefit describing the relative sensitivity to reward and sequence length was significantly different from zero, indicating that monkeys readily integrated these two factors to guide their behavior (all p < 0.01). Had either the sensitivity to sequence length or reward size changed following administration of clonidine, this parameter would have varied. For example, an increase in effort sensitivity would have been translated in an increase in the k
cost-benefit parameter because animals would have given up some reward to exert fewer squeezes. But as shown in Fig. 4a, this parameter estimate was again not affected by the treatment (linear regression taking into account variability across subjects, t(10) = − 0.10, p = 0.92), indicating a lack of effect of clonidine on the cost-benefit analysis. In line with the previously described model-agnostic analysis (Fig. 3b), monkeys had different bias parameter values, but there was no systematic bias to stay with the current option or switch to the alternative at the group level (bias parameter not significantly different from zero, all p > 0.55) and no effect of treatment on this bias parameter (linear regression taking into account variability across subjects, t(10) = − 0.10, p = 0.92) (Fig. 4b). By contrast, clonidine induced a dose dependent increase in choice consistency (Fig. 4c). To analyze this formally, we ran a linear regression taking into account the variability across subjects. This revealed a significant linear effect of dose on the consistency parameter’s estimates (β = 0.248 ± 0.070, t(10) = 3.54, p < 0.01). Figure 4d, e illustrates the influence of the highest dose of clonidine on choices. The slope of the choice curve is noticeably higher under clonidine, reflecting a reliable increase in choice consistency (see online resource ESM_2 for individual subject’s choice curves).
To ensure that our model accurately captured monkeys’ choices, we computed the model balanced accuracy for each subject and each treatment condition and it was overall between 0.80 and 0.86 (Fig. 4f). There was close but not significant positive effect of dose on balanced accuracy (linear regression taking into account variability across subjects, β = 0.008 ± 0.004, t(10) = 2.08, p = 0.06) and close but not significant correlation between the balanced accuracy and the consistency parameter (linear regression taking into account variability across subjects, β = 12.39 ± 6.13, t(10) = 2.02, p = 0.07). However, because these statistical tests were close to the significance threshold and to ensure that the balanced accuracy was not driving the effect on the consistency parameter, we compared a model in which the consistency parameter is linearly dependent on the balanced accuracy (linear regression taking into account variability across subjects, Bayesian information criterion: BIC = 23.9) and linearly dependent on the dose (same, BIC = 18.8). Since the later model won the comparison (∆BIC = 5.1 > 3, meaning that there is a strong evidence in favor of the dose-effect model), the dose effect was better explained by a change on the consistency parameter than by a change in goodness of fit, evaluated by the balanced accuracy.
Finally, we used a more complex model (full model) to try to capture other factors potentially affecting choices. Notably, we generally found a significant side bias in monkeys’ choices but it was not systematically significant and changed direction across treatment conditions (or weeks of testing) in the same animal. We also added a parameter capturing the effect of the number of squeezes done before the choice and found a significant positive effect of this parameter on the probability to take the alternative option. Hence, monkeys were more likely to take the alternative option if they had done more squeezes to get to the choice. We also added a parameter to capture an imbalance in the evaluation of the options, but this parameter was not different from 1 across all subjects and doses (T test, p = 0.08). Overall, including these parameters in the choice model did not affect the results presented above, and there was no effect of the treatment on any of them (linear regression taking into account the variability across subjects: all P > 0.41), except on the consistency parameter (β(consistency) = 0.265 ± 0.088, t(10) = 3.00, P = 0.01) consistency (see online resource ESM_3 for subjects’ full model parameters estimates).
