Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Can conflict affect individuals’ preferences for income redistribution?

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Population Economics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examines the effect of conflict on individuals’ preferences for income redistribution. To this end, I compare individuals’ preferences before and after a war between Israel and a Lebanese terror organization in 2006. Using information from both panel and repeated cross-sectional datasets, I find that residing in war-affected regions increases individuals’ support for income redistribution. An examination of several mechanisms that may elicit this finding reveals that conflict increases the importance of luck in individuals’ perceptions and rules out other channels such as changes in individuals’ risk preferences or beliefs. Placebo analyses using the years preceding the war and individuals’ preferences unrelated to violence (e.g., attitudes about the environment) reinforce my main findings.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the International Social Survey Programme (http://www.issp.org/data-download/by-topic/), from the European Social Survey (https://www.europeansocialsurvey.org/), and from the Survey of Health, Aging and Retirement in Europe (http://www.share-project.org/home0.html). Restrictions apply to the availability of these data, which were used under license for this study.

Notes

  1. For example, the terror attacks on the USA on September 11, 2001, and the ongoing Russian–Ukrainian conflict and Syrian civil war.

  2. Namely, they are less likely to believe in a just world (Bénabou and Tirole 2006).

  3. This study also relates to the literature that explores how experiencing income inequality (Kuziemko et al. 2015; Bechtel et al. 2018; Roth and Wohlfart 2018; Pellicer et al. 2019), recessions (Fisman et al. 2015), and living under a communist regime (Alesina and Fuchs-Schündeln 2007) affect individuals’ preferences for redistribution. In addition, this study relates to the literature that explores the effect of culture (Eugster et al. 2012), perception about intergenerational mobility (Alesina et al. 2018), and ethnic diversity (Dahlberg et al. 2012) on support for redistribution.

  4. This study also relates to the literature that examines the effects of violence on risk preferences (Kim and Lee 2014; Callen et al. 2014; Wang and Young 2020; Jakiela and Ozier 2019), time preferences (Voors et al. 2012; Shai 2022b), and pro-social behavior (e.g., Blattman 2009; Bellows and Miguel 2009; Gneezy and Fessler 2011; Grosjean 2014; Bauer et al. 2016; Lupu and Peisakhin 2017).

  5. Known as the Second Lebanon War (2006). For more information, please visit: https://www.gov.il/en/Departments/General/the-second-lebanon-war.

  6. Individuals therefore did not leave the northern region, as there had been no anticipation of the war.

  7. The last military confrontation between Israel and Hezbollah occurred in 1996. In 2000, Hezbollah abducted three Israeli soldiers. That incident, however, was not followed by violence large-scale military response.

  8. For more information on the Israeli National Health Insurance Law, visit: http://www.health.gov.il/English/Topics/RightsInsured/Pages/default.aspx.

  9. For more information on the National Insurance Institute, visit: https://www.btl.gov.il/English%20Homepage/Benefits/Pages/default.aspx.

  10. For more information, visit: http://www.oecd.org/economy/surveys/Israel-2018-OECD-economic-survey-overview.pdf.

  11. I estimate Eq. (1) using linear probability model (LPM).

  12. For more detailed information on the data and the constructed variables, see the data in the Appendix.

  13. European Social Survey Round 1 Data (2002) and European Social Survey Round 4 Data (2008). For more information, visit: www.europeansocialsurvey.org/.

  14. The ESS was conducted in Israel in four additional years (2010, 2012, 2014, 2016). However, in order to capture a short time window before and after the war, I use only the years 2002 and 2008.

  15. Answers were given using the following scale: strongly agree, agree, neither agree nor disagree, disagree, and strongly disagree.

  16. In Israel, political orientation is usually associated with attitudes regarding the peace process between Israel and the Arabs, and it is less associated with economic perceptions. I therefore did not use measures of political orientation.

  17. ISSP Research Group (199520032019). For more information, visit: www.issp.org/menu-top/home/.

  18. Importantly, the upcoming analysis examines individuals’ preferences before and after the war. Due to missing information, I did not use other ISSP modules.

  19. Other studies (e.g., Corneo and Grüner 2002; Bernasconi 2006; Breznau and Hommerich 2019) also use individuals’ preferences for redistribution, taken from the ISSP.

