Introduction

In Europe, research addressing the association between immigration background and adolescent mental health problems generally supports a risk perspective, with immigrant adolescents being at a higher risk of mental health problems than their native peers [1,2,3,4]. Several studies highlight that socioeconomic status (SES) is important to consider in such research because immigrant adolescents relatively often grow up in low SES families [e.g., 5]. Intersectionality theory [6] also highlights that the association between immigration background and adolescent mental health problems cannot be fully understood without acknowledging that adolescents belong to multiple, interconnected social groups (e.g., immigration background, SES, gender) and that these social group memberships jointly shape (mental) health outcomes [7, 8]. However, studies explicitly testing the role of SES and gender in the association between immigration background and adolescent mental health problems are scarce. This is unfortunate, as an examination of the role of SES and gender may foster our understanding of the explanatory role of SES and help us identify those immigrant adolescents who are most at risk for mental health problems. This may inform intervention and prevention efforts aimed at reducing mental health disparities between immigrant and native adolescents. Therefore, this study investigated to what extent the association between immigration background and adolescent self-reported mental health problems (a) can be explained by the adolescents’ family affluence and educational level and (b) differs with the adolescents’ family affluence, educational level, or gender.

The explanatory role of family SES and adolescent educational level

Family SES and educational level may explain the association between immigration background and adolescent mental health problems. Research indicates that immigrant adolescents are less likely to grow up in affluent families [2, 9], less likely to attend higher educational tracks compared to natives [10], and both family SES and adolescent educational level are inversely related to adolescent mental health problems [11,12,13].

In Europe, studies mainly controlled for SES or examined the explanatory role of SES simultaneously with other family background variables. Studies testing the unique mediational pathways of family SES and educational level are thus rare. Moreover, those studies that focused on the explanatory role of family SES reported inconsistent results for different types of adolescent self-reported mental health problems. Overall, in Europe, family SES was found to (fully or partially) explain the association between immigration background and internalizing problems and social problems, but for externalizing problems results were inconsistent [2, 14]. For educational level even less empirical evidence is available. To our knowledge, only one study conducted in the Netherlands examined the explanatory role of adolescent educational level and found that educational level could not explain differences in mental health problems between immigrants and natives [14]. Hence, the explanatory role of family SES and educational level in the association between immigration background and different types of adolescent mental health problems remains poorly understood.

The differential impact of family SES and adolescent educational level

Family SES and educational level may also have a differential impact on the mental health problems of native and immigrant adolescents. According to the double jeopardy hypothesis [15], the effects of having an immigration background and a low SES will strengthen each other’s impact. Immigrants encounter relatively many stressors (e.g., discrimination), which may deplete their coping resources and make them more vulnerable to additional stressors that are prevalent in low SES environments than natives [16, 17]. Additionally, the combination of an immigration background and low SES might also result in a particularly unfavorable perceived social status [16]. If such perceptions of relative deprivation are seen as being unfair, they may lead to mental health problems [18]. Thus, due to the expected combined disadvantage of having an immigration background and a low SES, mental health gaps between native and immigrant adolescents may be greater for adolescents with a low than with a high SES.

In contrast, according to the diminishing returns hypothesis [19], attaining a higher SES may confer less mental health benefits to immigrant than native adolescents. Immigrant adolescents with a higher SES may perceive more discrimination than immigrant adolescents with a lower SES [20, 21] and, at the same time, they may receive less social support from other ethnic group members who may not perceive them as ‘prototypical’ ethnic group members anymore [22, 23]. Both processes might diminish the protective effect of a high SES for immigrant adolescents. From this perspective it may be inferred that mental health gaps between native and immigrant adolescents may be greater for adolescents with a high than with a low SES.

Studies examining the intersection of immigration background and SES are extremely scarce. Previous studies are almost exclusively conducted in the United States on adult samples and focused on family SES. The scarcely available literature revealed inconsistent findings for different types of adolescent self-reported mental health problems. For internalizing problems (e.g., depression), some studies found support for the double jeopardy hypothesis [17, 24], while others found support for the diminishing returns hypothesis [21, 25], or did not find a differential impact of SES for native and immigrant adolescents [26]. The latter was also found for adolescent hyperactivity–inattention problems, externalizing problems and social problems [24]. Consequently, little (family SES) to nothing (adolescent educational level) is known about whether the association between immigration background and mental health problems depends upon the adolescents’ SES.

