Introduction

There is now a substantial body of literature indicating that children with type 1 diabetes are at increased risk of cognitive and behavioural difficulties [13]. Both intercurrent fluctuations in blood glucose level (BGL) [4, 5] and longer-term metabolic control [6] have been shown to affect cognition and behaviour. The cognitive skills that appear most affected by fluctuating BGLs include attention, working memory, complex information processing and cognitive flexibility [2, 7, 8].

Data supporting the benefits of insulin delivery via continuous subcutaneous insulin infusion (CSII) in children with type 1 diabetes are increasing, with studies showing improved metabolic control, lower rates of hypoglycaemia and improved quality of life [913]. Anecdotally, families and teachers frequently report improvements in cognition, mood and behaviour after the commencement of insulin pump therapy, although to date there have been no published attempts to study this systematically.

The purpose of this uncontrolled pilot study was to investigate whether improved metabolic control associated with commencing CSII is associated with measurable improvements in cognition, mood and behaviour.

Methods

Study design

Children with pre-existing type 1 diabetes attending The Children’s Hospital Westmead, Sydney (n = 15) or The Royal Children’s Hospital, Melbourne (n = 17), aged 6–16 years, were recruited as they were started on CSII. This age range corresponded to the validated age range for the selected behavioural and cognitive tests employed. The children commenced CSII for a range of reasons including unstable glycaemia, a desire to improve control, lifestyle flexibility and personal or physician therapeutic preference. The decision to commence CSII was independent of any involvement in the study; there were no other specific eligibility criteria. Ethical approval was obtained from the Human Ethics Research Committees at both participating hospitals and informed consent was obtained from participants and parents. Psychometric and glycaemic assessments were conducted in the week before CSII was commenced and 6–8 weeks after CSII commencement.

Socioeconomic status (SES) was assessed based on the principal income earner of the family, and quantified using the Australian and New Zealand Standard Classification of Occupations [14], which provides a 1–8 rating scale (high SES 1–2.5, medium SES 2.6–5.4, low SES 5.5–8).

The Behaviour Assessment System for Children—Second Edition (BASC-2)

This standardised instrument was used to assess mood and behaviour using parent and teacher report forms. Self-report forms were also completed by children aged 9 years and over. Analyses of BASC-2 data were carried out using BASC-2-enhanced ASSIST, which generates summary T scores (mean of 50 and SD of 10) standardised for age and sex for externalising and internalising behaviour. The externalising scale measures behaviour and includes symptoms of hyperactivity, aggression and conduct problems, while the internalising scale measures mood and comprises anxiety, depression and somatisation scores. High scores indicate a greater tendency to psychopathology.

Cognitive test battery

The instruments used in this study are described in Table 1. Test selection was guided by previous research indicating that complex information processing tasks, vigilance, efficient decision making, processing speed and working memory are the skills most sensitive to intercurrent or recent blood glucose variations and to longer-term diabetes-related effects [1, 2, 7, 8]. The selected neuropsychological tests are accepted as valid measures of the variables of interest [15, 16] and are of sufficient task difficulty to be sensitive to subtle changes in cognitive functioning. In order to minimise practise effects, the follow-up assessment protocol included alternative forms where possible, as shown in Table 1. Some test–retest data are available from the testing manuals [17, 18] and are compared with our results.

Table 1 Cognitive test battery

Cognitive assessment was performed only if the child had a BGL between 4 and 18 mmol/l, based on limited available data suggesting that acute effects on cognition are unlikely inside this range [1921].

