Skip to main content
Log in

Human Aggressive and Escape Responding: Effects of Provocation Frequency

  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The effect of point loss frequency on aggressive and escape responding was investigated in two experiments. Subjects could respond on levers marked “A,” “B,” or “C” available as nonreversible options. Subjects were instructed that “A” responses earned points exchangeable for money, “B” responses subtracted points from a second subject, and “Cm responses protected their counter from point loss for a period of time. These options were defined as nonaggressive (A), aggressive (B), and escape (C) responses, respectively. A variable time schedule of point loss engendered aggressive and escape responding. The preexperimental instructions attributed point losses to the subject’s fictitious partner. Levers B and C responding escaped point loss by initiating an interval free of provocation (PFI). In Experiment 1 systematically decreasing PFI increased the frequency of point loss and increased aggressive responding for three of the four subjects. In Experiment 2 Pfi durations were systematically alternated between 500 seconds and 125 seconds in an Abab or Baba design. Escape responding increased as a function of increased provocation frequency for all subjects. Order of exposure affected frequency of aggressive and escape responses.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • AZRIN, N. H. (1970). Punishment of elicited aggression. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 14, 7–10.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • AZRIN, N. H., HUTCHINSON, R. R., & HAKE, D. F. (1967). Attack, avoidance, and escape reactions to aversive shock. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 10, 131–148.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • AZRIN, N. H., HUTCHINSON, R. R., & SALLERY, R. D. (1964). Pain aggression toward inanimate objects. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 7, 223–229.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • BANDURA, A. (1973). Aggression: A social learning analysis. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • BLANCHARD, R. J., BLANCHARD, D. C., & TAKAHASHI, L. K. (1978). Pain and aggression in the rat. Behavioral Biology, 23, 291–305.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • BUSS, A. H. (1961). The psychology of aggression. New York: J. Wiley and Son.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • CHEREK, D. R. (1981). Effects of smoking different doses of nicotine on human aggressive behavior. Psychopharmacology 75, 339–345.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • CHEREK, D. R., SPIGA, R., STEINBERG, J. L., & KELLY, T. H. (1990). Human aggressive responses maintained by avoidance or escape from point loss. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 53, 293–303.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • CHEREK, D. R., STEINBERG, J. L., & BRAUCHI, J. T. (1983). Effects of caffeine on human aggressive behavior. Psychiatry Research, 8, 137–145.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • CHEREK, D. R., STEINBERG, J. L., KELLY, T. H., & ROBINSON, D. E. (1986). Effects of d-amphetamine on human aggressive behavior. Psychopharmacology, 88, 381–386.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DENGERINK, H. A., & COVEY, M. K. (1983). Implications of an escape-avoidance theory of aggressive responses to attack. In R. G. Geen & E. I. Donnerstein (Eds.), Aggression: Theoretical and empirical reviews (Vol. 1, pp. 163–188). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • HUTCHINSON, R. R. (1977). By-products of aversive control. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp. 415–431). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • HUTCHINSON, R. R. (1983). The pain-aggression relationship and its expression in naturalistic settings. Aggressive Behavior, 9, 229–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • PATTERSON, G. R., & COBB, J. A. (1973). Stimulus control for classes of noxious behavior. In J. F. Knutson (Ed.), The control of aggression: Implications from basic research (pp. 145–201). Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • TAYLOR, S. P. (1967). Aggressive behavior and physiological arousal as a function of provocation and the tendency to inhibit aggression. Journal of Personality, 35, 297–310.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • ULRICH, R. (1967). Interactions between reflexive fighting and cooperative escape. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 10, 311–317.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • WEINER, H. (1969). Conditioning history and the control of human avoidance and escape responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 1039–1043.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Cherek, D.R., Spiga, R., Bennett, R.H. et al. Human Aggressive and Escape Responding: Effects of Provocation Frequency. Psychol Rec 41, 3–17 (1991). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395090

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395090

Navigation