Effect of clonidine on reaction times
We next evaluated the effects of clonidine on reaction times across task conditions. We separated squeezes where monkeys had to make a choice between two options from equivalent points in the sequence on single option squeeze, where they only squeezed the grip to progress through the trial. As classically observed, monkeys were slower to respond in choice than no-choice trials (Fig. 5a). We examined the influence of clonidine on reaction times in these two types of trials, and a multi-level linear regression taking into account variability across subjects revealed a significant linear effect of choice (β = 0.641 ± 0.022, t(21) = 6.30, p < 0.001) and dose (β = 0.070 ± 0.007, t(21) = 2.90, p < 0.01), but no significant interaction (t(20) = 0.13, p = 0.89). Hence, clonidine significantly increased reaction times, but its effects were undistinguishable between choice and non-choice conditions. We also separated choice reaction times according to the absolute difference in value of the two options (choice difficulty) and found a negative main effect of difference in value on reaction times (multi-level linear regression, β = − 0.31 ± 0.11, t(57) = − 2.73, p = 0.008) and a main effect of dose (β = 0.41 ± 0.11, t(57) = 3.63, p < 0.001) but once again no interaction (t(56) = − 0.45, p = 0.65) (Fig. 5b). Hence, both clonidine and choice difficulty increase reaction time but their effects are simply additive, indicating that clonidine does not interfere with the influence of difficulty on reaction times. Overall, monkeys’ reaction times were clearly modulated across conditions: animals took longer to respond when they had to make a choice, especially if it was difficult. High doses of clonidine also increased monkeys’ reaction times, but because their effects were equivalent across conditions (no interaction), it did not affect the influence of difficulty on reaction times.
Together, our analyses therefore revealed two effects of clonidine on behavior: it dose-dependently increased both choice consistency (as captured by the model-based analysis) and choice reaction times. We then examined the relation between these two effects across treatments and animals. We found a positive correlation (linear regression taking into account variability across subjects, β = 0.321 ± 0.095, t(10) = 3.47, p < 0.01) between the estimated consistency parameter and the choice reaction time (Fig. 5c). This correlation between the effect of treatments on reaction time and choice consistency suggests that clonidine affects a single functional entity, which we will refer as the “speed-consistency trade-off.”
Effect of clonidine on motivation: willingness to work
After assessing the implication of noradrenaline in behavioral flexibility, we examined the influence of clonidine on two additional behavioral measures that are classically used to assess motivation: willingness to work and physical force production (Fig. 6). We first measured monkeys’ willingness to work by counting the proportion of accepted squeezes. Since the action is very easy, the number of squeezes that they perform in a session reflects their general motivation to engage with the task. At the subject level, we found a marginally significant effect of the dose on the willingness to work during 1-h-long sessions for monkey E only (monkey E: β = − 0.08 ± 0.02, t(2) = − 4.14, p = 0.054; monkey A: β = 0.02 ± 0.01, t(2) = 2.53, p = 0.15; monkey D: β = 0.04 ± 0.02, t(2) = 1.77 p = 0.22). However, it was not significantly affected by dose at the group level (linear regression taking into account the variability across subjects, t(10) = − 0.36, p = 0.73) (Fig. 6a).
We next examined the effect of clonidine on animals’ trial-by-trial willingness to work trial as a function of the upcoming effort cost and future reward size. To do this, we first examined willingness to work on the first squeeze of all sequences. We included every trial, since there was no way for monkeys to predict at the start of a sequence if a choice was going to be offered later in that sequence. We again examined the influence of reward size and sequence length on the willingness to perform the first squeeze using a linear regression taking into account the variability across subjects. This analysis revealed a significant negative effect of sequence length (i.e., the animals were less willing to engage on long sequences: β = − 18.81 ± 2.44, t(104) = − 7.71, p < 0.001) and a marginally significant positive effect of reward size (animals were more willing to engage for greater reward: β = − 4.79 ± 2.44, t(104) = 1.96, P = 0.052), but no effect of dose of clonidine (t(104) = 1.43, P = 0.16) and no interaction with sequence length (t(102) = 0.20, p = 0.84) or reward (t(102) = − 0.65, p = 0.52). We also looked at the effect of clonidine on reaction times during the first squeeze of the sequence and found a positive effect of sequence length (multi-level linear regression, β = 0.08 ± 0.04, t(104) = 2.05, p = 0.04) but no effect of reward size (t(104) = 0.08, p = 0.93). We found a positive main effect of dose (β = 0.09 ± 0.03, t(104) = 2.93, p = 0.004) but no interaction with sequence length and reward size (both p > 0.40). Hence, the treatment did not interfere with subjects’ evaluation of whether or not to engage in the sequence.