  20. Over the years, Israel had several military clashes with Palestinian forces in the Gaza Strip, located near Israel’s southern region. Moreover, individuals who reside in the Judea–Samaria region are more likely to be exposed to terror attacks. Therefore, throughout the analysis, I exclude Israelis who reside in these areas from the control group. Using the ISSP dataset, I exclude individuals who reside in the southern region. The Judea–Samaria and Gaza Strip region categories are not mentioned in this dataset.

  21. Following the ESS guidelines, in Fig. 2, I use post-stratification weights (PSPWGHT) throughout the calculations, adjusting for sampling error and non-response bias. For more information, visit: https://www.europeansocialsurvey.org/docs/methodology/ESS_weighting_data_1.pdf.

  22. One should note that the ISSP and the ESS datasets include different years.

  23. Unfortunately, neither the ESS nor the ISSP contains information on preferences for redistribution toward out-groups (e.g., Arabs).

  24. Specifically, the Palestinian uprising against Israel (known as the “Second Intifada”) began in 2000 and was characterized by terror attacks against Israelis. One could argue that these terror attacks affected the control group because they occurred in certain and central places such as Jerusalem. Excluding Israelis who reside in Tel Aviv and Jerusalem (Israel’s largest cities) and the central district from the control group addresses this concern (panel A of online Appendix Table 2).

  25. The results remain the same when controlling for region fixed effects, instead of an indicator for whether individuals reside in the northern region or not (panel B of online Appendix Table 2).

  26. To reinforce the results, I conducted both propensity score matching (nearest-neighbor matching with a caliper of 0.01 and replacement) and coarsened exact matching. Specifically, I used data from the post-war period (year 2008) and matched individuals who reside in the war-affected region with individuals who reside in the regions not affected by war, based on the following similar characteristics: employment status, age, age squared, education, whether or not individuals’ children live at home, household income, and marital status. As can be seen in columns 1 and 2 in panel C of online Appendix Table 2, individuals who reside in the war-affected region are more likely to support redistribution, than individuals who reside in regions that were not affected by the war.

  27. To mitigate the concern that individuals who reside in the war-affected area differ in their characteristics from those who reside in regions not affected by war, I conducted entropy balancing (Hainmueller 2012). Specifically, using the ESS, I balanced treated individuals (those who reside in the war-affected region) with untreated individuals (those who reside in unaffected regions) on the following exogenous variables: age, age squared, education, whether or not individuals’ children live at home, and gender. By weighting the observations, I then estimated the difference-in-differences model, using Eq. (1). As can be seen in panel D of online Appendix Table 2, the results show that residing in war-affected regions increases support for income redistribution.

  28. Specifically, gaining information during war, such as the extent of property damage, may affect individuals’ support for redistribution. I therefore use individuals’ current news consumption as a proxy for news consumption during the war.

  29. Panels A and B of online Appendix Table 3 present the estimates for all the covariates using Eq. (1).

  30. To reinforce the results, I use data from the post-war period (year 2008), and as additional proxies for individuals’ preferences for redistribution, I utilize responses to the following two questions: “For a society to be fair, differences in people’s standard of living should be small,” and “Large differences in people’s incomes are acceptable to properly reward differences in talents and efforts” (these questions were not available in the pre-war survey). I then compare the preferences of individuals who reside in the northern region, against a control group of individuals who reside in other regions that were less exposed to the war. Using these measures, my findings reveal that after the war (year 2008), those who reside in the conflict area are more likely to support redistribution than those who reside in other regions (the difference between these two groups is statistically significant).

  31. The results remain the same when clustering the standard errors by individuals’ age (panel B of online Appendix Table 2).

  32. I also use the original statement: “It is the responsibility of the government to reduce the differences in income between people with high incomes and those with low incomes” and construct an outcome variable scaled between 1 (“strongly agree”) and 5 (“strongly disagree”). I then estimate Eq. (1), using this outcome, and find that the qualitative results remain the same. That is, residing in a war-affected area increases support for redistribution.

  33. To reinforce the results, I conducted both propensity score matching (nearest-neighbor matching with a caliper of 0.01 and replacement) and coarsened exact matching. Specifically, I used data from the post-war period (year 2010) and matched individuals who reside in the war-affected region with individuals who reside in the regions not affected by war, based on the following similar characteristics: employment status, age, age squared, marital status, income, gender, and education. As can be seen in columns 3 and 4 in panel C of online Appendix Table 2, individuals who reside in the war-affected region are more likely to support redistribution, than individuals who reside in regions that were not affected by the war.

