Skip to main content
Log in

Rules, Culture, and Fitness

  • Published:
The Behavior Analyst Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Behavior analysis risks intellectual isolation unless it integrates its explanations with evolutionary theory. Rule-governed behavior is an example of a topic that requires an evolutionary perspective for a full understanding. A rule may be defined as a verbal discriminative stimulus produced by the behavior of a speaker under the stimulus control of a long-term contingency between the behavior and fitness. As a discriminative stimulus, the rule strengthens listener behavior that is reinforced in the short run by socially mediated contingencies, but which also enters into the long-term contingency that enhances the listener’s fitness. The long-term contingency constitutes the global context for the speaker’s giving the rule. When a rule is said to be “internalized,” the listener’s behavior has switched from short- to long-term control. The fitness-enhancing consequences of long-term contingencies are health, resources, relationships, or reproduction. This view ties rules both to evolutionary theory and to culture. Stating a rule is a cultural practice. The practice strengthens, with short-term reinforcement, behavior that usually enhances fitness in the long run. The practice evolves because of its effect on fitness. The standard definition of a rule as a verbal statement that points to a contingency fails to distinguish between a rule and a bargain (“If you’ll do X, then I’ll do Y”), which signifies only a single short-term contingency that provides mutual reinforcement for speaker and listener. In contrast, the giving and following of a rule (“Dress warmly; it’s cold outside”) can be understood only by reference also to a contingency providing long-term enhancement of the listener’s fitness or the fitness of the listener’s genes. Such a perspective may change the way both behavior analysts and evolutionary biologists think about rule-governed behavior.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alcock, J. (1993). Animal behavior (5th ed.). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allan, L. G., & Jenkins, H. M. (1983). The effect of representations of binary variables on judgment of influence. Learning and Motivation, 14, 381–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1979). Judgment of contingency in depressed and non-depressed students: Sadder but wiser? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 441–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baum, W. M. (1994a). John B. Watson and behavior analysis: Past, present, and future. In J. T. Todd & E. K. Morris (Eds.), Modern perspectives on John B. Watson and classical behaviorism (pp. 133–140). Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baum, W. M. (1994b). Understanding behaviorism: Science, behavior, and culture. New York: HarperCollins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blakely, E., & Schlinger, H. (1987). Rules: Function-altering contingency-specifying stimuli. The Behavior Analyst, 10, 183–187.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cerutti, D. T. (1989). Discrimination theory of rule-governed behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 259–276.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, R. (1989). The selfish gene (new ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, A., Shanks, D., & Evenden, J. (1984). Judgement of act-outcome contingency: The role of selective attribution. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36A, 29–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenn, S. S. (1988). Contingencies and meta-contingencies: Toward a synthesis of behavior analysis and cultural materialism. The Behavior Analyst, 11, 161–179.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Glenn, S. S. (1991). Contingencies and meta-contingencies: Relations among behavioral, cultural, and biological evolution. In P. A. Lamal (Ed.), Behavioral analysis of societies and cultural practices (pp. 39–73). New York: Hemisphere.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M. (1980). Cultural materialism. New York: Vintage Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M. (1987). Foodways: Historical overview and theoretical prolegomenon. In M. Harris & E. B. Ross (Eds.), Food and evolution (pp. 57–90). Philadelphia: Temple University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M., & Ross, E. B. (1987). Death, sex, and fertility. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, L. J., & Chase, P. N. (Eds.). (1991). Dialogues on verbal behavior. Reno, NV: Context.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C. (Ed.). (1989). Rule-governed behavior: Cognition, contingencies, and instructional control. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein, R. J. (1969). Method and theory in the study of avoidance. Psychological Review, 76, 49–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hineline, P. N., & Wanchisen, B. A. (1989). Correlated hypothesizing and the distinction between contingency-shaped and rule-governed behavior. In S. C. Hayes (Ed.), Rule-governed behavior: Cognition, contingencies, and instructional control (pp. 221–268). New York: Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Logue, A. W. (1978). Behaviorist John B. Watson and the continuity of the species. Behaviorism, 6, 71–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logue, A. W. (1994). Watson’s behaviorist manifesto: Past positive and current negative consequences. In J. T. Todd & E. K. Morris (Eds.), Modern perspectives on John B. Watson and classical behaviorism (pp. 109–123). Westport, CT: Greenwood.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malagodi, E. F., & Jackson, K. (1989). Behavior analysts and cultural analysis: Troubles and issues. The Behavior Analyst, 12, 17–33.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Petrovich, S. B., & Gewirtz, J. L. (1991). Imprinting and attachment: Proximate and ultimate considerations. In J. L. Gewirtz & W. M. Kurtines (Eds.), Intersections with attachment (pp. 69–93). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reese, H. W. (1989). Rules and rule-governance: Cognitive and behavioristic views. In S. C. Hayes (Ed.), Rule-governed behavior: Cognition, contingencies, and instructional control (pp. 3–84). New York: Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Segal, E. F. (1972). Induction and the provenance of operants. In R. M. Gilbert & J. R. Millenson (Eds.), Reinforcement: Behavioral analyses (pp. 1–34). New York: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1945). The operational analysis of psychological terms. Psychological Review, 52, 270–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1969). An operant analysis of problem solving. In Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical analysis (pp. 133–171). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1971). Beyond freedom and dignity. New York: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1974). About behaviorism. New York: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1981). Selection by consequences. Science, 213, 501–504.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sommerville, C. J. (1982). The rise and fall of childhood. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Staddon, J. E. R. (1977). Schedule-induced behavior. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp. 125–152). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Staddon, J. E. R. (1983). Adaptive behavior and learning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Todd, J. T. (1994). What psychology has to say about John B. Watson: Classical behaviorism in psychology textbooks, 1920–1989. In J. T. Todd & E. K. Morris (Eds.), Modern perspectives on John B. Watson and classical behaviorism (pp. 75–107). Westport, CT: Greenwood.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist views it. Psychological Review, 20, 158–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. S., & Sober, E. (1994). Re-introducing group selection to the human behavioral sciences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 585–654. (Includes commentary)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zettle, R. D., & Hayes, S. C. (1982). Rule-governed behavior: A potential theoretical framework for cognitive-behavioral therapy. In P. C. Kendall (Ed.), Advances in cognitive-behavioral research and therapy (Vol. 1, pp. 73–118). New York: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to William M. Baum.

Additional information

This paper is gratefully dedicated to my teacher, Richard J. Herrnstein. A version was presented at the Association for Behavior Analysis meeting in Atlanta, May 1994.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Baum, W.M. Rules, Culture, and Fitness. BEHAV ANALYST 18, 1–21 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392688

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392688

Key words

Navigation