Glycaemic assessment

A continuous glucose monitoring system (CGMS) (Medtronic CGMS System Gold; Medtronic, Northridge, CA, USA) was used by participants for a 72 h period in the 7 days prior to CSII commencement and at a time-point 6–8 h afterwards. Data were summarised using CGMS Gold software and an algorithm previously reported by this group [22], which examines a number of variables of BGL variation including mean of daily differences and continuous overlapping net glycaemic action (CONGA). CONGA is a measure of intra-day glycaemic variation over specified time periods of 1, 2 or 4 h with reported mean values of CONGA1, CONGA2 and CONGA4 being 0.7, 0.8 and 1.0, respectively, in non-diabetic individuals and 2.5, 3.6 and 4.6 in randomly selected diabetic patients [22]. The percentage of time spent in hypoglycaemic (CGMS <4 mmol/l), normoglycaemic (4–12 mmol/l) and hyperglycaemic (>12 mmol/l) glucose ranges was calculated for each participant.

HbA1c was measured by capillary blood sampling at the commencement of CSII and at the second assessment 6–8 weeks later. Measurement was by HPLC (Variant; Bio-Rad Laboratories, Hercules, USA, USA) with a non-diabetic range of 4.6–6.5%.

Statistical analyses

Paired-sample t tests or Wilcoxon signed-ranks test (for non-normal data) were used to analyse change in variables at baseline and follow-up. Relationships between variables were explored by regression analysis. For cognitive and behavioural variables, standardised scores are shown where possible; otherwise raw scores. In order to indicate size of change, z scores are shown for each test measure.

Results

Thirty-two children (57% male) participated in the study with mean age 12.9 years (range 6.2–15.9 years). Mean duration of diabetes was 3.25 years (0.24–12.7 years). The median SES score of 3 was in the higher-status end of the medium range with 19% low SES, 31% medium SES and 50% high SES.

Cognition

As shown in Table 2, a significantly better performance was observed after the commencement of CSII on the following tasks: perceptual reasoning, selective attention, divided attention, cognitive flexibility (trials 2, 3 and 4) and working memory. The change we found in perceptual reasoning score of +1.13 was approximately double the test–retest effect of 0.6 reported in the test manual [17] and similarly the change we found in working memory of +0.94 was approximately double the test–retest change of 0.4 reported [18]. No changes in performance were observed for vocabulary, immediate and sustained attention, planning and organisation, new learning or letter fluency. Performance on a task of semantic fluency was significantly poorer at follow-up.

Table 2 Cognitive variables at baseline and follow-up

Mood and behaviour

Data are summarised in Table 3. Children demonstrated fewer mood-related symptoms (internalising problems), based on parent (p < 0.001), teacher (p < 0.005) and self (p < 0.005) reports after commencement of CSII. Parent reports indicated fewer behavioural problems (externalising problems) following commencement of CSII (p < 0.001), while self-reports of change in behaviour approached significance (p = 0.06). There was no significant change based on teacher reports.

Table 3 Comparison of mood and behaviour before and after commencement of CSII: parent, teacher and self-reports

Glycaemia

There was a significant improvement in HbA1c from a mean of 8.23% to 7.48% (p < 0.01) after the commencement of insulin pump therapy (Table 4). Time spent in the normoglycaemic range increased by 24% (p < 0.01) and time spent in the hyperglycaemic range decreased by 30% (p < 0.05). There was no significant change in time spent in the hypoglycaemic range. Glycaemic variability was reduced after commencement of CSII (Table 4) as measured by CONGA1 (p < 0.05) and CONGA4 (p < 0.05) and a trend to reduction in CONGA2 (p = 0.07) [22].

Table 4 Glycaemic variables at baseline and follow-up

Regression analyses did not demonstrate a direct relationship between improvement in HbA1c or other glycaemic variables and improvement in measures of mood, behaviour or cognition.

Discussion

In this preliminary study of children commencing CSII, improvements in glycaemic control, cognition, mood and behaviour were observed over the first 6–8 weeks. These preliminary findings support anecdotal reports from families after commencing CSII therapy and limited information from the literature about the association between BGLs and cognition, mood and behaviour. We are only aware of one other small observational study in children commencing CSII therapy. Daley et al. [23] reported improved classroom concentration in four boys in on-task (20% improvement) and off-task (34% improvement) behaviour after insulin pump commencement, which was associated with improved stability of BGLs.

