Skip to main content
Log in

Background knowledge and the construction of a situational representation from a diagram

  • Published:
European Journal of Psychology of Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Comprehension of a diagram requires viewers to construct from its graphic constituents a mental representation that captures the situational entities and relationships referred to by the diagram. However, this implies viewers possess appropriate background knowledge concerning the depicted situation. Meteorologists' and non-meteorologists' mental representations were investigated using a three-stage card sorting task during which subjects generated hierarchical groupings of the graphic elements of an Australian weather map diagram. Cluster analysis indicated that the two subject groups differed fundamentally in the basis of their sorting behaviour. Subjects' justifications of the groupings suggested that non-meteorologists' mental representation of the diagram elements was primarily based upon domain-general, visuospatial characteristics whereas in meteorologists' representations, these characteristics were subservient to a domain-specific, situational interpretation of the graphic array. The findings indicate that background knowledge deficiencies may make it difficult for learners beginning study of a domain to construct suitable mental representations from domain-related diagrams.

Résumé

La compréhension d'un graphique exige du sujet qu'il construise, sur la base de la représentation figurale, une représentation mentale des éléments du graphique et de leurs relations. Cela suppose, de la part du sujet, des connaissances adéquates déjà constituées à propos de la situation représentée par le graphique. Les représentations mentales de météorologues et de non-météorologues ont été comparées à la faveur d'une tâche de sélections successives (trois étapes) d'éléments d'une carte météorologique qu'ils devaient regrouper en fonction de leurs relations et selon une catégorisation hiérarchique. Une analyse en clusters a mis en évidence des différences très nettes dans les principes des classifications appliquées par les deux groupes de sujets. Les justifications fournies ont révélé que les non-météorologues fondent leur représentation mentale des éléments du graphique sur des caractéristiques générales et visuo-spatiales du domaine; les météorologues s'appuient sur une interprétation spécifique et située de l'information apportée par le graphique. L'inadéquation des connaissances antérieures gène l'acquisition dans des domaines nouveaux où le sujet doit construire des représentations appropriées à partir de représentations graphiques du domaine étudié.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Akin, O. (1986).Psychology of architectural design. London: Pion.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allard, F., & Burnett, N. (1985) Skill in sport.Canadian Journal of Psychology, 39, 294–312.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blystone, R.V., & Dettling, B.C., (1990). Visual literacy in science textbooks. In M. Budd Rowe (Ed.),What research says to the science teacher — The process of knowing (pp. 19–40). National Science Teachers Association.

  • Chase, W.G., & Simon, H.A. (1973). Perception in chess.Cognitive Psychology, 4, 55–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M.T.H., Feltovich, P.J. & Glaser, R. (1981). Categorisation and representation of physics problems by experts and novices.Cognitive Science, 5, 121–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Constable, H., Campbell, R., & Brown, R. (1988). Sectional drawings from science books: An experimental investigation into pupils' understanding.British Journal of Educational Psychology, 58, 89–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Egan, D.E., & Schwartz, B.J. (1979). Chunking of recall of symbolic drawings.Memory & Cognition, 7, 149–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fincher-Kiefer, R., Post, T.A., Greene, T.R., & Voss, J.F. (1988). On the role of prior knowledge and task demands in the processing of text.Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 416–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gillespe, C.S. (1993). Reading graphic displays; What teachers should know.Journal of Reading, 36, 350–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A.M., & Langston, W.E. (1992). Comprehension of illustrated text: Pictures help to build mental models.Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 129–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kindfield, A.C.H. (1993). Biology diagrams: Tools to think with.Journal of the Learning Sciences.3, 1–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (1983). Learning from text.Cognition and Instruction, 3, 87–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larkin, J.H. (1983). The role of problem representation in physics. In D. Gentner & A.L. Stevens (Eds.),Mental Models (pp. 75–98). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larkin, J.H., & Simon, H.A. (1987). Why a diagram is (sometimes) worth ten thousand words.Cognitive Science, 11, 65–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, J.R., Anglin, G.J., & Carney, R.N. (1987). On empirically validating functions of pictures in prose. In D.M. Willows & H. A. Houghton (Eds.),The psychology of illustration (vol. 1, pp. 51–85). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lowe, R.K. (1988). “Reading” scientific diagrams: Characterising components of skilled performance.Research in Science Education, 18, 112–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lowe, R.K. (1989). Search strategies and inference in the exploration of scientific diagrams.Educational Psychology, 9, 27–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lowe, R.K. (1993). Constructing a mental representation from an abstract technical diagram.Learning and Instruction, 3, 157–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lowe, R.K. (1994). Selectivity in diagrams: Reading beyond the lines.Educational Psychology, 14, 467–491.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mandl, H., & Levin, J.R. (Eds.) (1989).Knowledge acquisition from text and pictures. Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R.E. (1989). Models for understanding.Review of Educational Research, 59, 43–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R.E., & Anderson, R.B. (1992). The instructive animation: Helping students build connections between words and pictures in multimedia learning.Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 444–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R.E., & Gallini, J.K. (1990). When is an illustration worth ten thousand words?Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 715–726.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schnotz, W., & Kulhavy, R. (1994).Comprehension of graphics. Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spilich, G.J., Vesonder, G.T., Chiesi, H.L., & Voss, J.F. (1979). Text processing of domain-related information for individuals with high and low domain knowledge.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior.18, 275–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Willows D.M., & Houghton, H.A. (Eds.) (1987).The psychology of illustration: Volume 1. Basic research. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winn, W.D. (1991). Learning from maps and diagrams.Educational Psychology Review.,3, 211–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winn, W.D. (1993). An account of how readers search for information in diagrams.Contemporary Educational Psychology, 18, 162–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winn, W.D. (1994). Contributions of perceptual and cognitive processes to the comprehension of graphics. In W. Schnotz & R.W. Kulhavy (Eds.),Comprehension of graphics (pp. 3–27). Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Winn, W.D., & Li, T.-Z. (1989, April).Do diagrams permit more rapid and accurate problem solving than text? Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Francisco.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

The cooperation and support of the Director and Staff of the Western Australian Regional Office of the Australian Bureau of Meteorology is gratefully acknowledged.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lowe, R.K. Background knowledge and the construction of a situational representation from a diagram. Eur J Psychol Educ 11, 377–397 (1996). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03173279

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03173279

Key words

Navigation