Skip to main content
Log in

The Role of Conditioned Reinforcement in the Acquisition and Maintenance of Omission Responding

  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The role of the stimulus change accompanying food reinforcement, a potential conditioned reinforcer, in the development and maintenance of omission responding was examined in two omission paradigms. In the conventional response-terminated procedure a response during the trial stimulus immediately extinguishes the key light and initiates the intertrial interval (ITI), while in the standard fixed-trial procedure the trial stimulus terminates at the end of a fixed period of time irrespective of responding during the trial. A response during the trial stimulus in either procedure cancels the food reinforcement ordinarily available at the termination of the trial. In the present experiment, a particular key color was paired explicitly with food presentations on response-free trials. The conditioned reinforcing value of this stimulus was assessed in the fixed-trial and response-terminated procedures by presenting or withholding it at the end of trials containing a peck. The results suggest no critical role for conditioned reinforcement in the acquisition and maintenance of omission responding. For both procedures the rate of key peck acquisition, steady-state levels of responding, response latency and response rate were independent of the availability of conditioned reinforcement on food-omission trials. Moreover, the performances of subjects trained on the response-terminated procedure were no different from those of subjects exposed to the fixed-trial procedure.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Reference Note

  1. BALDOCK, M. 1974. Trial and intertribal interval durations in the acquisition of autoshaped key pecking. Paper presented at the meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, Philadelphia.

    Google Scholar 

References

  • BARRERA, F. J. 1974. Centrifugal selection of signal-directed pecking. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 22, 341–355.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • BROWN, P., & JENKINS, H. 1968. Autoshaping of the pigeon’s key peck. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11, 1–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • GAMZU, E., & SCHWAM, E. 1974. Autoshaping and automaintenance of a key-press response in squirrel monkeys. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 21, 361–371.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • GRICE, G. R. 1948. The relation of secondary reinforcement to delayed reward in visual discrimination learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 1–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • HEARST, E., & JENKINS, H. 1974. Sign tracking: The stimulus-reinforcer relation and directed action. Austin: The Psychonomic Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • HERRNSTEIN, R., & LOVELAND, D. 1972. Food-avoidance in hungry pigeons, and other perplexities. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 18, 369–384.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • HURSH, S., NAVARICK, D., & FANTINO, E. 1974. “Automaintenance”: The role of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 21, 117–124.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • KELLEHER, R., & FRY, W. 1962. Stimulus functions in chained fixed-interval schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 5, 167–173.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • KELLEHER, R., & GOLLUB, L. 1962. A review of positive conditioned reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 5, 543–597.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • KLING, J. W., & SCHRIER, A. M. 1971. Positive reinforcement. In J. W. Kling & I. A. Riggs (Eds.), Woodworth and Schlosberg’s experimental psychology (3rd ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • POWELL, R., & KELLY, W. 1976. Responding under positive and negative response contingencies in crows and pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 25, 219–225.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SCHWARTZ, B. 1972. The role of positive conditioned reinforcement in the maintenance of key pecking which prevents delivery of primary reinforcement. Psychonomic Science, 28, 277–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SCHWARTZ, B. 1973. Maintenance of key pecking by response-independent food presentation: The role of the modality of the signal for food. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 20, 17–22.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • ASCHWARTZ, B., & GAMZU, E. In Press. Pavlovian control of operant behavior: An analysis of autoshaping and the interactions between multiple schedules of reinforcement. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-Hall.

  • SCHWARTZ, B., & WILLIAMS, D. 1972a. The role of the response-reinforcer contingency in negative automaintenance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 17, 351–357.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SCHWARTZ, B., & WILLIAMS, D. 1972b. Two different kinds of key peck in the pigeon: Some properties of responses maintained by negative and positive response-reinforcer contingencies. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 18, 201–216.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SNAPPER, A., KNAPP, J., & KUSHNER, H. 1970. Mathematical description of schedules of reinforcement. In W. N. Schoenfeld (Ed.), The theory of reinforcement schedules. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • STADDON, J. E. R., & SIMMELHAG, V. 1971. The superstition experiment: A reexamination of its implications for the principles of adaptive behavior. Psychological Review, 78, 3–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • STIERS, M., & SILBERBERG, A. 1974. Lever-contact responses in rats: Automaintenance with and without a negative response-reinforcer dependency. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 22, 497–506.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • TERRACE, H., GIBBON, J., FARRELL, L., & BALDOCK, M. 1975. Temporal factors influencing the acquisition and maintenance of an autoshaped response. Animal Learning and Behavior, 3, 53–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • WESSELS, M. 1974. The effects of reinforcement upon the prepecking behaviors of pigeons in the autoshaping experiment. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 21, 125–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • WILLIAMS, D., & WILLIAMS, H. 1969. Automaintenance in the pigeon: Sustained pecking despite contingent nonreinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 511–520.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

This research was partially supported by a grant from the Faculty Research Fund, Western Michigan University. The study was conducted while the first author held a Graduate Fellowship sponsored by the Graduate School, Western Michigan University. Preparation of the manuscript was completed while the first author held an NIMH Predoctoral Fellowship (1F31 MH05993-01).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Petersen, M.R., Lyon, D.O., Stone, W. et al. The Role of Conditioned Reinforcement in the Acquisition and Maintenance of Omission Responding. Psychol Rec 27, 235–254 (1977). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03394443

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03394443

Navigation