Abstract
The exploration of the drivers of interpersonal alignment processes in romantic partners is particularly relevant to policies that promote green behavior because social anchoring in families and relationships can undermine effective behavioral change. The present paper aims to investigate the role important life events, such as childbirth, marriage, and the acquisition of real estate, in explaining the alignment of pro-environmental behavior within couples over time. The adoption of a life-course perspective is a novelty. We draw on a representative longitudinal panel data set to investigate the significance of important life events and the distribution of economic power within a relationship for green alignment. The empirical results are double edged. They show that partners who are expecting the birth of their first child are more similar than partners who are not expecting children. After childbirth, partners become significantly more dissimilar. Furthermore, we find evidence of a reconciliation effect. Over time, partners who divorce and continue cohabiting align more than partners who do not divorce.
1 Introduction
Romantic relationships are often the center of the social life of the individual. Prior studies of spousal correlations identify similarities in, among others, risk preferences (Di Falco and Vieider 2018; Kimball et al. 2009), stock-market participation (Hellström et al., 2013), and the uptake of smoking (Clark and Etilé, 2006). While numerous cross-sectional studies also reveal similarities in pro-environmental behavior (Grønhøj and Ölander, 2007; Grønhøj and Thøgersen 2011; Longhi 2013; Thøgersen and Grønhøj, 2010), only a few address timing and the question whether alignment processes are punctuated by important life events.
This paper concerns similarities in the pro-environmental behavior of couples and examines the extent to which partners become more similar empirically. The investment model of partnership attachment (Leonhardt et al. 2021; Rusbult et al. 2012) suggests that relationship quality is an economic good that partner co-produce by aligning their behaviors in a life-cycle process of preference socialization through daily interaction (Grønhøj, 2006; Moschis 1987). It predicts that partnership commitment is expressed as a reduction in intrahousehold preference heterogeneity, because highly committed partners renounce personal preferences in order to accommodate each other’s interests (Rusbult et al., 2012). This psychological model fits with and informs economic models of intrahousehold resource allocation, which assume that the preferences of household members are heterogeneous and emphasize the dependence of household consumption on the weighted utility functions of all household members (Browning et al. 1994; Browning and Chiappori 1998).
Moreover, the empirical literature on investment model of partnership attachment shows that the strength of commitment depends on the relationship stage of a couple (Leonhardt et al., 2021; Rusbult et al., 2012). This entails that the level of alignment regarding pro-environmental behaviors should change throughout the life-course. Hence, the paper adopts a life-course perspective, which is a novelty. We consider the impact of significant life events, such as marriage, the birth of a first child, and property acquisition, on the alignment process. Considering important life events is also warranted because they might present windows of opportunity for behavioral change. Many pro-environmental behaviors are habitual. Important life events allow habits to be reviewed and new practices to be established. Those practices, in turn, can develop into new routines (Spaargaren, 2003). The paper inquires whether life-changing events, such as the birth of a first child, marriage, or the purchase of a home, change the degree of similarity that partners exhibit in their pro-environmental behavior.
Using two waves of the UK Household Longitudinal Study (UKHLS), we observed absolute difference in the green behavior indices of couples to identify within-couple similarities. The indices are derived from eleven pro-environmental behaviors. We employed a basic difference-in-differences (DiD) approach to estimate changes in the differences between partners’ pro-environmental behavior. If partners align their pro-environmental behavior, the difference between them should decrease over time; if the partners diverge, the difference should increase. Our results show that the differences between partners only decrease marginally over time. The results also provide evidence of a double-edged window-of-opportunity effect in pro-environmental partnership alignment. One of our findings is that partners become more similar in their pro-environmental behavior before the birth of their first child. After the child is born, they become more dissimilar. Similarly, we find evidence of a reconciliation effect. Divorced partners who remain in the same household align more closely.
Our empirical findings add to the literature on similarities in individual pro-environmental behavior by providing a longitudinal analysis of life-changing events as an explanation of pro-environmental alignment between partners. Some studies examine the similarities between the pro-environmental preferences and behaviors of partners (Grønhøj and Ölander, 2007; Grønhøj and Thøgersen 2011; Longhi 2013; Thøgersen and Grønhøj, 2010). While it is possible to draw inferences about these similarities from previous studies, the channels that underlie the correlations have not yet been examined. Instead of studying cross-sectional correlations, we focus on whether couples become more similar in their green behavior over time, and we consider important life changing events as possible turning points.