We observed, as expected, an improvement in HbA1c after CSII commencement, although the period of 6–8 weeks would not allow the changes to be fully reflected by HbA1c. Blood glucose control was improved, as evidenced by greater time spent in the normoglycaemic range and less time in the hyperglycaemic range at follow-up as measured by CGMS. Blood glucose variability was also reduced as measured by a reduction in CONGA values calculated from CGMS. However, we were unable to demonstrate a direct relationship between improvement in glycaemic variables and improvement in behaviour and cognitive variables. We recognise that these findings are limited by having only performed 72 h of CGMS monitoring at each time-point and these may not accurately reflect longer periods. There was no difference in mean BGL at the time of cognitive testing across the two assessments, suggesting that any differences in performance were not attributable to variation in intercurrent BGLs.

While CSII patients were the group studied here, it may also be possible that similar effects could be found in groups of patients having intensification of therapy and significantly improving their control by changes in injection regimen. However, emerging data suggest advantages of CSII over injection regimens in terms of improving overall control and reducing BGL variability. It is also possible that improvements in mood and cognition could have been related to a number of other factors associated with starting an insulin pump, such as optimism and improved perception associated with a new technology, renewed motivation or a positive psychological response to increased diabetes input. While these factors are difficult to isolate, the advantages of improved confidence and perception should not be discounted, although longer term studies would be necessary to see if they are sustained.

Our findings relating to cognition suggest that after the commencement of insulin pump therapy children do not demonstrate improvement for tasks drawing on simple, information processing skills; however, moderate improvement is observed on tasks that are more complex and cognitively demanding and require more active mental processing, such as those involving cognitive flexibility and divided attention. These findings are consistent with existing neuropsychological literature, which suggests that, following subtle changes in brain function, deficits are less likely on tasks requiring simple, automatic responses and more likely on complex tasks requiring higher order cognitive skills [24]. We recognise that this is an uncontrolled pilot study; however, the cognitive measures used are not subject to significant practise effects according to available data [17, 18].

There are some published data demonstrating adverse effects of hypoglycaemia or hyperglycaemia on mental functioning, although the degree of blood glucose fluctuation required to affect these processes adversely is understood poorly. Deficits in cognitive functioning associated with hypoglycaemia have been reported to persist beyond the time of resolution of hypoglycaemia [25, 26] and one speculation is that hypoglycaemia may contribute to alterations in cerebral blood flow away from the dominant hemisphere [27]. It has also been postulated that recurrent episodes of hypoglycaemia may have a cumulative, detrimental effect on cognitive functioning [7, 8, 28] and poorer attention and organisational skills have been reported in those children whose diabetes began at a younger age [7, 29-31]. Regarding hyperglycaemia and cognitive functioning, Davis et al. showed that eight of 12 children with type 1 diabetes had a decrease in performance IQ (mean deterioration 6 IQ points) when hyperglycaemic (20–30 mmol/l) [19].

The improvements we observed in mood with commencement of CSII were consistent across parent, teacher and self-reports. In contrast, only parent reports indicated a significant improvement in behaviour, and this could relate to positive expectations as discussed above. However, these data are consistent with recent findings by McDonnell and colleagues [5], who found that more time spent in the hyperglycaemic range was associated with more externalising behaviours, which are more likely to be recognised by parents at home. Nocturnal hypoglycaemia has been shown to adversely affect mood in children the following day [28] and higher rates of mood variability have been documented in children with type 1 diabetes compared with sibling and classmate controls [32].

Our data suggest an association between improved glycaemic control after CSII commencement and improvements in more complex cognitive tasks, mood and behaviour. While we postulate that this might be caused by a reduction in blood glucose variability we were unable to demonstrate this relationship directly. Clearly, there may also be other positive factors associated with starting CSII that were not measured in this study. Nevertheless, these data give support to anecdotal parental and teacher observations that children’s mood, behaviour and cognition improve following the commencement of insulin pump therapy. We hope that further study in a randomised controlled trial and longer follow-up will clarify these issues.