The differential impact of gender

The association between immigration background and mental health problems may also differ for boys and girls. The intersectional invisibility hypothesis [27] argues that adolescents with multiple disadvantaged social group memberships do not fit the prototypes of the social group they belong to. To illustrate, the prototype of an immigrant is an immigrant boy and the prototype of a girl is a native girl. This may render adolescents with multiple disadvantaged social group memberships (i.e., immigrant girls), ‘invisible’ compared to their single disadvantaged peers (i.e., immigrant boys, native girls). Due to this ‘intersectional invisibility’ immigrant girls may experience distinct advantages (e.g., avoid more active forms of discrimination) and distinct disadvantages (e.g., experience invisibility discrimination) compared to their single disadvantaged peers.

In Europe, studies testing the intersection of immigration background and gender are relatively scarce and revealed inconsistent findings across different types of adolescent self-reported mental health problems. Some studies found that especially immigrant boys reported higher levels of internalizing problems [28, 29], and that immigrant girls—but not immigrant boys—reported higher levels of peer problems but lower levels of conduct problems than their native peers [30]. In contrast, other studies did not find gender differences in the association between immigration background and different types of adolescent self-reported mental health problems (e.g., internalizing problems, externalizing problems, peer relationship problems, hyperactivity–inattention problems) [24, 31, 32]. Hence, it remains unclear whether immigrant boys or girls are more vulnerable for, different types of, mental health problems than native adolescents.

This study

Given the increasingly negative socio-political climate towards immigrants in the Netherlands [33, 34], it is highly relevant to examine the association between immigration background and adolescent mental health problems in the Dutch context. Using a nationally representative sample of 11–16-year old, native Dutch and non-western immigrant adolescents, the present study examined to what extent differences in a broad spectrum of self-reported mental health problems between immigrants and natives (a) can be explained by the adolescents’ family affluence and educational level and (b) varied with the adolescents’ family affluence, educational level, and gender. Since previous studies show that immigrant adolescents relatively often grow up in less affluent families and are overrepresented in lower educational tracks, family affluence and adolescent educational level were expected to—at least partly—explain the higher risk for mental health problems in immigrant adolescents. Although the literature is inconclusive about the direction, intersectionality theory [6, 7] assumes that the association between immigration background and adolescent mental health problems differs with the adolescents’ family affluence, educational level and gender.

Methods

Participants

Data from the 2017 Dutch Health Behavior in School-aged Children (HBSC) study were used. HBSC is a cross-sectional, school-based survey that is conducted every 4 years in over 40 countries. Using a standardized research protocol, web-based, self-report questionnaires were administered to a nationally representative sample of adolescents in the classroom. Samples were drawn using cluster sampling, with schools being the primary sampling units. In the present study, adolescents attending primary education and special education were excluded.

The response rate on the school level was 37%. Main reasons for non-response were participation in other surveys (38%) or too frequent research participation requests (19%). Additional analyses indicated no selection bias concerning immigration background or the number of students in the school. Only schools in urban areas were somewhat less likely to participate than schools in rural areas, which most likely stems from them getting more frequent research participation requests. Within randomly selected classes in these schools, the average response rate was 92% on the student level. Non-response was mostly due to absence because of illness or truancy. More information about the data collection can be found elsewhere [35]. Adolescents who did not fill in any of the items on mental health problems (n = 32) or immigration background (n = 1) were excluded from the sample.

In line with previous Dutch research [24], we focused on immigrant adolescents with a non-western immigration background. Since mass immigration to the Netherlands started from the 1960s in the last century predominantly from so-called non-western countries [33], non-western immigrants constitute by far the largest group of immigrant adolescents (> 70%). Studies show that non-western immigrant adolescents are especially likely to have a low SES [36, 37], and that the socio-cultural distance between the origin and receiving country is usually larger for immigrants originating from non-western than from western countries [38]. Therefore, we did not combine data from adolescents with a western and non-western immigration background.