Investigating dynamic social interaction effects in adults also contributes indirectly to the literature on the transmission of green behaviors across generations. So much is suggested by recent research on endogenizing preference formation in children through intergenerational transmission (see Bisin and Verdier 2010; Hellström et al., 2020). Several studies also emphasize the relevance of the intergenerational transmission of preferences and behaviors to the socialization of environmental concerns among children (Gotschi et al., 2009; Grønhøj and Thøgersen 2009, 2012; Matthies et al. 2012; Moschis 1987). The socialization of sustainable development into children is assumed to be more effective when it is reinforced by parents with matching preferences. A study of the mechanisms by which important life events change the degree of similarity that partners exhibit in their pro-environmental behaviors is thus warranted.
The remainder of the paper is structured as follows: Sect. 2 briefly describes the data set and the variables that are included in the empirical analysis. The econometric approach is also presented. Section 3 describes and discusses the estimation results, and Sect. 4 concludes.
2 Data, variables, and econometric approach
2.1 Data
We used the UKHLS, a long-run panel of nationally representative, randomly sampled married and cohabiting individuals, for the empirical analysis of couple-level alignment. Wave 1 (2009–2010) and Wave 4 (2012–2013) contained questions on pro-environmental behaviors and preferences. We combined the two waves, which yielded a total of 98,151 responses from 67,557 distinct individuals. Among the 50,994 individuals who were observed in the 2009–2010 wave, 30,594 were still present in the 2012–2013 one, corresponding to an attrition rate of approximately 40%, which is in line with attrition levels in comparable household surveys. We then excluded respondents who did not have a partner in either of the two waves or who had changed partners between waves. This reduction yielded a total of 8,249 constant couples (16,498 individuals). In the last step, we also excluded all couples for which there were missing dependent-variable values in either of the two waves. This procedure resulted in a balanced panel of 6,349 couples (12,698 individuals) that had been observed on two different occasions. We imputed the independent variables through Markov chain Monte Carlo procedures for handling missing cases. Those procedures are premised on the assumption that all variables in the imputation model follow a joint multivariate normal distribution.
2.2 Variables
Our main dependent variable is based on 11 questions on environmental behavior.Footnote 1 We used the answers to create a mean pro-environmental behavior scale, as proposed by Binder and Blankenberg (2017). In our scale, higher values indicate more environment-friendly behavior. Our aim was to measure alignment between partners. Accordingly, we created the variable “Green behavior” to capture the absolute differences between the pro-environmental behavior scores of partners and the changes in those differences between the two waves.
We consider important life events (changes in marital status, childbirth, and real estate acquisition) to be our main explanatory variables. Previous work demonstrates that individual characteristics drive pro-environmental behaviors (Stern 2000). Accordingly, we also account for attitudinal factors, such as beliefs in the necessity of climate action, and for socioeconomic factors, such as age, education, and income. Since differences in these factors are likely to influence couples' pro-environmental behavior, we also consider differences in the corresponding level variables.
Appendix 1 contains a detailed description of the variables in the data set. Table 1 overviews the main variables of interest. The couples in the sample overwhelmingly exhibited green behavior and medium-to-strong agreement with the proposition that something needs to be done about climate change, a proxy for pro-environmental attitudes. On average, the differences in green behavior were small, and the differences in green attitudes were even smaller.
About 83% of the couples in Wave 2009/10 and 88% of the couples in Wave 2012/13 were married. A small fraction of respondents (4.7%) married between the waves, as Table 2 indicates. In both waves, most couples did not have children but owned a house. Between the waves, the number of couples that faced a transition to parenthood was equal to the number of couples that entered the empty nest phase. Most respondents reported income above the median. Income distribution tended to be unequal in both waves.
