Abstract
Purpose
The aim of this study is to examine the association between household energy poverty (EP) and trajectories of emotional and behavioural difficulties during childhood.
Methods
The Growing up in Ireland study is two nationally representative prospective cohorts of children. The Infant Cohort (n = 11,134) were recruited at age 9 months (9 m) and followed up at 3, 5, 7 and 9 years (y). The Child Cohort (n = 8,538) were recruited at age 9 y and followed up at 13 y and 17/18 y. EP was a composite of two relative measures of EP. Emotional and behavioural difficulties were repeatedly measured using the strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ). Linear spline multilevel models were used, adjusted for confounders to examine the association between (1) EP (9 m or 3 y) and trajectories of emotional and behavioural difficulties from 3 to 9 y in the Infant Cohort and (2) EP at 9 y and the same trajectories from 9 to 18 y in the Child Cohort.
Results
In adjusted analyses, EP at 9 m or 3 y of age was associated with higher total difficulties score at 3 y (0.66, 95% CI 0.41, 0.91) and 5 y (0.77, 95% CI 0.48, 1.05) but not at 7 y or 9 y. EP at 9 y was associated with higher total difficulties score at 9 y (1.73, 95% CI 1.28, 2.18), with this difference reducing over time leading to 0.68 (95% CI 0.19, 1.17) at 17/18 y.
Conclusions
Our study demonstrates a potential association between early life EP and emotional and behavioural difficulties that may be transient and attenuate over time during childhood. Further studies are required to replicate these findings and to better understand if these associations are causal.
Similar content being viewed by others
Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.
Introduction
Energy inequality has received greater political attention internationally in recent years [10, 35, 40]. Housing is a social determinant of health and good housing incorporates thermal comfort, secure living conditions and access to local amenities [20]. Energy inequality (‘energy insecurity’, ‘energy poverty’, ‘fuel poverty’) has increased due to higher energy costs, rising inflation and the stock of poor energy efficient accommodation. Energy poverty may result in objective effects such as lower temperatures within homes, (i.e. condensation, dampness) and in subjective effects such as perceived inability to pay for heat.
There is growing evidence of the association between energy poverty and the development of physical and mental health difficulties as a child develops [25, 30, 37]. Despite this, energy poverty has been more extensively studied in older populations and there is a need for prospective cohorts examining energy poverty in children and adolescents and health outcomes (e.g. mental health, respiratory health, cognitive wellbeing) [5, 6]. Most studies investigating energy poverty focus on outcomes at a single time point and, there is limited understanding about how associations of energy poverty and outcomes change over time as a child develops (e.g. emotional and behavioural difficulties) [30]. This approach is needed to understand if the associations are transient or persistent as a child develops which in turn would provide supportive evidence to add to the growing literature that household energy poverty requires a public health and not just an economic solution.
In addition, a recent systematic scoping review by Champagne and colleagues highlighted the need for sex-specific analyses of the association of energy poverty with outcomes and they discussed that female adults may be at a higher risk of poor health outcomes following energy poverty exposure [6]. As such, there is a need to understand sex differences in outcomes following energy poverty exposure. Moreover, they highlight that energy poverty measures in the literature are inconsistent (e.g. housing quality, low-expenditure households, parental unemployment, cold homes, vulnerable groups) [6]. These raise an important point of ensuring that studies investigating energy poverty and an outcome are adequately adjusted for confounders for both the exposure and outcome in analysis (e.g. parental depression, home ownership, parental health, income) [19, 21, 39].
Using two nationally representative prospective cohorts, we examined the association between (1) energy poverty prior to 3 years of age and trajectories of emotional and behavioural difficulties from 3 to 9 years of age, (2) energy poverty at 9 years and the same trajectories from 9 to 18 years and (3) sex interaction analysis.
