Skip to main content
Log in

Tactual Equivalence Class Formation and Tactual-to-Visual Cross-Modal Transfer

  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Five adults, two who were deaf-blind and three who were hearing-sighted but blindfolded for the experiment, learned the conditional discriminations AB and BC that consisted of complex tactual stimuli. Class-consistent responding to symmetry, transitivity, and equivalence probes demonstrated the emergence of tactual equivalence classes by four of the subjects. These data confirm that visual or auditory stimuli are not needed for the induction of equivalence classes. They also suggest that equivalence class procedures might facilitate the formation of communicational skills by individuals who are deaf and blind. Following the formation of the tactual equivalence classes by the two hearing-sighted adults, they immediately showed tactual-to-visual cross-modal generalization of equivalence relations. These results suggest that prior equivalence class formation facilitate the reliable emergence of cross-modal transfer.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • ADAMS, B. J., FIELDS, L., & VERHAVE, T. (1993). Effects of the test order on the establishment and expansion of equivalence classes. The Psychological Record, 43, 133–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ADAMS, B. J., FIELDS, L., & VERHAVE, T. (in press). Effects of unreinforced conditional selection training, multiple negative comparison training, and feedback on equivalence class formation. The Psychological Record.

  • ANNETT, J. M., & LESLIE, J. C. (1995). Stimulus equivalence classes involving olfactory stimuli. The Psychological Record, 45, 439–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • BARNES, D., BROWNE, M., SMEETS, P., & ROCHE, B. (1995). A transfer of functions and a conditional transfer of functions through equivalence relations in three- to six-year-old children. The Psychological Record, 45, 405–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • BARNES, D., McCULLAGH, P. D., & KEENAN, M. (1990). Equivalence class formation in non-hearing impaired children and hearing impaired children. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 8, 19–30.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • BOELENS, H. (1994). A traditional account of equivalence. The Psychological Record, 44, 587–605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • BUFFINGTON, D. M., FIELDS, L., & ADAMS, B. J. (1997). Enhancing equivalence class formation by pretraining of other equivalence classes. The Psychological Record, 47, 69–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • BUSH, K. M. (1993). Stimulus equivalence and cross-modal transfer. The Psychological Record, 43, 567–584.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • BUSH, K. M., SIDMAN, M., & De ROSE, T. (1989). Contextual control of emergent equivalence relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 29–45

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • DeGRANDPRE, R. J., BICKEL, W. K., & HIGGINS, S. T. (1992). Emergent equivalence relations between interoceptive (drug) and exteroceptive (visual) stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 58, 9–18.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • DUBE, W. V., GREEN, G., & SERNA, R. W. (1993). Auditory successive conditional discrimination and auditory stimulus equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 59, 103–114.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • FIELDS, L., ADAMS, B. J., VERHAVE, T., & NEWMAN, S. (1990). The effects of nodality on the formation of equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 53, 345–358.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • FIELDS, L., NEWMAN, S., ADAMS, B. J., & VERHAVE, T. (1992). The expansion of equivalence classes through simple discrimination training and fading. The Psychological Record, 42, 3–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • FIELDS, L., & REEVE, K. F. (in press). Equivalence classes and natural categories from the generalization of perceptual and relational classes. In J. C. Leslie & D. Blackman (Eds.), Issues in Experimental and Applied Analyses of Human behavior. Reno, NV: Context Press.

  • FIELDS, L., REEVE, K. F., ADAMS, B. J., & VERHAVE, T. (1991). Stimulus generalization and equivalence classes: a model for natural categories. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 55, 305–312.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • FIELDS, L., REEVE, K., ADAMS, B. J., BROWN, J. L., & VERHAVE, T. (1997). Predicting the extension of equivalence classes from the primary generalization gradients: The merger of equivalence classes and perceptual classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 68, 67–92.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • FIELDS, L., & VERHAVE, T. (1987). The structure of equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 48, 317–332.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • GESCHWIND, N. (1965). Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man: Part 1. Brain, 88, 237–293.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • GODFREY, N. W., & COSTELLO, M. A. (1995). Communication issues and strategies for deaf-blind individuals. American Rehabilitation, 21, 40–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • GOODALL, D., & EVERSON, J. (1995). Communication instruction and support strategies for young adults who are deaf-blind. In J. Everson (Ed.), Supporting young adults who are deaf-blind in their communities (pp. 203–226). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • HAYES, L. J., TILLEY, K. J., & HAYES, S. C. (1988). Extending equivalence class membership to gustatory stimuli. The Psychological Record, 38, 473–482.