We then examined the influence of clonidine on adjustments to willingness to work across the steps of a trial (Fig. 6b). In all treatment conditions, monkeys displayed a sharp increase in their willingness to work after the first squeeze. We fitted the curves depicted in Fig. 6b with Eq. 4 for all trials during which no choice was offered. In this model, k
intercept controls the intercept (initial willingness to work) and k
slope the slope of the rise of the willingness to work across the sequence. We then examined the influence of reward and sequence length on each of these parameter estimates using a multi-level linear regression taking into account the variability across monkeys. The parameter k
intercept displayed a significant positive linear effect of sequence length (β = 0.160 ± 0.019, t(104) = 8.09, p < 0.001) and a negative effect of reward size (β = − 0.048 ± 0.019, t(104) = − 2.44, p < 0.05). Importantly, however, there was again no significant linear effect of dose (t(104) = − 1.85, p = 0.07) and no interaction with either the sequence length (t(102) = 0.32, p = 0.75) or the reward size (t(102) = − 0.12, p = 0.91). Moreover, neither the task factors (reward and sequence length) nor the dose of clonidine and its interaction with the tasks factors affected the parameter k
slope, which captured the slope of the change in willingness to work across the sequence (all p > 0.30).
In short, while monkeys’ willingness to start and persist with an action sequence was sensitive to the sequence length and reward size on each trial, none of these parameters were affected by clonidine.
Effect of clonidine on motivation: force production
Lastly, we examined the effect of clonidine on another key component of motivation: force production. As shown in Fig. 6c, force peak was significantly decreased under clonidine treatment (linear regression, t(10) = − 3.24, p < 0.01).
As described in an earlier section, clonidine also increased overall the reaction times (linear regression, t(10) = 2.63, P = 0.02). Therefore, we considered the possibility that clonidine was having non-specific motivational effect, for instance on arousal or vigilance, which would then be responsible for both longer reaction times and smaller force peaks. In such a scenario, we might therefore expect a strong relation between the effects of clonidine on force peak and reaction time.
To assess whether the effect of dose was the same on the two measures, we performed a two-way ANOVA on z-scored force peak and reaction as the dependent variables. Independent variables were dose and measure type (force peak vs. reaction time). We found a significant main effect of dose (F
3,16 = 5.25, P = 0.01) and measure (F
1,16 = 4.72, p = 0.04), but importantly also a significant interaction between the two (F
3,16 = 8.59, p = 0.001), indicating that the effect of clonidine differs significantly between the two measures. We further explored this using a linear regression (Fig. 6d); there was no reliable correlation between the two measures (r(10) = − 0.4, p = 0.19). Moreover, we ran separate linear regressions excluding the highest dose of clonidine, where the effect on reaction times appeared most prominently (see Fig. 5a). While the effect on force peak was still significant even when only analyzing the low and mid doses (t(7) = − 3.93, p = 0.005), this was not the case for reaction time (t(7) = 0.16, p = 0.88). This was coherent with a stronger linear effect of dose on force peak (β = − 0.222 ± 0.068) than on reaction time (β = 0.106 ± 0.040). Therefore, effort production appears more sensitive to clonidine manipulations than any reaction time measures.
Finally, given that we had found a relationship between changes in reaction times and choice consistency under different doses of clonidine, we explored whether there was any similar connection between each animals’ average force peak and consistency parameter. However, we found no significant linear relationship between measures (t(10) = − 1.39, p = 0.15). Together, this demonstrates that clonidine has a specific, separable, and dose-dependent influence on different aspects of motivation and that the effect of clonidine on force production and choice behavior (as indexed by the monkeys’ speed-consistency trade-off) is at least partially dissociable.