Methods
Data
Data were used from the Growing up in Ireland (GUI) study, which is comprised of a separate Infant and Child Cohort [33, 34]. Using the national child benefit register as a sampling frame, the Infant Cohort were recruited in 2008 at age 9 months (n = 11,134) and followed up at 3 (n = 9793), 5 (n = 9001), 7 (n = 5344) and 9 (n = 8032) years of age. The Infant Cohort is estimated to represent 1-in-9 children born in 2008 in Ireland (n = 11,194, included in wave 1). Using Irish national school records as a sampling frame, the Child Cohort was recruited in 2008 (born in 1998) at age 9 (n = 8538) years of age and followed up at ages 13 (n = 7495), and 17/8 (n = 6186) years of age. The Child Cohort is estimated to represent 1-in-7 children at 9 years of age.
The primary care giver (PCG) and children in later waves (9 years and older) were interviewed face-to-face using computer-aided personal interviewing by trained interviewers. Quail et al., provide a detailed description of the study design, interview method, and follow-up procedure [33, 34]. All waves had similar interview procedures except wave 4 (age 7) in the Infant Cohort which was a postal questionnaire. The authors received approval from the Central Statistics Office to use the RMF Child and Infant Cohort datasets. The GUI study received ethical approval from the Research Ethics Committee within the Irish Department of Health and Children, Ireland. All participants gave informed consent to enrol in the GUI study. This study is in accordance with the ethical standards as per the Declaration of Helsinki (1964) and subsequent amendments.
Exposure
In the Infant Cohort, ‘energy poverty’ was classified as having energy poverty at 9 months or 3 years as we were interested in the effects of energy poverty in the sensitive period of early life, in particular prior to entering preschool or formal education. In the Child Cohort, the exposure ‘energy poverty’ was classified as having energy poverty at 9 years only. We used a previously reported approach of deriving relative energy poverty [24, 25]. Briefly, the primary caregiver was asked “Does the household keep the home adequately warm?” Either of the following responses “no, cannot afford” or “no, other reason” were constructed into a variable to represent “cold home”. The second question “Have you ever had to go without heating during the last 12 months through lack of money?” The response “yes” was used to construct into a variable to represent “gone without heat”. Therefore, the main exposure was ‘energy poverty’ which was a composite of either a “cold home” or “gone without heat”.
Outcomes
Strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ)
The SDQ was devised to identify current and early signs of emotional difficulties, hyperactivity, conduct behaviour, peer problems and prosocial issues in children [15]. Each question item has a three-point scale, ‘not true’, ‘somewhat true’ or ‘certainly true’ and each subscale has a total score (0–10). The use of SDQ as a continuous outcome has been validated in many jurisdictions [7, 13, 17, 22, 26]. The SDQ has strong psychometric properties and is used in many longitudinal cohorts [14, 36, 38]. We were interested in total difficulties, internalising, and externalising scores. The internalising score is the summed figure of emotional difficulties and peer problems. The externalising score is the summed score of hyperactivity and conduct behaviour scores. The total difficulties score was the total score of both the internalising and externalising scores. The internalising and externalising scores are appropriate in low-risk population samples. In the Infant Cohort, the PCG completed the SDQ for each wave, at 3, 5, 7 and 9 years of age. In the Child Cohort, the primary care giver completed the SDQ for each wave, at 9, 13 and 17/18 years of age. For repeated-measures analysis, we used continuous scores of SDQ total difficulties, internalising, and externalising scores.
Confounders
A directed acyclic graph was used to identify the potential confounders (Fig. S1, Fig. S2). These included socioeconomic characteristics (household income, house ownership and household composition), primary care giver characteristics (age and educational status (obtained a degree), and primary care giver health-related characteristics measured before exposure (has a chronic health condition and depression symptom rating). All confounders were measured prior to exposure, including chronic health condition and depression symptom rating, which were measured prior to exposure in both cohorts.