    Google Scholar 

  • HAYES, S. C. (1991). A relational control theory of stimulus equivalence. In L. J. Hayes & P. N. Chase (Eds.), Dialogues on verbal behavior (pp. 19–40). Reno, NV: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • HAYES, S. C., & HAYES, L. J. (1989). The verbal action of the listener as a basis for rule-governance. In S. C. Hayes (Ed.), Rule-governed behavior: Cognition, contingencies, and instructional control. New York: Plenum Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • HAYES, S. C., KOHLENBERG, B. S., & HAYES, L. J. (1991). The transfer of specific and general consequential functions through simple and conditional equivalence relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 119–137.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • HOLLIS, J. H., FULTON, R. T., & LARSON, A. D. (1986). An equivalence model for vocabulary acquisition in profoundly hearing impaired children. Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 6, 331–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • KAZDIN, A. E. (1982). Single-case research designs. Oxford University Press: New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • KREKLING, S., TELLEVIK, J. M., & NORDVIK, H. (1989). Tactual learning and cross-modal transfer of an oddity problem in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 47, 88–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LAZAR, R. (1977). Extending sequence-class membership with matching to sample. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 27, 381–392.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • LYNCH, D. C., & GREEN, G. (1991). Development and crossmodal transfer of contextual control of emergent stimulus relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 139–154.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • O’LEARY, C. A., & BUSH, K. M. (1996). Stimulus equivalence in the tactile modality. The Psychological Record, 46, 509–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • OSBORNE, J. G., & GATCH, M. B. (1989). Stimulus equivalence and receptive reading by hearing-impaired preschool children. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 20, 63–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • RABINOWITZ, F. M. (1983). Cross-modal transfer and intermediate-size transposition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 35, 223–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ROSE, S. A., & ORLIAN, E. K. (1991). Asymmetries in infant cross-modal transfer. Child Development, 62, 706–718.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • SAUNDERS, R. R., SAUNDERS, K. J., KIRBY, K. C., & SPRADLIN, J. E. (1988). The merger and development of equivalence classes by unreinforced conditional selection of comparison stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 145–162.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M. (1971). Reading and auditory-visual equivalences. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 14, 5–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M. (1994). Equivalence relations and behavior: A research story. Boston, MA: Authors Cooperative, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M., & CRESSON, O. J. (1973). Reading and cross-modal transfer of stimulus equivalences in severe retardation. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 77, 515–523.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M., & TAILBY, W. (1982). Conditional discrimination vs. matching to sample: An expansion of the testing paradigm. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 5–22.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SIGURDARDOTTIR, S. G., GREEN, G., & SAUNDERS, R. R. (1990). Equivalence classes generated by sequence training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 53, 47–63.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SKINNER, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: The MacMillan Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • SPENCER, T. J., & CHASE, P. N. (1996). Speed analyses of stimulus equivalence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 643–659.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SPRADLIN, J. E., COTTER, V. W., & BAXLEY, N. (1973). Establishing a conditional discrimination without direct training: a study of transfer with retarded adolescents. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 77, 556–566.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • TIERNEY, K. J., De LARGY, P., & BRACKEN, M. (1995). Formation of an equivalence class incorporating haptic stimuli. The Psychological Record, 45, 431–437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • WETHERBY, B., KARLAN, G. R., & SPRADLIN, J. E. (1983). The development of derived stimulus relations through training in arbitrary-matching sequences. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 40, 69–78.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • WULFERT, E., GREENWAY, D. E., & DOUGHER, M. J. (1994). Third-order equivalence classes. The Psychological Record, 44, 411–439.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • WULFERT, E., & HAYES, S. C. (1988). Transfer of a conditional ordering response through conditional equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 40, 125–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

We thank Kenneth F. Reeve and Barbara J. Adams for their valuable suggestions during the preparation of this manuscript. This research was conducted with support from Contracts MDA903-90-C-0132 and DASW01-96-K-0009 from the U. S. Army Research Institute.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Belanich, J., Fields, L. Tactual Equivalence Class Formation and Tactual-to-Visual Cross-Modal Transfer. Psychol Rec 49, 75–91 (1999). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395308

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395308

Navigation