The resulting final sample consisted of 6337 11–16 year old native Dutch and non-western immigrant adolescents. In our sample, adolescents with a non-western immigration background (n = 1054) were mainly born in Morocco (n = 294, 28%), Turkey (n = 173, 16%), Suriname (n = 131, 12%) and the Antilles (n = 60, 6%) and belonged to the second generation (85.5%) (i.e., they were born in the Netherlands, but at least one of their parents was born abroad).

Measures

Mental health problems

Adolescents filled in the Dutch translation of the problem scales of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) [39], consisting of four subscales measuring mental health problems (conduct problems, emotional symptoms, peer relationship problems, hyperactivity–inattention problems). Each subscale includes five ordinal items with answer categories: ‘Not true’ = 0, ‘Somewhat true’ = 1; ‘Certainly true’ = 2. The present study used a revised version of the problem scales: the SDQ-R [40]. Confirmatory factor analyses showed that the SDQ-R, that excluded the five reverse worded items of the original self-report SDQ, showed a better fit to the data than the original SDQ. The SDQ-R showed a good model fit (χ2 (84) = 1168.67, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.939, TLI = 0.923, RMSEA = 0.045), while the original SDQ did not demonstrate an acceptable model fit (χ2 (164) = 2775.37, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.861, TLI = 0.839, RMSEA = 0.050).

Acceptable internal consistencies (ordinal α above 0.70) [41] were found for the conduct problems (α = 0.74), emotional symptoms (α = 0.81), and hyperactivity–inattention problems (α = 0.79) subscales of the SDQ-R. A somewhat lower internal consistency was found for the peer relationship problems subscale (α = 0.63). Invariance tests indicated that the SDQ-R allowed for valid comparisons of mental health problems between natives and non-western immigrants, the three family affluence levels, the four educational levels, and boys and girls (see Online Resource 1). The Mplus syntax of all models testing the SDQ-R can be found on https://osf.io/cmga8/. In the final sample, the occurrence of missing values on the individual SDQ-R items ranged between 1 (0.02%) and 21 (0.3%).

Demographic variables

In line with previous international studies [e.g., 2], to measure immigration background, adolescents were asked to indicate the country of birth of both their parents. Following previous Dutch studies [24] and the definition of Statistics Netherlands, adolescents were considered having a non-western immigration background if at least one of their parents was born in a non-western country [Africa, Latin America, Asia (excluding Indonesia or Japan) and Turkey]. Adolescents of whom both parents were born in the Netherlands were considered having a native Dutch background. Age was assessed based upon the adolescents’ month and year of birth and the date of the survey administration. Adolescents indicated their gender by responding to the question: ‘Are you a boy or a girl?’ with answer categories ‘boy’ and ‘girl’. Adolescents also reported their educational level. In the Netherlands, secondary education consists of four different levels: low, medium, high prevocational education and pre-university education.

The Revised Family Affluence Scale (FAS) was used as a proxy for family SES. FAS is a valid indicator of family wealth and comprises six items on material assets in the family [42]. Examples of items are: ‘Does your family own a car, van, or truck?’ and ‘Do you have your own bedroom for yourself?’. Responses were summed into a total FAS score. In line with previous studies [35], adolescent were identified as having a low (lowest 20%), middle (middle 60%), or high (highest 20%) family affluence based upon the FAS distribution in the total sample. In the final sample, data on the FAS were missing for 106 (2%) of the native Dutch and for 21 (2%) of the non-western immigrant adolescents.

Analytic strategy

We conducted structural equation modelling analyses in Mplus version 8.2 [43] using the weighted least squares mean and variance-adjusted (WLSMV) estimator. Following recent recommendations on the SDQ [44], latent variable modeling was used to measure adolescent mental health problems. Analyses were corrected for cluster effects of adolescents in the same school (the primary sampling unit) and controlled for gender and age. To assure national representativeness, data were weighted for educational level, school grade, gender, and urbanicity. Given the large sample size, a stringent significance level was applied (p  < 0.01). To examine the size of the associations between immigration background and adolescent mental health problems, standardized regression coefficients were interpreted using Cohen’s d [negligible (d < 0.20), small (d = 0.20–0.50), medium (d = 0.50–0.80), large (d > 0.80)] [45].