Statistical analysis
Primary analyses: repeated-measures analysis
We used linear spline multilevel modelling (2 levels: measurement occasion and individual) to examine the association between EP and trajectories of total, internalising and externalising SDQ scores in each cohort. Linear spline multilevel models estimate mean trajectories of the outcome while accounting for the non-independence (i.e. clustering) of repeated measurements within individuals and differences in the number and timing of measurements between individuals (using all available data from all eligible participants under a missing at random assumption) [27,28,29]. Linear splines allow knot points to be fit at different ages to derive periods in which change is approximately linear [28]. Linear spline periods were chosen to reflect ages in whole years that were closest to mean age at interview and hence where the density of measures was greatest. Age (in years) was centred at the first available measure (3 years for Infant Cohort and 9 years for Child Cohort). For the Child Cohort, we had two knots at 13 and 17/18 years; this produced two different linear slopes of the repeated outcome measure (i.e. SDQ): 9 to ≤ 13, and 13 to ≤ 17/18 years. For the Infant Cohort, we had three knots at 5, 7 and 9 years; this produced three different linear slopes of the repeated outcome measure (i.e. SDQ): 3 to ≤ 5, 5 to ≤ 7, and 7 to ≤ 9 years. All models included individual level random effects for the intercept and each linear spline period. For inclusion in the Infant Cohort analyses, participants required data on EP prior to 3 years with at least one measure of SDQ from 3 to 9 years and complete confounder data. For inclusion in the Child Cohort analyses, participants required data on EP at 9 years with at least one measure of SDQ from 9 to 17/18 years and complete confounder data. The growth parameters (SDQ) subsequently vary following exposure to EP. Model fit statistics compared observed values of each outcome at each age with those predicted by the models. The difference in mean or rate of change was calculated by subtracting estimates between EP and no EP (STATA syntax is available in Supplementary material 2). All data analysis was carried out using the statistical software package STATA (v.17).
Sensitivity analyses
In the Infant Cohort, we examined whether associations of EP at 9 months only, 3 years only, and 9 months and 3 years combined were similar to the results of our main analysis (EP at 9 months or 3 years). In both cohorts, we also examined whether associations of ‘cold home’ or ‘gone without heat’ separately were similar to results of our main analysis (which was a composite of these). We compared participants include to those not included in the analysis for each cohort. We examined whether our results differed by gender by performing an interaction analysis using both cohorts. We provide descriptive analysis of the outcome variable over time at each age and number of outcome measurements per participant included in analysis. Finally, we repeated the analysis examining the associations of EP between 3 and 9 years in the Infant Cohort and 9 and 17/18 years in the Child Cohort with a measure of complete outcomes in each Cohort.
Results
Summary of demographics of Infant Cohort
Of 10 170 included in the Infant Cohort analysis, 16.9% (n = 1 473) was exposed to energy poverty (Table 1). Participants that experienced EP prior to 3 years of age had higher levels of single parent households, depression scores, lower levels of household income and home ownership and educational attainment compared with participants that did not experience EP prior to 3 years of age. Gender was similar between the groups.
Summary of demographics of Child Cohort
Of 8 560 included in the Child Cohort analysis, 5.3% (n = 451) was exposed to energy poverty (Table 2). Participants who experienced EP at 9 years of age had higher levels of single parent households, depression scores, lower levels of household income, and home ownership, and educational attainment compared with participants who did not experience EP at 9 years of age. Gender was similar between the groups.
Primary analyses
Mean trajectories of total difficulties, internalising and externalising scores from 3 to 9 years by EP prior to age 3 are shown in Fig. 1. In fully adjusted models in the Infant Cohort, EP prior to 3 years was associated with a higher total difficulties scores at 3 years (0.66, 95% CI 0.41, 0.91), 5 years (0.77, 95% CI 0.48, 1.05) but not 7 years (0.26, 95% CI −0.77, 1.29) or 9 years (0.52, 95% CI −0.23, 1.27) of age (Table 3). Energy poverty prior to 3 years of age was associated with higher internalising scores at 3 years (0.24, 95% CI 0.12, 0.37) and 5 years (0.19, 95% CI 0.04, 0.33) and the results spanned the null value at 7 years (0.40, 95% CI −0.20, 1.00) and 9 years (0.41, 95% CI −0.02, 0.85) of age. Similarly, EP prior to 3 years of age was associated with higher externalising scores at 3 years (0.41, 95% CI 0.23, 0.59) and 5 years (0.58, 95% CI 0.38, 0.78) but not 7 years (−0.15, 95% CI −0.85, 0.55) or 9 years (0.09, 95% CI −0.40, 0.60) of age.