  34. To mitigate the concern that individuals who reside in the war-affected area differ in their characteristics from those who reside in regions not affected by war, I conducted entropy balancing (Hainmueller 2012). Specifically, using the ISSP, I balanced treated individuals (those who reside in the war-affected region) with untreated individuals (those who reside in unaffected regions) on the following exogenous variables: age, age squared, education, and gender. By weighting the observations, I then estimated the difference-in-differences model, using Eq. (1). As can be seen in panel D of online Appendix Table 2, the results show that residing in war-affected regions increases support for income redistribution.

  35. Panel C of online Appendix Table 3 presents the estimates for all the covariates using Eq. (1).

  36. This paper uses data from SHARE Waves 1 and 5 (DOIs: 10.6103/SHARE.w1.710, 10.6103/SHARE.w2.710, 10.6103/SHARE.w3.710, 10.6103/SHARE.w4.710, 10.6103/SHARE.w5.710, 10.6103/SHARE.w6.710, 10.6103/SHARE.w7.711, 10.6103/SHARE.w8.100, 10.6103/SHARE.w8ca.100); see Börsch-Supan et al. (2013) for methodological details. (1) The SHARE data collection has been funded by the European Commission, DG RTD, through FP5 (QLK6-CT-2001-00360), FP6 (SHARE-I3: RII-CT-2006-062193, COMPARE: CIT5-CT-2005-028857, SHARELIFE: CIT4-CT-2006-028812), FP7 (SHARE-PREP: GA No. 211909, SHARE-LEAP: GA No. 227822, SHARE M4: GA No. 261982, DASISH: GA No. 283646), and Horizon 2020 (SHARE-DEV3: GA No. 676536, SHARE-COHESION: GA No. 870628, SERISS: GA No. 654221, SSHOC: GA No. 823782) and by DG Employment, Social Affairs & Inclusion through VS 2015/0195, VS 2016/0135, VS 2018/0285, VS 2019/0332, and VS 2020/0313. Additional funding from the German Ministry of Education and Research, the Max Planck Society for the Advancement of Science, and the U.S. National Institute on Aging (U01_AG09740-13S2, P01_AG005842, P01_AG08291, P30_AG12815, R21_AG025169, Y1-AG-4553-01, IAG_BSR06-11, OGHA_04-064, HHSN271201300071C, RAG052527A) and from various national funding sources is gratefully acknowledged (see www.share-project.org). The project development and data collection in Israel were supported by the National Institutes of Health (NIH) of the USA, National Insurance Institute of Israel, German-Israeli Foundation for Scientific Research and Development (GIF), European Commission through the 7th framework program, Ministry of Science and Technology, and Ministry of Senior Citizens. The data was collected by the Israeli Gerontological Data Center at the Hebrew University in Jerusalem. Wave 3 data collection in Israel was funded by the NIH (R01-AG031729) and the Ministry for Senior Citizens. The SHARE-Israel Project is administered by the Israel Gerontological Data Center at the Hebrew University in Jerusalem, Israel. The data were collected by the B. I. and Lucille Cohen Institute for Public Opinion Research.

  37. SHARE-Israel was conducted in 2005–2006 (Wave 1), 2009–2010 (Wave 2), 2013 (Wave 5), 2015 (Wave 6), and 2017 (Wave 7). Israel did not participate in the third and fourth waves of the SHARE survey.

  38. Although this question refers to violence originating from the Lebanon war or other terrorist actions (missile attacks from the Gaza Strip and hostile attacks), it allows me to investigate the mechanisms behind the increase in support for redistribution following violence.

  39. I did not use SHARE Wave 2 in my analysis because following the same individuals over several time periods may exacerbate attrition problems.

  40. Individuals who were interviewed in SHARE Wave 1 during or after the Second Lebanon War (i.e., after June 2006) were dropped from the analysis.

  41. I did not construct a general index of self-exposure to violence (including injuries/property damage) because suffering from injuries/property damage induces damage claims and, consequently, may increase support for redistribution, whereas the goal of this study is to examine whether violence exposure (but not necessarily property damage or injuries) alters individuals’ attitudes about the importance of luck and redistribution.