2.3 Econometric approach
The effect of interest is the change in the absolute difference between the environmental behavior scale scores of partners who had experienced an important life event and those who had not experienced such events. Therefore, we adopted a basic DiD estimation technique with several categories of contextual factors j and two time periods that represent Wave 2009/10 (t = 0) and Wave 2012/13 (t = 1). Our econometric model is as follows:
where \(Green\, difference_{ct}\) is the absolute difference in the pro-environmental behavior scale score for couple c at time t. \(Wave_{t}\) denotes time, and the vector \(Life Event_{j} { }\) denotes exposure to important life-changing events. \(X_{ct}\) includes attitudinal factors at the relationship level, such as beliefs about the necessity of climate action (climate attitudes) and socioeconomic controls, such as income, education, age, and place of residence. \(\varepsilon_{ct}\) is the error term.\({ }Wave_{t}\) represents time effects, regardless of the occurrence of life changing events. \(Life Event_{j} { }\) is equal to 1 if the couple in question experienced a life-changing event (for instance, if they married between t = 0 and t = 1), regardless of the value of t, and it is equal to 0 if the couple did not experience such an event. The interaction term \(LifeEvent_{j} \times Wave_{t}\) reveals evidence of the effect of these life-changing events on changes in the difference between the pro-environmental behavior of the partners.
3 Effects of life-changing events on differences in pro-environmental behavior in couples
Table 3 presents parameter estimates (including robust cluster t-statistics) from the DiD estimation that we used to evaluate the differences between the pro-environmental behavior scale scores of couples across the two waves. The analysis accounts for important life-changing events. We expect that couples that experience climactic events should exhibit a more pronounced tendency to change their routines, and thus to align or diverge, than couples that do not. We first considered changes in marital status as determinants of differences in pro-environmental behavior. Specification (1) shows that the pro-environmental behavior scale scores of couples that were not married in either wave exhibit no significant change over time. Similarly, marital status in Wave 2009/10 did not, in and of itself, produce statistically significant variations in behavioral differences. In addition, being married in both waves did not accelerate the alignment effect significantly, as compared to being unmarried in Wave 2012/13. However, changes in marital status between waves did. While marrying was not found to exert a statistically significant influence on the alignment process, the pro-environmental behavior scale scores of partners who had divorced became more similar than those of couples that were unmarried in Wave 20,012/13. Thus, couples that divorced but did not change households (otherwise, they would not have entered the sample) became more similar than unwed couples. It must be noted that the reconciliation effect that we observed is most likely a result of our selection procedure: we compared couples that had continued to cohabit after divorcing, with couples that had not divorced. It stands to reason that the divorced couples under observation were very different from the (unobserved) divorced couples in which one or both partners had left the family home after divorcing. The observed effect is thus not attributable to divorce but, more likely, to a reconciliation that facilitated the continuation of the relationship (and selection into the sample). Studies on divorce and reconciliation show that partners who are interested in reconciliation after divorce often cite differences in attitudes, beliefs, and tastes (“growing apart”), as well as economic problems, as the main reasons for the divorce. Divorced partners who are not interested in reconciliation cite abuse and/or adultery as the main reason for the end of the marriage (Hawkins et al. 2012; Pearce Plauche et al. 2016). These findings suggest that couples that reconcile often address divergences in attitudes, beliefs, and tastes by realigning themselves to each other to address the perceived cause of their divorce. This realignment, in turn, leads to a reduction in the difference between their preferences for environmental behaviors, explaining our finding.
As far as childbirth is concerned, Specification (2) demonstrates that differences in pro-environmental behavior declined significantly over time among couples that were childless ex post. The differences between partners in couples that reported childbirth between the two waves but did not have children in Wave 2009/10 were lower than the same differences in couples that did not have children in either wave but also in the two groups of couples that had children in Wave 2009/10 (the results on the impact of the presence of children in Wave 2009/10 on differences in couple behavior are not statistically significant and are very close to being zero). The implication is that partners probably align their behavior before the birth of their first child. Furthermore, we found a significant decrease in alignment following the birth of a first child.