Mean trajectories of total difficulties, internalising and externalising scores from 9 years to 17/18 years by EP at 9 years are shown in Fig. 2. In fully adjusted models in the Child Cohort, EP at 9 years of age was associated with a higher total difficulties scores at 9 years (1.73, 95% CI 1.28, 2.18) with evidence of weakening associations at 13 years (0.69, 95% CI 0.05, 1.33) and also subsequently 17/18 years (0.68, 95% CI 0.19, 1.17) of age. Energy poverty at 9 years of age was associated with a higher externalising scores at 9 years (0.63, 95% CI 0.34, 0.93) and internalising scores at 9 years (1.09, 95% CI 0.83, 1.35). These patterns of association were weaker than associations for total difficulties scores but similarly indicated evidence of weakening or attenuating by 18 years of age (Table 4).
Sensitivity analyses
Associations of EP at 9 months only and trajectories were similar to main analysis. Energy poverty at 3 years only was associated with a higher total difficulties score at each age across the trajectory from 3 to 9 years of age compared with our main analysis (Table S1). The analysis of the variables ‘cold home’ and ‘gone without heat’ are available in Table S2, Table S3, and Table S4. This analysis demonstrated that each on its own did not explain the ‘energy poverty’ composite variable result. Participants included in our Infant Cohort analysis (n = 10,170) had a higher proportion of females, higher proportions of 2 parent families, higher household income and home ownership compared with participants excluded from our analyses (n = 964) (Table S5). In the Child Cohort, only (n = 8) were not included in analysed sample and we, therefore, did not have sufficient excluded numbers to examine characteristics of included versus excluded participants.
Energy poverty prior to 3 years was associated with higher total difficulties scores at 3 years (difference: 1.45, 95% CI 1.14, 1.75) and 5 years (difference: 0.66, 95% CI 0.41, 0.91) in girls compared with boys ((Table S6, Fig. S3) but these sex differences attenuated thereafter at age 7 and 9. Energy poverty at 9 years in the Child Cohort resulted in higher total difficulties scores at 9 years in girls (difference: 2.44, 95% CI 1.94, 2.93) compared to boys (difference: 1.73, 95% CI 1.29, 2.19). Contrastingly, at age 13 years, there was a higher total difficulties scores in boys (difference: 1.12, 95% CI 0.29, 1.94) compared to girls (difference: 0.60, 95% CI -0.30, 1.50), and again at age 17/18 years in boys (difference: 1.00, 95% CI 0.38, 1.62) compared to girls (difference: 0.24, 95% CI −0.43, 0.91) (Table S7, Fig. S4). Table S8 and Table S9 describe the outcome variable over time at each age and the number of outcome measurements per participant. The mean (standard deviation) number of available SDQ outcomes per participant was 2.9 (1.3) in the Infant Cohort and 2.6 (0.7) in the Child Cohort. Complete case analysis of SDQ outcome showed similar results to our main analysis for both the Child and Infant Cohorts (Table S10 and Table S11).
Discussion
In this large Irish prospective cohort study, we found that children, if exposed to energy poverty prior to 3 years of age may have a transient higher caregiver reported internalising and externalising scores at age 3, and 5 but not 7 or 9 years of age. Contrastingly, energy poverty at 9 years of age was associated with higher internalising and externalising scores at age 9, 13, and 17/18 years of age. Total difficulties scores showed similar results, energy poverty prior to 3 years was associated with higher total difficulties scores at 3 and 5 years but not at 7 or 9 years of age. Energy poverty at 9 years was associated with higher total difficulties scores at 9 years with this difference reducing over time at 17/18 years.
First, we have demonstrated an association between energy poverty exposure and childhood emotional and behavioural difficulties. This is similar to Fernandez et al., who demonstrated a higher odds of internalising and externalising behaviours in 9 year olds exposed to dual food and energy poverty in the previous 12-month period [9]. Clinically, the awareness of poverty exposures (e.g. energy poverty) is important to appreciate reported childhood emotional and behavioural difficulties.