Based on the literature, five different models were specified to test our hypotheses. First, the direct associations between immigration background and the four latent factors representing mental health problems were examined in the total sample (a graphical representation of Model 1 is displayed in Online Resource 2). Second, a mediation model was ran to examine if family affluence and adolescent educational level can explain differences in the mental health problems of native Dutch and non-western immigrant adolescents (Model 2). Since children growing up in higher SES families tend to have higher educational attainments [46, 47], a sequential two-mediator model with family affluence and adolescent educational level as ordinal mediators was examined (Fig. 1). With the WLSMV estimator a probit link functionFootnote 1 was used to model the coefficients for the regression paths with the ordinal mediators as outcome variables. To facilitate interpretation, probit regression probabilities were calculated (see Online Resource 3). All other regression paths can be interpreted as linear regression coefficients [43]. To compute the indirect effect of immigration background on adolescent mental health problems via family affluence and/or educational level, percentile bootstrapped confidence intervals (using 5000 replications) were obtained. Partially standardized effects were used to interpret the size of indirect effects [48].

Fig. 1
figure 1

Sequential two-mediator model showing the association between immigration background and adolescent mental health problems mediated by family affluence and educational level. Continuous lines indicate significant (p  < 0.01) and dotted lines indicate non-significant (p  > 0.01) regression coefficients. Non-bold numbers represent unstandardized and bold numbers represent standardized regression coefficients (using STD standardization, except for the probit regressions using STDY standardization). The rectangles represent observed variables and the ovals represent latent variables. To increase readability, latent factor indicators, (co)variances, and residual error terms were not modelled

Finally, three multigroup models (see Online Resource 2 for a graphical representation of the model tested in each subgroup) were tested to examine family affluence (Model 3), educational level (Model 4), and gender (Model 5) differences in the association between immigration background and adolescent mental health problems. The Wald test was used to test whether the associations between immigration background and adolescent mental health problems differed significantly between the three family affluence levels, four educational levels, and boys and girls. The Mplus syntax of all the five models can be found on https://osf.io/cmga8/.

Results

Differences between the native Dutch and non-western immigrant adolescents

Table 1 shows the family affluence, educational level, and gender distributions for the native Dutch and non-western immigrant adolescents. There were no significant differences in the age (F (3.61, 303.15) = 0.45, p = 0.75), gender (F (1,84) = 5.97, p  = 0.02), and educational level (F (2.05, 171.75) = 1.48, p  = 0.23) of both groups. Significant family affluence differences were found (F (1.62,135.93) = 48.87, p < 0.001), with non-western immigrant adolescents reporting a lower family affluence than native Dutch adolescents.

Table 1 Demographic statistics of the native Dutch and non-western immigrant adolescents

Since measurement invariance was established (see Online Resource 1), latent means could be validly compared between the native Dutch and non-western immigrant adolescents. Table 2 shows that non-western immigrant adolescents reported more conduct problems and peer relationship problems, less hyperactivity–inattention problems and equal levels of emotional symptoms compared to native Dutch adolescents.

Table 2 Immigration background and adolescent self-reported mental health problems

The explanatory role of family affluence and adolescent educational level

The sequential two-mediator model (Fig. 1) showed an acceptable fit to the data (χ2 (143) = 1391.61, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.929, TLI = 0.907, RMSEA = 0.037) and explained a significant percentage of the variance in conduct problems (R2 = 0.12, p  < 0.001), emotional symptoms (R2 = 0.17, p  < 0.001), peer relationship problems (R2 = 0.11, p  < 0.001), and hyperactivity–inattention problems (R2 = 0.05, p  < 0.001).

Significant indirect effects were found for immigration background on emotional symptoms (B = 0.04, β = 0.06, 99% CI [0.02, 0.07]), peer relationship problems (B  = 0.05, β = 0.08, 99% CI [0.02, 0.08]), and hyperactivity–inattention problems (B  = − 0.05, β = − 0.04, 99% CI [− 0.09, − 0.01]) through family affluence. No indirect effect was found for immigration background on conduct problems (B  = − 0.00, β = − 0.00, 99% CI [− 0.04, 0.03]) via family affluence. Being a non-western immigrant (compared to native Dutch) resulted in an increase in emotional symptoms, an increase in peer relationship problems, but a decrease in hyperactivity–inattention problems through changes in family affluence. Non-western immigrant adolescents were less likely to report a high family affluence than native Dutch adolescents (Online Resource 3). Adolescents in more affluent families reported somewhat lower levels of emotional symptoms and peer relationship problems but somewhat higher levels of hyperactivity–inattention problems (Fig. 1).