These findings largely mirror the results of family psychological studies of changes in couples’ relationship quality and attachment during the transition to parenthood. The current literature identifies the birth of a first child as an especially dynamic phase in the development of a couple’s division of labor and in the allocation of economic goods, such as leisure time. The antenatal phase is typically associated with practical preparation processes, such as childproofing, relocating to a larger home, or transitioning to homeownership (Kley and Drobnič, 2019), which entail changes in household tasks and leisure activities. These practical adjustments are accompanied by investments into relationship quality that take the form of shared leisure activities, practices, and routines. Recent research into the typical course of the decline in relationship quality after the transition to parenthood (Doss et. Al 2009; Kluwer 2010; Adamsons 2013) show that the decline in question is mitigated significantly by higher antenatal relationship quality and relationship attachment (Leonhardt et al. 2021; Ter Kuile et al. 2021). This literature suggests that most couples invest in relationship quality and attachment before childbirth in order to buffer the decline in relationship quality that follows it. Overall, these findings show that the period of preparing for a first child is characterized by very strong commitment to the partnership, with no marked tendency to divergence in the division of labor between the genders (Goldberg et al. 1985; Yavorsky et al. 2015). This can explain the high concordance between the green behaviors of antenatal couples in the UKHLS: according to the investment model of partnership attachment (Leonhardt et al. 2021; Rusbult et al. 2012), shared routines and beliefs are means of expressing attachment and stabilizing relationship quality.
Conversely, the studies that focus on the postnatal phase describe a very different dynamic. First, that phase is marked by a substantial change in the division of labor in couples. New parents spend considerably more time on family work after childbirth (Gauthier and Furstenberg 2002). This results in a strong reduction in the availability of leisure time (Cantwell and Sanik 1993; Kluwer 2010; Yavorsky et al. 2015). The redistribution of the increased volume of labor and the reduced amount of leisure time is often gendered, leading to a divergence in temporal and economic constraints. According to economic models of intrahousehold resource allocation, that divergence increases preference heterogeneity (Browning et al. 1994; Browning and Chiappori 1998). This point can be illustrated through one of the environmental behaviors that we included in our environmental behavior scale: using public transportation often is much less convenient for the partner who does most of the childcare, even if both partners preferred it prior to childbirth.
In conclusion, our results are concordant with the findings that show that the birth of a first child puts pressure on the availability of time and on the observance of environmental practices. This pressure may suppress alignment, causing couples that have recently had their first child to diverge more than childless couples. To simplify somewhat, Specification (2) suggests a two-step process. Partners align their pro-environmental behaviors to each other before childbirth (nestmaking). After childbirth, they diverge in order to readapt to the exigencies of parenting (role differentiation). As the estimation results depicted by Specification (3) show, we found several null effects, which must be interpreted with caution. They run counter to our theoretical expectations. Family scientific studies report that the transition to home ownership is regularly linked to family formation (Mulder and Smits 1999; Pearce Plauche et al. 2016; Wiik et al. 2009). That process should be expected to follow the same pattern as that observed around the birth of a first child, that is, couples should invest in the relationship prior to the acquisition of a home, aligning their behaviors. Afterward, they should diverge until they settle into stable family roles. The (non-significant) effects in the data cannot rule out such an effect, and it must be noted that the effects of the acquisition of a home point into the expected direction. Moreover, from the perspective of the investment theory of attachment, differences in behavior should remain rather stable or increase very slowly in couples that own homes, mirroring the typical slow long-term decline in relationship quality that is observed in marriages after family formation (Adamsons 2013; Doss et al. 2009; Kluwer 2010). This theoretical expectation is also at odds with the data. While no time effect could be demonstrated for either non-houseowners or for respondents whose ownership status changed, the difference in pro-environmental behavior decreased among couples that owned a house in both waves (compared to non-houseowners). However, the effect that we found is only marginally significant and might therefore reflect a chance finding.
