Results of cross-terms with wave dummies
Tables 2, 3, and 4 report the results of the fixed-effects model with cross-terms between Primary (Junior High) and wave dummies. The sample on which the results of Table 2 are based is those respondents equal to or below 50 years old, because we limited respondents who were possibly parents of primary school students. Furthermore, we also used the sub-sample to examine how results differ according to educational background closely. We divided the sample into a sub-sample of respondents whose schooling years were equal to or greater than 16 (i.e. graduated from university at least) and those below 16 years, respectively. The former is defined as a sub-sample of a high-educated sub-sample of high-educated persons, while the latter is defined as that of low-educated persons. Table 3 shows the results using the sub-sample of high-educated persons, and Table 4 shows the results of the sub-sample of low-educated ones. In these tables, the right and left parts indicate the results of males and females, respectively.
Table 2 Results of interaction terms Table 3 Results of interaction terms for university graduates Table 4 Results of interaction terms for high school graduates who are not university graduates Table 2 suggests that the cross-terms are not statistically significant using the male sample. However, using the female sample, Wave5 × Primary shows a negative sign and statistical significance in columns (5)–(7), meaning that female mental health improved after reopening schools than the most serious period, Wave 3. The absolute values of the coefficients of Wave5 × Primary are 0.18, 0.20, and 0.23 for Anger, Fear, and Anxiety, respectively. Compared to the third wave, mental health values improved by approximately 0.2 on the 5-point scale in the fifth wave. These results are consistent with Hypothesis 1. However, the happiness level is not influenced by any cross-terms. Interestingly, cross-terms between Junior High and wave dummies are positive and statistically significant in column (6), meaning that mothers of junior high school students were less likely to suffer Fear in the strict school closure time (Wave 3) than other periods. This is consistent with Hypothesis 2. However, these cross-terms are not statistically significant in most cases in other estimations of mental health. Therefore, Hypothesis 2 is partially supported. Concerning wave dummies, most of the results show statistical significance in all columns. Furthermore, the coefficients of Anger, Fear, and Anxiety indicate negative signs, while Happiness shows a positive sign. Considering the results of Tables 2 and 11 in the Appendix and Fig. 1 together, we argue that the time point at which the spread of COVID-19 peaked resulted in the worst mental health states.
For a robustness check, the results of the random effects of the ordered probit model are shown in the Appendix (Table 11). The findings in Table 11 in the Appendix did not change from Table 2. Hence, the results in Table 2 do not depend on the estimation method and are robust to alternative estimation.
In Table 3, most of the cross-terms did not show statistical significance. However, it is interesting to observe that Wave5 × Primary indicates a negative sign and statistically significant in estimations of Fear and Anxiety for males, but not for females. Its absolute values of coefficients are 0.20 and 0.23 for Fear and Anxiety, respectively. These values are almost the same as those of the females in Table 2. In our interpretation, highly educated fathers were more likely to bear the burden of childcare, and so their mental health was better after the reopening of schools than directly after the declaration of the state of emergency. One interpretation is that high-educated males are more likely to have progressive views and behaviours (Oswald and Powdthavee 2010). Hence, high-educated males participate in housework and childcare during the COVID-19 pandemic (Del Boca et al. 2020). Assuming that the wife of a highly educated husband is also highly educated, the mother’s burden of childcare is smaller because her husband spends his time on housework. This is reflected in the fact that the amount of mother’s childcare is not very influenced by school closure. This is in line with the argument of Suzuki et al. (2009) that husbands played an important role in reducing the stress related to child-rearing on mothers.
Turning to the results of the low-educated respondents, Table 4 shows that the results of the cross-terms are similar to those in Table 2 when the dependent variables are Anger, Fear, and Anxiety. This implies that low-educated mothers spent a larger amount of time on childcare during the school closure period. The absolute values of the coefficients of Wave5 × Primary are 0.21, 0.30, and 0.30 for Anger, Fear, and Anxiety, respectively. These values are larger than those of Table 2, arguably because the gap in the amount of childcare between the wife and the husband is larger for couples with lower education. As for the results of the estimation of Happiness, Wave1 × Primary and Wave2 × Primary show a positive sign and statistical significance, whereas Wave4 × Primary and Wave5 × Primary are not statistically significant. This shows that the mother of the child of primary school students was happier before the declaration of the state of emergency, compared with directly after the declaration. However, her happiness level after reopening school was not higher than that directly after the declaration. In our interpretation, the happiness level is unlikely to be influenced by mental health.
Results of cross-terms with closure rates
Tables 5 and 6 report the results of the fixed-effects model with cross-terms between Primary (Junior High) and school closure rates using male and female samples, respectively. Furthermore, results using low- and high-educated respondents are shown in Tables 7, 8, 9 and 10, respectively. In columns (1)–(4), the results of Specification A, where School Closure A is used. Meanwhile, in columns (5)–(8), the results of Specification B, where School Closure B is utilised.