No significant indirect effects were found for immigration background on conduct problems (B = 0.03, β = 0.02, 99% CI [− 0.08, 0.14]), emotional symptoms (B = 0.00, β = 0.00, 99% CI [− 0.01, 0.01]), peer relationship problems (B = 0.01, β = 0.02, 99% CI [− 0.04, 0.06]), and hyperactivity–inattention problems (B = 0.02, β = 0.02, 99% CI [− 0.07, 0.10]) through adolescent educational level. Results did however indicate a significant indirect effect of immigration background on conduct problems (B = 0.04, β = 0.04, 99% CI [0.02, 0.07]), peer relationship problems (B = 0.02, β = 0.03, 99% CI [0.01, 0.03]), and hyperactivity–inattention problems (B = 0.03, β = 0.03, 99% CI [0.02, 0.05]) via family affluence and adolescent educational level. No indirect effect was found for immigration background on emotional symptoms (B = 0.00, β = 0.00, 99% CI [− 0.01, 0.01]) via family affluence and adolescent educational level. Being an non-western immigrant (compared to native Dutch) resulted in an increase in conduct problems, peer relationship problems, and hyperactivity–inattention problems through changes in the adolescents’ family affluence and educational level. Compared to native Dutch adolescents, non-western immigrant adolescents were less likely to report high levels of family affluence. Native Dutch and non-western immigrant adolescents in highly affluent families were more likely to attend higher educational levels (Online Resource 3). Adolescents attending higher educational levels reported lower levels of conduct problems, peer relationship problems, and hyperactivity–inattention problems (Fig. 1).

All indirect effects were fairly small in size. Furthermore, comparing the model without family affluence and educational level (Table 2) with the mediation model including family affluence and educational level (Fig. 1), non-western immigrants still reported more conduct problems and peer relationship problems, less hyperactivity–inattention problems and equally high levels of emotional symptoms as their native Dutch peers.

The differential impact of family affluence and adolescent educational level

The association between immigration background and conduct problems (Wald χ2 (2) = 11.14, p = 0.004) and hyperactivity–inattention problems (Wald χ2 (2) = 12.31, p = 0.002) differed across family affluence levels. In contrast, the associations between immigration background and emotional symptoms (Wald χ2 (2) = 2.37, p = 0.31) and peer relationship problems (Wald χ2 (2) = 3.06, p = 0.22) were equal across family affluence levels. In families with relatively low levels of affluence, non-western immigrant adolescents reported equal levels of conduct problems (B = 0.10, β = 0.13, p = 0.11, 99% CI [− 0.06, 0.26]) as native Dutch adolescents, whereas in families with middle (B = 0.37, β = 0.45, p < 0.001, 99% CI [0.21, 0.52]) and high (B = 0.36, β = 0.44, p < 0.001, 99% CI [0.10, 0.61]) levels of affluence, non-western immigrant adolescents reported higher levels of conduct problems than native Dutch adolescents. Family affluence did not impact the level of conduct problems of non-western immigrant adolescents, while middle or high levels of family affluence decreased the conduct problems for native Dutch adolescents compared to a low family affluence (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Differences in conduct problems between native Dutch and non-western immigrant adolescent in low, middle and high affluent families

In addition, in families with low (B = − 0.29, β = − 0.40, p < 0.001, 99% CI [− 0.46, − 0.13]) and middle (B = − 0.22, β = − 0.30, p < 0.001, 99% CI [− 0.35, − 0.09]) levels of affluence non-western immigrant adolescents reported lower levels of hyperactivity–inattention problems than native Dutch adolescents, whereas in highly affluent families (B = 0.05, β = 0.07, p = 0.53, 99% CI [− 0.15, 0.25]) equal levels of hyperactivity–inattention problems were reported by both groups. Figure 3 shows that family affluence was not related to the hyperactivity–inattention problems in native Dutch adolescents, while for non-western immigrant adolescents, high levels of family affluence increased their hyperactivity–inattention problems compared to low and middle levels of family affluence.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Differences in hyperactivity–inattention problems between native Dutch and non-western immigrant adolescent in low, middle and high affluent families

No educational level differences were found in the association between immigration background and adolescent mental health problems. The association between immigration background and conduct problems (Wald χ2 (3) = 4.52, p = 0.21), emotional symptoms (Wald χ2 (3) = 0.44, p = 0.93), peer relationship problems (Wald χ2 (3) = 7.47, p = 0.06), and hyperactivity–inattention problems (Wald χ2 (3) = 7.07, p = 0.07) was similar across all four educational levels.