4 Conclusion and discussion
Do important life events affect the similarities in the pro-environmental behavior of romantic partners? The paper investigated the alignment of pro-environmental behavior in relationships by using two waves of UKHLS data. We adopted a DiD approach to analyze the effects of important life events on pro-environmental behavior. Our results furnish some evidence of a window of opportunity for the alignment of pro-environmental behavior. As an important life event, the birth of a child disrupts established behaviors and forces couples to review their common pro-environmental behaviors in a two-stage process. First, in the nestmaking phase that precedes childbirth, partners grow closer in their pro-environmental behaviors. Second, in the role differentiation phase, the scarcity of time and practical constraints cause divergence. Notably, in our sample, partners who divorced and continued cohabiting aligned more closely than unmarried couples, suggesting an (unexpected) reconciliation effect.
In summary, we provided correlational evidence for the proposition that important life events drive alignment in the pro-environmental behavior of partners, at least to some extent, and we made progress toward establishing causality through a DiD approach. This finding accords with field experimental evidence of the alignment of spousal risk preferences (Di Falco and Vieider 2018). However, all of the observed effects are small. The initial similarity between couples was already high. One explanation of the limited explanatory power of the results may be that alignment predates cohabitation and marriage. However, the periods before cohabitation are not captured by most longitudinal data sets. Observing the first years of a relationship is particularly important to understanding alignment. Furthermore, our estimates may be biased due to the challenges of estimating social interaction effects from observational data, which result from the absence of exogenous variation. Direct influencing effects, assortative mating, selection effects (Becker 1974), and common social-environment factors (Manski, 2003) can lead to correlation, though biased, between peers' preferences and their behavior in both space and time (Powdthavee, 2009). The extensive literature on assortative mating documents the selection of partners on the basis of similarities in preferences (for risk preferences, see Dohmen et al. (2012); for social preferences, see Tognetti et al. (2014)), personal characteristics, such as education and income (Di Domingue et al. 2014; Greenwood et al. 2014), and the pursuit of public goods (Lam 1988). It is conceivable that individuals who behave in an environmentally friendly manner tend to select partners who act similarly. Similarities in environmental behavior can also emerge when partners share the same social environment.
It is possible that the estimation results suffer from simultaneity bias. Most household surveys that measure environmental behavior follow an annual rhythm. However, influence processes among partners likely operate on a shorter time scale—most partners interact every day. From a modeling perspective, the processes of alignment between partners must be treated as being simultaneous (Breznau, 2018). If that is the case, the environmental attitudes and behaviors of partners must be analyzed as endogenous variables. Dynamic simultaneous causation models that include unobserved fixed individual and couple effects can potentially account for simultaneity bias. However, while such estimations are feasible in theory, they are often impracticable. For instance, dynamic panel approaches commonly suffer from weak instruments because second lags are often poor predictors of lagged first differences. Two strategies are commonly employed to resolve these problems: the addition of further lags and lagged differences as instruments and the estimation of the more efficient GMM estimators proposed by Arellano and Bond (1991) and Blundell and Bond (1998). However, this is only feasible for T > 3, and T > 10 is more realistic in practice. Regrettably, high-quality panel data with a sufficient number of waves that measure pro-environmental behaviors do not, as far as we know, exist.
The last limitation concerns selection bias. Since only cohabitating and married couples are observed in a household panel, the estimates for alignment over time may be biased by the filtering process that unfolds during early couple formation and the transition to cohabitation and marriage. For instance, individuals who are very prone to transitioning to cohabitation or marriage might also be more susceptible to aligning their behavior to that of their partner. All these limitations notwithstanding, our results show that further research on alignment processes in couples is worthwhile and warranted. The results also highlight the need for dense, high-quality panel data on pro-environmental behaviors. This data may be gathered through novel collection approaches, such as multi-actor panels, which are better suited to the observation of couples before cohabitation.
Data availability
There is no public use file available for the data used in the present study. Requests to access the present data should be directed to anja.koebrich@uni-kassel.de.
Change history
22 February 2022
The corresponding author’s first and last name was incorrectly tagged in article XML. It has been updated with the correct first name “Anja” and family name “Köbrich León”.
Notes
Appendix 1 contains a list of the environmental behaviors considered.