Second, our analyses provides preliminary evidence that energy poverty exposure may result in a marginally higher total difficulties score suggesting worse emotional and behaviour difficulties. Positively, the total difficulties score does not worsen over time, nor does the rate of change in total difficulties score in those children exposed to energy poverty prior to 3 years of age. It is possible to postulate, older children exposed to energy poverty may have had a longer exposure period and be more susceptible to perceived poverty. This may explain the higher total difficulties scores at 9, 13 and 17/18 years in those exposed to energy poverty at 9 years of age. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate energy poverty exposure and trajectory of emotional and behavioural wellbeing longitudinally during early and late childhood.
Thirdly, the mechanism that explains our findings is not well understood and it could be the case that families may prioritise a ‘warm home’ if they have children despite the cost [2]. It is possible that children in energy poverty exposure may have a poorer quality of home, and as such spend a greater amount of time outdoors. According to Gold [12], poor housing may lead to higher externalising and conduct-related issues [12]. Furthermore, the mixed method “Cool? Study” in New Zealand, demonstrated that almost half the adolescents studied felt their home was not warm enough during the winter months, and that adolescents restrict their activities due to a cold home, e.g. invite friends over to their house, or do their homework [32]. Moreover, externalising behaviours are associated with poor academic performance [1, 4, 11, 23]. There is emerging evidence of gender behavioural differences in young people exposed to different poverty exposures [18]. According to our findings, we cannot conclude that the associations were sex specific. Albeit, females in comparison to males had higher total and externalising difficulties initially following exposure. However, these results warrant further studies to understand if males or females moderate the level of externalising behaviours (e.g. school refusal, truancy, conduct-related behaviours) due to energy poverty or some other unknown aspect of poverty.
This study contains limitations. First, the exposure variable ‘energy poverty’ was subjectively measured and is also a composite variable. The exposure variable is also subject to recall bias by the primary care giver, although, a similar approach has been used in other studies [31, 37] and relative or subjective measures of energy poverty are used in the European Union’s Survey of Income and Living Conditions (EU-SILC) [8, 37]. Relative binary energy poverty measures do not provide exact temperature or cost data, albeit it provides the family centred feedback on the sense of being in energy poverty [3]. Moreover, this interpretation provides information of the lived and perceived household experience of relative energy poverty. Second, the SDQ outcomes are dependent on the subjective evaluation by a caregiver. However, these questionnaires have good correlation with identifying children requiring further investigation [26, 38]. We did not use the prosocial scale of the SDQ as it has a poor correlation with the other subscales and as such, was not incorporated in the analysis. Third, families with mental health and behavioural issues may be more likely to be lost to follow-up in longitudinal studies. This may also be due to poor educational attainment, challenges pertaining to ethnic status and employment mobility. This is particularly important when attempting to capture components of poverty. Fourth, we attempted to control for all known and available confounders following the literature review. However, residual confounding may persist due to unmeasured confounding, Potential unmeasured confounders may include household heating type, accommodation size and accommodation quality (including energy efficiency rating). Fifth, energy poverty may be a component of underlying overall household poverty that influences differences in SDQ and may reflect overall socioeconomic household circumstances. Finally, the available dataset utilised is not dated in the context of recent increases in energy costs, rising inflation and ongoing coronavirus pandemic implications. Indeed, the current global energy crisis seems likely to further exacerbate existing energy inequalities internationally.
The strengths of this study includes using a large contemporary and relatively recent nationally representative cohort, the use of repeated-measures analysis to maximise potential participants, reduce loss to follow-up and selection bias. Moreover, the study outcomes were measured prospectively.