  42. For more information, visit: https://www.cbs.gov.il/EN/Pages/search/yearly.aspx.

  43. The SHARE data used in this study do not contain information on the individuals’ place of residence and locality or on the region/district in which they reside.

  44. To the first two questions, responses were coded into the following categories: (1) Often, (2) Sometimes, (3) Rarely, and (4) Never, and to the last question as follows: (1) Obvious excessive guilt or self-blame, (2) No such feelings, and (3) Mentions guilt or self-blame, but it is unclear if these constitute obvious or excessive guilt or self-blame.

  45. Specifically, I constructed a responsibility index scaled between 0 and 3 that counts whether individuals report that they rarely/never feel that what happens to them is out of their control and often/sometimes feel that they can do the things they want to do or tend to blame themselves or feel obvious excessive guilt.

  46. To reinforce the results, I conducted both propensity score matching (nearest-neighbor matching with a caliper of 0.01 and replacement) and coarsened exact matching. Specifically, I used data from the post-war period (year 2013) and matched individuals who were exposed to the war with individuals who were not exposed to the war, based on the following similar characteristics: income, age, age squared, marital status, education, and gender. As can be seen in columns 5 and 6 in panel C of online Appendix Table 2, individuals who were exposed to violence are less likely to feel responsible for their fate than those who were not exposed to violence.

  47. Online Appendix Table 4 presents the estimates for all the covariates using Eq. (2).

  48. The economics literature provides three possible explanations, which are not mutually exclusive, for increased pro-social behavior following violence: an economic channel, a change in social preferences, and a psychological effect (Bauer et al. 2016). Investigating these channels, however, is beyond the scope of this paper.

  49. Similarly, following Ajzen’s (1991) seminal paper, individuals may change their support for redistribution because their requisite opportunities and resources were harmed due to the war, which, in turn, decreased their perceived behavioral control.

  50. For more information, visit: https://www.cbs.gov.il/EN/Pages/search/yearly.aspx.

  51. Specifically, only 0.26% of the population left the northern region in the years before the war (2002–2005).

  52. Specifically, there is only one pre-war year in the ESS, and there are only two pre-war years in the ISSP.

References

  • Ajzen I (1991) The theory of planned behavior. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 50(2):179–211

    Google Scholar 

  • Alesina A, Angeletos GM (2005) Fairness and redistribution. Am Econ Rev 95(4):960–980

    Google Scholar 

  • Alesina A, Fuchs-Schündeln N (2007) Goodbye Lenin (or not?): the effect of communism on people’s preferences. Am Econ Rev 97(4):1507–1528

    Google Scholar 

  • Alesina A, Giuliano P (2011) Preferences for redistribution. In: Handbook of social economics (Vol. 1, pp. 93-131). North Holland

  • Alesina A, La Ferrara E (2005) Preferences for redistribution in the land of opportunities. J Pub Econ 89(5–6):897–931

    Google Scholar 

  • Alesina A, Stantcheva S, Teso E (2018) Intergenerational mobility and preferences for redistribution. Am Econ Rev 108(2):521–54

    Google Scholar 

  • Angrist JD, Pischke JS (2008) Mostly harmless econometrics: an empiricist’s companion. Princeton University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauer M, Cassar A, Chytilová J, Henrich J (2014) War’s enduring effects on the development of egalitarian motivations and in-group biases. Psychol Sci 25(1):47–57

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauer M, Blattman C, Chytilová J, Henrich J, Miguel E, Mitts T (2016) Can war foster cooperation? J Econ Perspect 30(3):249–74

    Google Scholar 

  • Bechtel MM, Liesch R, Scheve KF (2018) Inequality and redistribution behavior in a give-or-take game. Proc Natl Acad Sci 115(14):3611–3616

    Google Scholar 

  • Bellows J, Miguel E (2009) War and local collective action in Sierra Leone. J Pub Econ 93(11–12):1144–1157

    Google Scholar 

  • Bénabou R, Tirole J (2006) Belief in a just world and redistributive politics. Q J Econ 121(2):699–746