References
Adamsons K (2013) Predictors of relationship quality during the transition to parenthood. J Reprod Infant Psychol 31(2):160–171
Arellano M, Bond S (1991) Some tests of specification for panel data: monte carlo evidence and an application to employment equations. Rev Econ Stud 58(2):277
Becker GS (1974) A theory of marriage: part II. J Polit Econ 82(2):S11–S26
Binder M, Blankenberg A-K (2017) Green lifestyles and subjective well-being: more about self-image than actual behavior? J Econ Behav Organ 137:304–323
Bisin A, Verdier T (2010) The economics of cultural transmission and socialization. National Bureau of Economic Research, Cambridge, MA
Blundell R, Bond S (1998) Initial conditions and moment restrictions in dynamic panel data models. J Econ 87(1):115–143
Breznau N (2018) Simultaneous feedback models with macro-comparative cross-sectional data. Met Data Anal 12(2):265–308. https://doi.org/10.12758/mda.2017.07
Browning M, Chiappori PA (1998) Efficient intra-household allocations: a general characterization and empirical tests. Econometrica 66(6):1241
Browning M, Bourguignon F, Chiappori P-A, Lechene V (1994) Income and outcomes: a structural model of intrahousehold allocation. J Polit Econ 102(6):1067–1096
Cantwell ML, Sanik MM (1993) Leisure before and after parenthood. Soc Indic Res 30(2–3):139–147
Clark AE, Etilé F (2006) Don’t give up on me baby: spousal correlation in smoking behaviour. J Health Econ 25(5):958–978
Di Falco S, Vieider FM (2018) Assimilation in the risk preferences of spouses. Econ Inq 56(3):1809–1816
Di Domingue BW, Fletcher J, Conley D, Boardman JD (2014) Genetic and educational assortative mating among US adults. Proc Natl Acad Sci 111(22):7996–8000
Dohmen T, Falk A, Huffman D, Sunde U (2012) The intergenerational transmission of risk and trust attitudes. Rev Econ Stud 79(2):645–677
Doss BD, Rhoades GK, Stanley SM, Markman HJ (2009) The effect of the transition to parenthood on relationship quality: an 8-year prospective study. J Pers Soc Psychol 96(3):601–619
Gauthier AH, Furstenberg FF (2002) The transition to adulthood: a time use perspective. Ann Am Acad Pol Soc Sci 580(1):153–171
Goldberg WA, Michaels GY, Lamb ME (1985) Husbands’ and wives’ adjustment to pregnancy and first parenthood. J Fam Issues 6(4):483–503
Gotschi E, Vogel S, Lindenthal T, Larcher M (2009) The role of knowledge, social norms, and attitudes toward organic products and shopping behavior: survey results from high school students in Vienna. J Environ Educ 41(2):88–100
Greenwood J, Guner N, Kocharkov G, Santos C (2014) Marry your like: assortative mating and income inequality. Am Econ Rev 104(5):348–353
Grønhøj A (2006) Communication about consumption: a family process perspective on ‘green’ consumer practices. J Consum Behav 5(6):491–503
Grønhøj A, Ölander F (2007) A gender perspective on environmentally related family consumption. J Consum Behav 6(4):218–235
Grønhøj A, Thøgersen J (2009) Like father, like son? Intergenerational transmission of values, attitudes, and behaviours in the environmental domain. J Environ Psychol 29(4):414–421
Grønhøj A, Thøgersen J (2011) Feedback on household electricity consumption: learning and social influence processes. Int J Consum Stud 35(2):138–145
Grønhøj A, Thøgersen J (2012) Action speaks louder than words: the effect of personal attitudes and family norms on adolescents’ pro-environmental behaviour. J Econ Psychol 33(1):292–302
Hawkins AJ, Willoughby BJ, Doherty WJ (2012) Reasons for divorce and openness to marital reconciliation. J Divorce Remarriage 53(6):453–463
Hellström J, Zetterdahl E, Hanes N (2013) Loved ones matter: family effects and stock market participation Umeå Economic Studies 865 Umeå University, Department of Economics. https://ideas.repec.org/p/hhs/umnees/0865.html
Hellstrrm J, Lapanan N, Olsson R (2015) Intergenerational transmission of pro-social values: socially responsible investment among parents and adult children SSRN Electron J
Hellström J, Lapanan N, Olsson R (2020) Socially responsible investments among parents and adult children. Eur Econ Rev 121, 103328. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroecorev.2019.103328