While further research is required to better understand the association between energy poverty and childhood emotional and behavioural outcomes, this study highlights the need for wider societal and public policy to understand and potentially alleviate material deprivation (including energy poverty). As Mohan [25] argued, a blended approach to poverty reform is needed, that incorporates direct short-term mechanisms of relief and long-term strategies to lift each subsequent generation from experienced and lived poverty [25]. A short-term mechanism may include energy vouchers and long-term strategies may include upgrading housing (private and social) to energy efficient standards with renewable resources to lessen the long-term risk of a household remaining in poverty or returning to poverty. Furthermore, a strategic focus on policies that target energy poverty households specifically is required and this may also require longitudinal tracking of energy poverty households and the health conditions of each household member over time to inform policy decision making and efficacy.
Health clinicians and educators should be aware of the present risk of externalising and internalising difficulties if a child is actively exposed to energy poverty. This is a collective reminder of the importance of addressing active social deprivation and taking a careful approach to addressing behavioural difficulties and inherent poverty at the same time. This research demonstrates that early life exposure to energy related deprivation has an association with difficulties in children. Although, we identified energy poverty exposure with emotional and behavioural outcomes in children, energy poverty exposure may be a proxy measure of wider socioeconomic factors within a child’s life. As such, government intervention in addressing material deprivation (including energy poverty) for young children may have later life protective effects in these children’s emotional and behavioural development, and potentially reduce future care or intervention costs.
From a research perspective, more objective and subjective metrics of energy poverty as an exposure are needed. There is a need to understand the longitudinal effect of constant and frequent transitioning into energy poverty on a child’s emotional and behavioural development and if it is a mediator or moderator of academic performance and attainment. Finally, energy poverty occurs within households and within neighbourhoods as such a multilevel approach to inform holistic poverty intervention polices (i.e. integrated public health, housing and welfare interventions) is greatly needed [16].
Conclusion
In this large prospective nationally representative cohort, we demonstrated that energy poverty exposure is associated with emotional and behavioural difficulties at several ages across early and late childhood. Further work is required to better understand these associations, specifically whether these associations are causal or whether energy poverty exposure association is being driven by wider socioeconomic deprivation. Such studies would have implications for further policy development and interventions to address energy poverty, which is an increasing problem internationally.
Data availability
The data analysed in this study may be accessed by applying for either the Anonymised Microdata File (AMF) and/or the Researcher Microdata File (RMF) through the Irish Social Science Data Archive and/or Central Statistics Office respectively.
References
Allan NP, Joye SW, Lonigan CJ (2017) Does gender moderate the relations between externalizing behavior and key emergent literacy abilities? Evidence from a longitudinal study. J Atten Disord 21:600–609. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054713512522
Ambrose A, Baker W, Sherriff G, Chambers J (2021) Cold comfort: Covid-19, lockdown and the coping strategies of fuel poor households. Energy Rep 7:5589–5596. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.egyr.2021.08.175
Bouzarovski S, Petrova S (2015) A global perspective on domestic energy deprivation: overcoming the energy poverty–fuel poverty binary. Energy Res Soc Sci 10:31–40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erss.2015.06.007
Breslau J, Miller E, Breslau N, Bohnert K, Lucia V, Schweitzer J (2009) The impact of early behavior disturbances on academic achievement in high school. Pediatrics 123:1472–1476. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2008-1406
Carrere J, Peralta A, Oliveras L, López MJ, Marí-Dell’Olmo M, Benach J, Novoa AM (2021) Energy poverty, its intensity and health in vulnerable populations in a Southern European city. Gac Sanit 35:438–444. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gaceta.2020.07.007
Champagne SN, Phimister E, Macdiarmid JI, Guntupalli AM (2023) Assessing the impact of energy and fuel poverty on health: a European scoping review. Eur J Public Health 33:764–770. https://doi.org/10.1093/eurpub/ckad108
Croft S, Stride C, Maughan B, Rowe R (2015) Validity of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire in preschool-aged children. Pediatrics 135:e1210–e1219. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2014-2920