The differential impact of gender

The association between immigration background and adolescent mental health problems did also not vary with the gender of the adolescent. Associations between immigration background and conduct problems (Wald χ2 (1) = 2.48, p = 0.12), emotional symptoms (Wald χ2 (1) = 1.70, p = 0.19), peer relationship problems (Wald χ2 (1) = 4.20, p = 0.04), and hyperactivity–inattention problems (Wald χ2 (1) = 4.30, p = 0.04) were equal for boys and girls.

Discussion

It has been theorized—but rarely explicitly examined—that SES and gender play an important role in the association between immigration background and adolescent mental health problems. Interestingly, the result of this study illustrate that the association between immigration background and adolescent mental health problems is largely independent of SES and gender. First, although non-western immigrant adolescents were less likely to grow up in highly affluent families, this explained only a very small proportion of their higher risk for conduct problems and peer relationship problems compared to the native Dutch adolescents. Second, these differences in mental health problems were quite robust across family affluence levels, educational levels and boys and girls. Only two interactions were revealed: for native Dutch adolescents higher levels of family affluence were associated with less conduct problems, while there was no association between family affluence and conduct problems for non-western immigrants. No association between family affluence and hyperactivity–inattention problems was revealed for native Dutch adolescents, while for non-western immigrants a high level of family affluence was associated with more hyperactivity–inattention problems.

Studies testing the unique mediational pathways of family SES and educational level in the association between immigration background and adolescent mental health problems are rare. Using advanced statistical methods, we were able to model the association between family affluence and adolescent educational level [e.g., 46] and to test the explanatory role of family affluence and adolescent educational level alone and together. Our finding that immigration background had an indirect effect on adolescent internalizing problems (i.e., emotional symptoms) and social problems (i.e., peer relationship problems), but not on externalizing problems (i.e., conduct problems) via family affluence is largely in line with previous studies [2, 14]. However, the size of these indirect effects was very small, which may be due to the weak association between family affluence and adolescent mental health problems. In a high income country like the Netherlands, low levels of family affluence may still reflect relatively high material living standards. Therefore, adolescents may have few material desires [49], which may diminish the impact of family affluence on adolescent mental health problems. Alternatively, the Dutch universal health care system that provides health care allowances for low income families may explain the weak associations found between family affluence and adolescent mental health problems.

In line with former results [14], adolescent educational level did not explain differences in mental health problems between non-western immigrant and native Dutch adolescents either. This is not surprising as there were no educational level differences between non-western immigrant and native Dutch adolescents. Although, via family affluence, adolescent educational level had some explanatory power, again the size of these indirect effects was fairly small. Thus, in the Netherlands, other factors than SES may explain the higher risk for conduct problems and peer relationship problems of non-western immigrant adolescents. Given increasingly negative attitudes towards immigrants in Europe [50], and evidence of an increasingly negative socio-political climate towards immigrants in the Netherlands [33, 34], perceptions of discrimination may be a relevant factor.

Although intersectionality theory [6] clearly emphasizes the need to study the intersections of adolescents’ social group memberships (e.g., immigration background, SES, gender), studies examining the role of SES and gender in the link between immigration background and adolescent mental health problems are scarce, especially in Europe [51]. Contributing to the scarce and inconsistent literature [e.g., 17, 21, 24, 25], our results indicated that differences in mental health problems between non-western immigrant and native Dutch adolescents were largely comparable for different family affluence and educational levels. This was also the case for gender differences, which is in line with previous Dutch studies [24, 31]. Two (out of a possible twelve) interactions between immigration background and family affluence were found, however.

Supporting the diminishing returns hypothesis, only for native Dutch adolescents higher levels of family affluence were associated with less conduct problems. Immigrants with a higher SES may be more aware of discrimination [52] and such perceptions of discrimination may diminish the protective effect of a high family SES for immigrant adolescents [21]. However, this effect was only found for conduct problems and not robust over the whole spectrum of adolescent mental health problems.