    Google Scholar 

  • Bergmann M, Kneip T, De Luca G, Scherpenzeel A (2019). Survey participation in the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE), Wave 1-7. Based on Release 7.0.0. SHARE Working Paper Series 41-2019. Munich: MEA, Max Planck Institute for Social Law and Social Policy

  • Bernasconi M (2006) Redistributive taxation in democracies: evidence on people’s satisfaction. Europ J Pol Econ 22(4):809–837

    Google Scholar 

  • Breznau N, Hommerich C (2019) No generalizable effect of income inequality on public support for governmental redistribution among rich democracies 1987–2010. Soc Sci Res 81:170–191

    Google Scholar 

  • Blattman C (2009) From violence to voting: war and political participation in Uganda. Am Polit Sci Rev 103(2):231–247

    Google Scholar 

  • Börsch-Supan A, Jürges H (eds) (2005) The survey of health, ageing and retirement in Europe – methodology. Mannheim Research Institute for the Economics of Aging, Mannheim

  • Börsch-Supan A, Brandt M, Hunkler C, Kneip T, Korbmacher J, Malter F, Schaan B, Stuck S, Zuber S (2013) Data resource profile: the survey of health, ageing and retirement in Europe (SHARE). Int J Epidemiol. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyt088

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Börsch-Supan A (2020a) Survey of health, ageing and retirement in Europe (SHARE) Wave 1. Release Version: 7.1.0. SHARE-ERIC. Data set. https://doi.org/10.6103/SHARE.w1.710

  • Börsch-Supan A (2020b) Survey of health, ageing and retirement in Europe (SHARE) Wave 5. Release

  • Callen M, Isaqzadeh M, Long JD, Sprenger C (2014) Violence and risk preference: experimental evidence from Afghanistan. Am Econ Rev 104(1):123–48

    Google Scholar 

  • Center for Substance Abuse Treatment (2014) Trauma-informed care in behavioral health services

  • Chuang Y, Schechter L (2015) Stability of experimental and survey measures of risk, time, and social preferences: a review and some new results. J Dev Econ 117:151–170

    Google Scholar 

  • Corneo G, Grüner HP (2002) Individual preferences for political redistribution. J Public Econ 83(1):83–107

    Google Scholar 

  • Croson R, Gneezy U (2009) Gender differences in preferences. J Econ Lit 47(2):448–74

    Google Scholar 

  • Dahlberg M, Edmark K, Lundqvist H (2012) Ethnic diversity and preferences for redistribution. J Polit Econ 120(1):41–76

    Google Scholar 

  • Di Tella R, Galiant S, Schargrodsky E (2007) The formation of beliefs: evidence from the allocation of land titles to squatters. Q J Econ 122(1):209–241

    Google Scholar 

  • Elster Y, Zussman A, Zussman N (2017) Rockets: the housing market effects of a credible terrorist threat. J Urban Econ 99:136–147

    Google Scholar 

  • Esarey J, Salmon T, Barrilleaux C (2012) Social insurance and income redistribution in a laboratory experiment. Polit Res Q 65(3):685–698

    Google Scholar 

  • European Social Survey Round 1 Data (2002) Data file edition 6.6. NSD - Norwegian Centre for Research Data, Norway – data archive and distributor of ESS data for ESS ERIC. https://doi.org/10.21338/NSD-ESS1-2002

  • European Social Survey Round 4 Data (2008) Data file edition 4.5. NSD - Norwegian Centre for Research Data, Norway – data archive and distributor of ESS data for ESS ERIC. https://doi.org/10.21338/NSD-ESS4-2008

  • Eugster B, Lalive R, Steinhauer A, Zweimüller J (2011) The demand for social insurance: does culture matter? Econ J 121(556):F413–F448

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisman R, Jakiela P, Kariv S (2015) How did distributional preferences change during the great recession? J Public Econ 128:84–95

    Google Scholar 

  • Gneezy A, Fessler DM (2011) Conflict, sticks and carrots: war increases prosocial punishments and rewards. Proc Biol Sci 279(1727):219–23

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein E (2007) Civilians under assault: Hezbollah’s rocket attacks on Israel in the 2006 war (Vol. 19, No. 3). Human Rights Watch

  • Grosjean P (2014) Conflict and social and political preferences: evidence from World War II and civil conflict in 35 European countries. Comp Econ Stud 56(3):424–451