Fahmy E, Gordon D, Patsios D (2011) Predicting fuel poverty at a small-area level in England. Energy Policy 39:4370–4377. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.enpol.2011.04.057
Fernández CR, Yomogida M, Aratani Y, Hernández D (2018) Dual food and energy hardship and associated child behavior problems. Acad Pediatr 18:889–896. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acap.2018.07.002
Fix B (2019) Energy, hierarchy and the origin of inequality. PLoS One 14:e0215692. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215692
Godinet MT, Li F, Berg T (2014) Early childhood maltreatment and trajectories of behavioral problems: exploring gender and racial differences. Child Abuse Negl 38:544–556. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2013.07.018
Gold S (2020) Is housing hardship associated with increased adolescent delinquent behaviors? Child Youth Serv Rev 116:105116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105116
Goodman A, Lamping DL, Ploubidis GB (2010) When to use broader internalising and externalising subscales instead of the hypothesised five subscales on the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ): data from British parents, teachers and children. J Abnorm Child Psychol 38:1179–1191. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-010-9434-x
Goodman R (2001) Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 40:1337–1345. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200111000-00015
Goodman R (1997) The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 38:581–586. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x
Harrington BE, Heyman B, Merleau-Ponty N, Stockton H, Ritchie N, Heyman A (2005) Keeping warm and staying well: findings from the qualitative arm of the Warm Homes Project. Health Soc Care Community 13:259–267. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2524.2005.00558.x
Heiervang E, Stormark KM, Lundervold AJ, Heimann M, Goodman R, Posserud M-B, Ullebø AK, Plessen KJ, Bjelland I, Lie SA, Gillberg C (2007) Psychiatric disorders in Norwegian 8- to 10-year-olds: an epidemiological survey of prevalence, risk factors, and service use. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 46:438–447. https://doi.org/10.1097/chi.0b013e31803062bf
Henninger WR, Luze G (2013) Moderating effects of gender on the relationship between poverty and children’s externalizing behaviors. J Child Health Care 17:72–81. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367493512447532
Hernández D (2016) Understanding “energy insecurity” and why it matters to health. Soc Sci Med 167:1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.08.029
Koeman J, Mehdipanah R (2021) Prescribing housing: a scoping review of health system efforts to address housing as a social determinant of health. Popul Health Manag 24:316–321. https://doi.org/10.1089/pop.2020.0154
Leifheit KM, Schwartz GL, Pollack CE, Black MM, Edin KJ, Althoff KN, Jennings JM (2020) Eviction in early childhood and neighborhood poverty, food security, and obesity in later childhood and adolescence: evidence from a longitudinal birth cohort. SSM Popul Health 11:100575. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssmph.2020.100575
Maurice-Stam H, Haverman L, Splinter A, van Oers HA, Schepers SA, Grootenhuis MA (2018) Dutch norms for the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ)—parent form for children aged 2–18 years. Health Qual Life Outcomes 16:123. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12955-018-0948-1
Miles SB, Stipek D (2006) Contemporaneous and longitudinal associations between social behavior and literacy achievement in a sample of low-income elementary school children. Child Dev 77:103–117. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00859.x
Mohan G (2022) The impact of household energy poverty on the mental health of parents of young children. J Public Health (Oxf) 44:121–128. https://doi.org/10.1093/pubmed/fdaa260
Mohan G (2021) Young, poor, and sick: The public health threat of energy poverty for children in Ireland. Energy Res Soc Sci 71:101822. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erss.2020.101822
Muris P, Meesters C, van den Berg F (2003) The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ)—further evidence for its reliability and validity in a community sample of Dutch children and adolescents. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 12:1–8. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-003-0298-2
O’Keeffe LM, Simpkin AJ, Tilling K, Anderson EL, Hughes AD, Lawlor DA, Fraser A, Howe LD (2018) Sex-specific trajectories of measures of cardiovascular health during childhood and adolescence: a prospective cohort study. Atherosclerosis 278:190–196. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.atherosclerosis.2018.09.030
O’Keeffe LM, Tilling K, Bell JA, Walsh PT, Lee MA, Lawlor DA, Davey Smith G, Kearney PM (2023) Sex-specific trajectories of molecular cardiometabolic traits from childhood to young adulthood. Heart 109:674–685. https://doi.org/10.1136/heartjnl-2022-321347