For hyperactivity–inattention problems findings at first seemed counterintuitive. Non-western immigrants reported lower levels of hyperactivity–inattention problems than their native peers and high levels of family affluence increased the hyperactivity–inattention problems of non-western immigrant adolescents only. Studies show that higher SES immigrants have more contacts with natives than lower SES immigrants [52, 53] and that Dutch adolescents report relatively high levels of hyperactivity–inattention problems compared to their European agemates [40]. Due to influence processes [54], immigrant adolescents in higher SES families—who are assumed to have more contacts with native peers—may become more similar in their behavior and/or attitudes towards hyperactive and inattentive behavior to native peers and report higher levels of hyperactivity–inattention problems.

Using a nationally representative sample of Dutch adolescents, two indicators of SES (family affluence and adolescent educational level), a broad spectrum of adolescent mental health problems, and a rigorous methodological design, this study showed that the role of SES and gender in the association between immigration background and adolescent mental health problems is at best small. Still, some limitations and alternative explanations of our findings should be considered. First, the cross-sectional nature of our data does not allow for causal inferences about the explanatory role of SES. Our findings suggest that higher SES environments may protect against mental health problems. However, the opposite may also be true; mental health problems may negatively impact adolescents’ educational careers [47]. Second, although by establishing measurement invariance we made sure that the native Dutch and non-western immigrant adolescents did not systematically differ in their responses on the mental health problems measures, we cannot rule out that cultural differences in socialization practices may have contributed to differences in the mental health problems of the non-western immigrant and native Dutch adolescents [55].

Third, the HBSC study is designed to gather a nationally representative sample of Dutch adolescents. This is a clear advantage as our findings are generalizable to the overall population of Dutch adolescents. However, our sample design did not allow for analyses stratified by specific immigrant group characteristics (i.e., country of origin, generational status, reasons for immigration). It remains to be seen whether this has sizeable consequences for our results, as previous Dutch studies that distinguished between immigrants with specific ethnic backgrounds found highly similar mental health differences in the different immigrant groups [56,57,58]. Also, in Europe, evidence for an immigration paradox (suggesting that later generations immigrants are doing worse than earlier generations) is much less convincing than in the United States [2, 59,60,61,62]. Notwithstanding these former findings, future studies testing the generalizability of our results in a broad scale of specific immigrant groups from different countries of origin (including western countries), different generational statuses, and with different reasons for immigration (e.g., labor, refugees) are highly relevant. Fourth, our questionnaire was only available in the Dutch language. For a small group of immigrant adolescents, especially those from the first generation who recently immigrated to the Netherlands, a language barrier may have impacted their self-reports. Finally, in this study we only used one measure of family SES; family affluence. Studies show that the association between family SES and adolescent mental health problems may vary with the operationalization of family SES [11]. However, given that especially for younger adolescents it is difficult to report on their parents’ occupation and educational level or family income, adolescents’ reports of family affluence provide a particularly reliable measure of family SES [63].

Conclusion

With non-western immigrant adolescents only reporting higher levels of conduct problems and peer relationship problems, our study does not unequivocally support the risk perspective of immigration [1,2,3]. The risk perspective of immigration may thus vary with the type of mental health problems. Family affluence and adolescent educational level could not explain the higher risk of conduct problems and peer relationship problems of non-western immigrant adolescents compared to native Dutch adolescents. Even more, these differences in mental health problems were quite robust across different family affluence levels, adolescent educational levels, and between boys and girls. Thus, although it is theorized that SES and gender play an important role in the association between immigration background and adolescent mental health problems, our findings indicate that in the Netherlands the role of SES and gender is limited.

Taken together, our results imply that not only immigrant adolescents from low SES backgrounds, but immigrants (both boys and girls) from a wide range of SES backgrounds may need prevention and intervention programs targeting their mental health, specifically concerning conduct problems and peer relationship problems. Future studies should test whether other factors—such as perceptions of discrimination—may account for differences in the mental health of immigrant and native adolescents. Moreover, internationally comparative research examining the intersection of immigration background with SES and gender is needed to examine the generalizability of our findings outside the Netherlands and to map mental health inequalities between immigrant and native adolescents with more precision.