    Google Scholar 

  • Gualtieri G, Nicolini M, Sabatini F (2019) Repeated shocks and preferences for redistribution. J Econ Behav Org 167:53–71

    Google Scholar 

  • Hainmueller J (2012) Entropy balancing for causal effects: a multivariate reweighting method to produce balanced samples in observational studies. Polit Anal 20(1):25–46

    Google Scholar 

  • ISSP Research Group (1995) International Social Survey Programme: environment I - ISSP 1993. GESIS Data Archive, Cologne. ZA2450 Data file Version 1.0.0. https://doi.org/10.4232/1.2450

  • ISSP Research Group (2003) International Social Survey Programme: environment II - ISSP 2000. GESIS Data Archive, Cologne. ZA3440 Data file Version 1.0.0. https://doi.org/10.4232/1.3440

  • ISSP Research Group (2019) International Social Survey Programme: environment III - ISSP 2010. GESIS Data Archive, Cologne. ZA5500 Data file Version 3.0.0. https://doi.org/10.4232/1.13271

  • Jakiela P, & Ozier O. (2019) The impact of violence on individual risk preferences: evidence from a natural experiment. Review of Economics and Statistics, forthcoming

  • Kim YI, Lee J (2014) The long-run impact of a traumatic experience on risk aversion. J Econ Behav Org 108:174–186

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuziemko I, Norton MI, Saez E, Stantcheva S (2015) How elastic are preferences for redistribution? Evidence from randomized survey experiments. Am Econ Rev 105(4):1478–1508

    Google Scholar 

  • Lupu N, Peisakhin L (2017) The legacy of political violence across generations. Am J Polit Sci 61(4):836–851

    Google Scholar 

  • Malter F, Börsch-Supan A (eds) (2015) SHARE Wave 5: innovations & methodology. MEA, Max Planck Institute for Social Law and Social Policy, Munich

    Google Scholar 

  • Nettle D, Saxe R (2020) Preferences for redistribution are sensitive to perceived luck, social homogeneity, war and scarcity. Cognition 198:104234

    Google Scholar 

  • Nikolova M, Popova O, Otrachshenko V (2022) Stalin and the origins of mistrust. Journal of Public Economics 208:104629

    Google Scholar 

  • OECD (2018) OECD Economic Surveys: Israel 2018

  • Pellicer M, Piraino P, Wegner E (2019) Perceptions of inevitability and demand for redistribution: evidence from a survey experiment. J Econ Behav Org 159:274–288

    Google Scholar 

  • Piketty T (1995) Social mobility and redistributive politics. Q J Econ 110(3):551–584

    Google Scholar 

  • Roth C, Wohlfart J (2018) Experienced inequality and preferences for redistribution. J Public Econ 167:251–262

    Google Scholar 

  • Rotter JB (1966) Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychol Monogr: Gen Appl 80(1):1

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheve K, Stasavage D (2010) The conscription of wealth: mass warfare and the demand for progressive taxation. International Organization, 529-561

  • Shai O (2021) Can conflict lead to pro-social behavior and positive psychological growth? Econ Lett 198:109684

    Google Scholar 

  • Shai O (2022) Does armed conflict increase individuals’ religiosity as a means for coping with the adverse psychological effects of wars? Soc Sci Med 296:114769

    Google Scholar 

  • Shai O (2022) Out of time? The effect of an infrequent traumatic event on individuals’ time and risk preferences, beliefs, and insurance purchasing. J Health Econ 86:102678

    Google Scholar 

  • Voors MJ, Nillesen EE, Verwimp P, Bulte EH, Lensink R, Van Soest DP (2012) Violent conflict and behavior: a field experiment in Burundi. Am Econ Rev 102(2):941–64

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang AY, Young M (2020) Terrorist attacks and investor risk preference: evidence from mutual fund flows. J Financ Econ 137(2):491–514

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Shai thanks editors Milena Nikolova and Madeline Zavodny, the three anonymous reviewers, and Todd Kaplan for their helpful comments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ori Shai.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The author declares no competing interests.

Additional information

Responsible editor: Milena Nikolova

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (PDF 496 KB)

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Shai, O. Can conflict affect individuals’ preferences for income redistribution?. J Popul Econ 36, 3071–3096 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00148-023-00963-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00148-023-00963-z

Keywords

JEL classification

Navigation