O’Keeffe LM, Yelverton CA, Bartels HC, O’Neill KN, McDonnell C, McAuliffe FM (2023) Application of multilevel linear spline models for analysis of growth trajectories in a cohort with repeat antenatal and postnatal measures of growth: a prospective cohort study. BMJ Open 13:e065701. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2022-065701
Oliveras L, Borrell C, González-Pijuan I, Gotsens M, López MJ, Palència L, Artazcoz L, Marí-Dell’Olmo M (2021) The association of energy poverty with health and wellbeing in children in a Mediterranean City. Int J Environ Res Public Health 18:5961. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18115961
O’Meara G (2015) A review of the literature on fuel poverty with a focus on Ireland. Soc Indic Res 128:285–303. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-015-1031-5
O’Sullivan KC, Howden-Chapman P, Sim D, Stanley J, Rowan RL, Harris Clark IK, Morrison LLA (2017) Cool? Young people investigate living in cold housing and fuel poverty. A mixed methods action research study. SSM Popul Health 3:66–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssmph.2016.12.006
Quail A, Williams J, McCrory C, Murray A, Thornton M (2011a) A Summary Guide to Wave 1 of the Infant Cohort (at 9 months of growing up in Ireland). The Economic and Social Research Institute, Office of the Minister for Children and Youth Affairs, and Trinity College Dublin, Dublin
Quail A, Williams J, McCrory C, Murray A, Thornton M (2011b) Sample design and response in Wave 1 of the Infant Cohort (at 9 months of growing up in Ireland). The Economic and Social Research Institute, Office of the Minister for Children and Youth Affairs, and Trinity College Dublin, Dublin
Sinha A, Balsalobre-Lorente D, Zafar MW, Saleem MM (2022) Analyzing global inequality in access to energy: developing policy framework by inequality decomposition. J Environ Manage 304:114299. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvman.2021.114299
Stone LL, Otten R, Engels RCME, Vermulst AA, Janssens JMAM (2010) Psychometric properties of the parent and teacher versions of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire for 4- to 12-year-olds: a review. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 13:254–274. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-010-0071-2
Thomson H, Snell C, Bouzarovski S (2017) Health, well-being and energy poverty in Europe: a comparative study of 32 European countries. Int J Environ Res Public Health 14:E584. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph14060584
Van Roy B, Veenstra M, Clench-Aas J (2008) Construct validity of the five-factor strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ) in pre-, early, and late adolescence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 49:1304–1312. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01942.x
Zhang Q, Appau S, Kodom PL (2021) Energy poverty, children’s wellbeing and the mediating role of academic performance: evidence from China. Energy Economics 97:105206. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eneco.2021.105206
Zhong H, Feng K, Sun L, Cheng L, Hubacek K (2020) Household carbon and energy inequality in Latin American and Caribbean countries. J Environ Manage 273:110979. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvman.2020.110979
Acknowledgements
Growing Up in Ireland (GUI) is funded by the Department of Children, Equality, Disability, Integration and Youth (DCEDIY). It is managed by DCEDIY in association with the Central Statistics Office (CSO). Results in this report are based on analyses of data from Research Microdata Files provided by the Central Statistics Office (CSO). Neither the CSO nor DCEDIY take any responsibility for the views expressed or the outputs generated from these analyses.
Funding
Open Access funding provided by the IReL Consortium. No direct funding was received in the preparation of this manuscript.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
David J O Driscoll: Roles: Conceptualization, formal analysis, investigation, methodology, project administration, validation, writing—original draft preparation. Elizabeth Kiely: Roles: Conceptualization, methodology, investigation, project administration, supervision, visualisation, writing—review and editing. Linda O’ Keeffe: Roles: Conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis, investigation, project administration, supervision, visualisation, writing—review and editing. Ali Khashan: Roles: Conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis, investigation, project administration, supervision, visualisation, writing—review and editing.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of interest
The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Supplementary Information
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
O Driscoll, D.J., Kiely, E., O’Keeffe, L.M. et al. Household energy poverty and trajectories of emotional and behavioural difficulties in children and adolescents: findings from two prospective cohort studies. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 59, 1299–1309 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-024-02616-2
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-024-02616-2