Skip to main content
Log in

An intentional interpretive perspective

  • Published:
The Behavior Analyst Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

To the extent that the concept of intention has been addressed within behavior analysis, descriptions of intention have been general and have not specifically included important distinctions that differentiate a behavior-analytic approach from vernacular definitions of intention. A fundamental difference between a behavior-analytic approach and most other psychological approaches is that other approaches focus on the necessity of intentions to explain behavior, whereas a behavior-analytic approach is directed at understanding the interplay between behavior and environment. Behavioranalytic interpretations include the relations between the observer’s behavior and the environment. From a behavior-analytic perspective, an analysis of the observer’s interpretations of an individual’s behavior is inherent in the subsequent attribution of intention. The present agenda is to provide a behavior-analytic account of attributing intention that identifies the establishing conditions for speaking of intention. Also addressed is the extent to which we speak of intentions when the observed individual’s behavior is contingency shaped or under instructional control.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baum, W. M. (1995). Rules, culture and fitness. The Behavior Analyst, 18, 1–21.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Blackman, D. E. (1979, September). On the mental element in crime and behaviorism. Paper presented at the meeting of the SSRC Law and Psychology Seminar Group, Oxford, England.

  • Cerutti, D. T. (1989). Discrimination theory of rule-governed behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 259–276.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dasser, V., Ulbaek, I., & Premack, D. (1989). The perception of intention. Science, 243, 365–367.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Day, W. F. (1975). Contemporary behaviorism and the concept of intention. In J. K. Cole & W. J. Arnold (Eds.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 65–129). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (1988). Précis of the intentional stance. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 495–546.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friman, P. C, Wilson, K. G., & Hayes, S. C. (1998). Behavior analysis of private events is possible, progressive, and nondualistic: A response to Lamal. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 707–708.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Glenn, S. S. (1983). Maladaptive functional relations in client verbal behavior. The Behavior Analyst, 6, 7–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, A. M., & Wojnilower, D. A. (1984). Self-directed behavior change in children: Is it self-directed? Behavior Therapy, 15, 501–514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C, Rosenfarb, I., Wulfert, E., Munt, E. D., Korn, Z., & Zettle, R. D. (1985). Selfreinforcement effects: An artifact of social standard setting. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18, 201–214.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hefferline, R. E, Keenon, B., & Harford, R. A. (1959). Escape and avoidance conditioning in human subjects without their observation of the response. Science, 130, 1338–1339.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hineline, P. N. (1992). A self-interpretive behavior analysis. American Psychologist, 47, 1274–1286.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hineline, P. N., & Wanchisen, B. A. (1989). Correlated hypothesizing and the distinction between contingency-shaped and rule-governed behavior. In S. C. Hayes (Ed.), Rulegoverned behavior: Cognition, contingencies, and instructional control (pp. 221–268). New York: Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, R. T., Nelson, R. E., & Kazdin, A. E. (1977). The role of external variables in selfreinforcement: A review. Behavior Modification, 1, 147–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lacey, H. (1998). On the limits of radical behaviorism: A reply to Leigland’s reply. Behavior and Philosophy, 26, 63–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lana, R. E. (1991). Assumptions of social psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lana, R. E. (1994). Social history and the behavioral repertoire. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 62, 315–322.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Landgrebe, L. (1981). The phenomenology of Edmund Husserl. New York: Cornell University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, J. (1979). Laboratory studies of selfreinforcement (SR) phenomena. Journal of General Psychology, 101, 103–149.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, J. (1980). External versus self-reinforcement: A review of methodological and theoretical issues. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 12, 111–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. (1962). Phenomenology of perception. New York: Humanities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. R. (1980). The intentionality of intention in action. Cognitive Science, 4, 47–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. R. (1990). Consciousness, explanatory inversion and cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13, 585–642.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1945). The operational analysis of psychological terms. Psychological Review, 52, 270–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

  • Skinner, B. F. (1966). An operant analysis of problem solving. In B. Kleinmuntz (Ed.), Problem solving: Research, method, and theory (pp. 225–257). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1971). Beyond freedom and dignity. New York: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1974). About behaviorism. New York: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sohn, D., & Lamal, P. A. (1982). Self-reinforcement: Its reinforcing capability and its clinical utility. The Psychological Record, 32, 179–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wann, T. W (1964). Behaviorism and phenomenology: Contrasting basis for modern psychology. In T. W. Wann (Ed.), Behaviorism and phenomenology (pp. 1–190). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkerson, W S. (1999). From bodily motions to bodily intentions. Philosophical Psychology, 12, 61–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zettle, R. D., & Hayes, S. C. (1982). Rule-governed behavior: A potential theoretical framework for cognitive-behavioral therapy. In P. C. Kendall (Ed.), Advances in cognitive behavioral research and therapy (pp. 73–117). New York: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paul Neuman.

Additional information

I dedicate this paper to Douglas P. Field. I thank the many readers of early versions of this manuscript.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Neuman, P. An intentional interpretive perspective. BEHAV ANALYST 27, 55–65 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392092

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392092

Key words

Navigation