Table 5 Results of school closure rate for men Table 6 Results of school closure rate for women Table 7 Results of school closure rate for male university graduates Table 8 Results of school closure rate for female university graduates Table 9 Results of school closure rate for male high school graduates who are not university graduates Table 10 Results of school closure rate for female high school graduates who are not university graduates Table 5 shows no statistical significance in any column when we checked the cross-terms. This implies that the child did not influence the male’s mental health during the school closure period. In contrast, Table 6 indicates the significant positive signs of School Closure A × Primary and School Closure B × Primary, with the exception of Happiness as the dependent variable. This implies that school closure had a detrimental effect on the mental health of mothers having children of primary school age. Meanwhile, the statistical insignificance of Happiness is consistent with the findings that there are differences between life satisfaction, happiness, and mental health (Clark et al. 2008). However, as a whole, the results are consistent with Hypothesis 1. There is no statistical significance for School Closure A × Junior High and School Closure B × Junior High. Hence, contrary to Table 2, Hypothesis 2 was not supported. For a robustness check, the results of the random effects of the ordered probit model are shown in the Appendix, Tables 12 and 13. The findings in Tables 12 and 13 in the Appendix are similar to those of Tables 5 and 6, showing that the results of Tables 5 and 6 are robust to alternative estimation methods.
As for the results of highly educated males, Table 7 indicates a significant positive sign of School Closure A × Primary and School Closure B × Primary in the estimation of Anxiety, although this tendency is not observed for Anger and Fear. The school closure partly deteriorates the mental health of highly educated males with children of primary school age, considering Tables 3 and 7 jointly. In Table 8, a significant positive sign is observed for School Closure B × Junior High, but not for School Closure A × Junior High when Anger is the dependent variable. Other results do not show statistical significance. As a whole, school closure did not influence mental health and happiness for highly educated mothers.
Turning to the low-educated sample, in Table 9, any statistical significance is observed for cross-terms. Hence, low-educated males having children of school students are not influenced by school closure. Switching our attention to Table 10, both School Closure A × Primary and School Closure B × Primary show a significant positive sign for the estimation of Anger, Fear, and Anxiety. The combined results of Tables 4 and 10 reveal that school closure deteriorates the mental health of females with children of primary school age. Hypothesis 1 is strongly supported. On the other hand, School Closure A × Junior High and School Closure B × Junior High are not statistically significant, which does not support Hypothesis 2.
Discussion
COVID-19 has various detrimental effects on society from various angles, and we cannot know the time when COVID-19 will be eradicated. It is important to consider health policies to cope with the COVID-19 pandemic to sustain society (Bal et al. 2020). The gender gap is larger in Japan than in other developed countries (Brinton 1993; Nemoto 2016). According to Yamamura and Tsutsui (2021b), as a consequence of school closures in Japan, female workers with children at primary school are more likely to work from home. However, this does not hold for female workers with students of junior high school and male workers with children, regardless of the type of school. Accordingly, working women with small children also work as caregivers for their children (Akerlof and Kranton 2000). In the case where the wife is a houseworker while her husband is a worker because of the division of labour within a household, the wife is likely to care for their child during school closures. Yamamura and Tsutsui (2020) found that people’s mental condition worsened with the spread of COVID-19 in Japan. Particularly, within a household, the gap in the childcare responsibility between the mother and father may have a negative influence on the mental health of the former. Under the novel setting of unexpected school closures due to the COVID-19 pandemic, the gender gap in childcare was demonstrated to increase.
Our findings are based on the assumption that mothers are more obliged to care for children. However, this assumption is inconsistent with findings in the UK that the gender childcare gap has reduced amidst the COVID-19 pandemic (Sevilla and Smith 2020).Footnote 26 The differences in findings between the UK and Japan can be interpreted in various ways. Women’s social status in Japan is far lower than that of women in other developed Western countries such as the UK (Nemoto 2016). Therefore, because of differences in bargaining power, mothers are more obliged to shoulder a larger share of childcare responsibilities (Yamamura and Tsutsui 2021a).
Due to the shortage of childcare in the market under the COVID-19 in Japan, especially during the state of emergency, mothers are more likely to spend time for childcare by themselves, regardless of their educational background. We found that mothers with low education levels suffer from a larger negative effect of COVID-19 than highly educated mothers. One possible interpretation is as follows: the low-educated mothers are less able to devise a way to enjoy spending time with small children, partly because they are less likely to gather appropriate and useful knowledge about childcare during the emergent situation when they cannot go out with their child. If so, a policy is required to support a low-educated mother to learn how to spend time with their child during their stay at home.
Under the school closures, marital relationships worsened in March 2020 but did not persist for 4 months until August 2020 (Takaku and Yokoyama 2020). Thus, this kind of negative effect of school closure is temporary if the school is reopened. However, the experience of school closure possibly dented parenting confidence. COVID-19 disturbed the rhythm of the child’s life, which increased the children’s body weight and hence the mother felt anxious about child-rearing, which persisted until August (Takaku and Yokoyama 2020). Researchers view the ‘mothers’ lack of parenting confidence…as one of the most serious problems facing families in contemporary.’ (Holloway 2010, p. 7). Policymakers should consider a care mechanism for the mental health of mothers, especially those with small children. Furthermore, by bridging health and labour issues, we should enhance fathers’ childcare responsibilities toward small children by promoting working remotely from home under the COVID-19 pandemic. From a long-term and broad perspective, it is critical to consider how to maintain work-life balance and reduce the childcare responsibility gap between husband and wife in designing the post-COVID-19 society.