Kimball MS, Sahm CR, Shapiro MD (2009) Risk preferences in the PSID: individual imputations and family covariation. Am Econ Rev 99(2):363–368
Kley S, Drobnič S (2019) Does moving for family nest-building inhibit mothers’ labour force (re-)entry? Demogr Res 40:155–184
Kluwer ES (2010) From partnership to parenthood: a review of marital change across the transition to parenthood. J Fam Theory Rev 2(2):105–125
Lam D (1988) Marriage markets and assortative mating with household public goods: theoretical results and empirical implications. J Hum Resour 23(4):462
Leonhardt ND, Rosen NO, Dawson SJ, Kim JJ, Johnson MD, Impett EA (2021) Relationship satisfaction and commitment in the transition to parenthood: a couple‐centered approach. J Marriage Family 84(1):80–100. https://doi.org/10.1111/jomf.12785
Longhi S (2013) Individual pro-environmental behaviour in the household context, ISER Working Paper Series [Colchester], 31 pp http://hdl.handle.net/10419/91690
Manski CF (2003) Identification problems in the social sciences and everyday life. South Econ J 70(1):11
Matthies E, Selge S, Klöckner CA (2012) The role of parental behaviour for the development of behaviour specific environmental norms – the example of recycling and re-use behaviour. J Environ Psychol 32(3):277–284
Moschis GP (1987) Consumer socialization: a life-cycle perspective. Lexington Books, Lexington, Mass.
Mulder CH, Smits J (1999) First-time home-ownership of couples: the effect of inter-generational transmission. Eur Sociol Rev 15(3):323–337
Pearce Plauche H, Marks LD, Hawkins AJ (2016) Why we chose to stay together: qualitative interviews with separated couples who chose to reconcile. J Divorce Remarriage 57(5):317–337
Powdthavee N (2009) I can’t smile without you: spousal correlation in life satisfaction. J Econ Psychol 30(4):675–689
Rusbult CE, Agnew CR, Arriaga XB (2012) The investment model of commitment processes. In: van Lange P, Kruglanski A, Higgins E, van Lange PA (eds) Handbook of theories of social psychology, 2. Sage, Los Angeles, Calif., pp 218–231
Spaargaren G (2003) Sustainable consumption: a theoretical and environmental policy perspective. Soc Nat Resour 16(8):687–701
Stern PC (2000) New environmental theories: toward a coherent theory of environmentally significant behavior. J Soc Issues 56(3):407–424
Ter Kuile H, Finkenauer C, van der Lippe T, Kluwer ES (2021) Changes in relationship commitment across the transition to parenthood: pre-pregnancy happiness as a protective resource. Front Psychol 12:622160
Thøgersen J, Grønhøj A (2010) Electricity saving in households—a social cognitive approach. Energy Policy 38(12):7732–7743
Tognetti A, Berticat C, Raymond M, Faurie C (2014) Assortative mating based on cooperativeness and generosity. J Evol Biol 27(5):975–981
Wiik KA, Bernhardt E, Noack T (2009) A study of commitment and relationship quality in Sweden and Norway. J Marriage Fam 71(3):465–477
Yavorsky JE, Dush CMK, Schoppe-Sullivan SJ (2015) The production of inequality: the gender division of labor across the transition to parenthood. J Marriage Fam 77(3):662–679
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. Financial support for this project by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF) under the dunding priority “FONA 3—Research for Sustainable Development” under grant agreement No. 01LN1708A is gratefully acknowledged.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Both authors contributed equally to the article and approved the submitted version.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.
Ethical Standards
Ethical review and approval were not required for the paper.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Appendices
Appendix 1: Variable description
See Table
4.
Appendix 2: Additional tables
See Tables
5,
6 and
7.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Köbrich León, A., Schobin, J. Romance and the ozone layer: panel evidence on green behavior in couples. Empir Econ 63, 2101–2123 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00181-021-02185-2
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00181-021-02185-2
Keywords
- Similarity
- Environmental behavior
- Couples
- Life-cycle perspective
- UKHLS
JEL Classification
- D10
- D91
- Q50