Skip to main content
Log in

Fictitious social position of competence, and performance in a foreign-language interaction situation: An experimental approach

  • Published:
European Journal of Psychology of Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A substantial body of research has shown that in experiments where subjects are required to accomplish an intellectual task, their cognitive performance is influenced by the social aspects of the situation. The goal of this study was to determine how a fictitious social position of inferiority or superiority affects cognitive activity. Two studies in which subjects interacted with a partner are reported. In the first, the subject’s position in comparison to his/her partner stemmed from a fictitious level of competence assigned on a single comparative dimension (foreign-language proficiency). In the second, another comparative dimension whose social status was either high (geometry) or low (art) was added. The results showed that in a communicative interaction, what subjects are told about themselves and about their partner along one or two comparative dimensions is a determinant of the quantity and quality of what they produce. Theoretical explanations are proposed. One of them is based on classical studies on the working self-concept. A general point that emerges from these studies is that cognitive performance should be investigated not only in a cognitive and didactic perspective, but also from a sociocognitive standpoint.

Résumé

Une série de recherches a montré que des sujets soumis expérimentalement à une tâche cognitive produisaient des performances varaiables selon les aspects sociaux de la situation dans laquelle ils devaient agir. L’objectif de l’étude présentée ici consiste à déterminer comment une position sociale fictive de supériorité ou d’infériorité influence l’activité cognitive. Deux expériences dans lesquelles des élèves interagissent avec un partenaire sont rapportées. Dans la première, en utilisant une seule dimension comparative, les élèves sont affectés à une position sociale fondée sur une compétence fictive dans le domaine des langues étrangères. Dans la seconde, une deuxième dimension comparative est introduite. Celle-ci est dotée soit d’un statut social élevé (la géométrie), soit d’un statut social faible (le dessin). Les résultats montrent que l’information de position sociale de compétence fournie aux sujets détermine significativement la quantité et la qualité de ce qu’ils produisent. Des explications théoriques sont développées dont une réfère plus particulièrement aux études classiques sur le soi de travail. Ce qui ressort de ces deux études c’est que les performances cognitives, en plus d’être examinées dans des perspectives cognitive et didactique, doivent être considérées d’un point de vue socio-cognitif.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abdi, H. (1987).Introduction au traitement statistique des données expérimentales. Grenoble: Presses Universitaires de Grenoble.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency.American Psychologist, 37, 122–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beebe, L.M., & Takahashi, T. (1989). Do you have a bag?: Social status and patterned variation in second language acquisition. In S. Gass, C. Madden, D. Preston, & L. Selinker (Eds.),Variation in second language acquisition (vol. 1, pp. 103–125). Discourse and diagnostics: Multilingual Matters Series.

  • Bialystok, E. (1990). The competence of processing: Classifying theories of second language acquisition.TESOL Quarterly, 24, 635–648.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., & Sharwood-Smith, M. (1985). Interlanguage is not a state of mind: An evaluation of the construct for second-language acquisition.Applied Linguistics, 6, 10–117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boekaerts, M. (1991). Subjective competence, appraisals and self-assessment.Learning and Instruction, 1, 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bongaerts, T., & Poulisse, N. (1989). Communication strategies in L1 and L2: Same or different?.Applied Linguistics, 10, 253–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brickman, P., & Bulman, R.J. (1977). Pleasure and pain in social comparison. In J.M. Suls & R.L. Miller (Eds.),Social comparison processes: Theoretical and empirical perspectives (pp. 149–186). Washington, DC: Hemispher.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cameron, J., & Epling, W.F. (1989). Successful problem solving as a function of interaction style for nonnative students of English.Applied Linguistics, 10, 392–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chambon, M. (1990a). La-perception d’une discipline scolaire par les élèves, représentations et effets identitaires.European Journal of Psychology of Education, 5, 337–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chambon, M. (1990b). La représentation des disciplines scolaires par les parents d’élèves: Enjeux de valeurs, enjeux sociaux.Revue Française de Pédagogie, 92, 31–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chambres, P. (1993). Social comparison and knowledge construction.Learning and Instruction, 3, 23–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chambres, P. (1994). Social position and cognitive activity in second language production. In H.C. Foot, C.J. Howe, A. Anderson, A.K. Tolmie, & D.A. Warden (Eds.),Group and Interactive Learning (pp. 403–410), Southampton Boston: Computational Mechanics Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chambres, P. (1995a). Position sociale de compétence fictive et production en langue étranère: Le rôle de la métacognition et des ressources attentionnelles.Revue de phonétique appliquée, 115–116–117, 169–181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chambres, P. (1995b). Influence of an expert social position on incidental learning: The role of metacognitive activity.International Review of Social Psychology, 1, 79–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chambres, P., & Bavent, L. (1989). Résolution de problème: Influence de contextes et marquage social. Une approche expérimentale. In J.M. Monteil & M. Fayol (Eds.),La psychologie scientifique et ses applications (pp. 289–298). Grenoble: Presse Universitaire de Grenoble.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crooks, G., & Rulon, K.A. (1988). Topic and feedback in native-speaker/nonnative-speaker conversation.TESOL Quarterly, 22, 675–681.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fantuzzo, J.W., Riggio, R.E., Connelly, S., & Dimeff, L.A. (1989). Effects of reciprocal peer tutoring on academic achievement and psychological adjustment: A component analysis.Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 173–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fraysse, J.C. (1991). Effect of social insertion mode on performance and interaction in asymmetric dyads.European Journal of Psychology of Education, 6, 45–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaonac’h, D. (1990). La gestion des ressources cognitives dans les activités de langage en langue étrangère.Revue de Phonétique Appliquée, 95–96–97, 165–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garner, R. (1987).Metacognition and reading comprehension. Norwood: Ablex Publishing Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gass, S., & Varonis, E. (1985). Variation in native speaker speech modification to nonnative speakers.Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 7, 37–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Genesee, F., & Bourhis, R.Y. (1988). Evaluative reactions to language choice strategies: The role of sociostructural factors.Language and Education, 8, 229–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, D.T., Giesler, R.B., & Morris, K.A. (1995). When comparisons arise.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 227–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goethals, G.R. (1986). Social comparison theory: Psychology from the lost and found.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 12, 261–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guerrin, B. (1989). Social inhibition of behavior.The Journal of Social Psychology, 129, 225–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huguet, P., & Monteil, J.M. (1992). Social comparison and cognitive performance: A descriptive approach in an academic context.European Journal of Psychology of Education, 7, 131–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knottnerus, J.D. (1988). A critique of expectation states theory: Theoretical assumptions and models of social cognition.Sociological Perspectives, 31, 420–445.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1985). Volitional mediators of cognition-behavior consistency: Self-regulatory processes and action versus state orientation. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckmann (Eds.),Action control: From cognition to behavior (pp. 101–128). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Long, M.H. (1983). Native speaker/nonnative speaker conversation and the negotiation of comprehensible input.Applied Linguistics, 4, 126–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H., & Kanda, Z. (1986). Stability and malleability of the self-concept.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 858–866.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H., & Nurius, P.S. (1986). Possible selves.American Psychologist, 41, 954–969.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H., & Wurf, E. (1987). The dynamic self-concept: A social psychological perspective.Annual Review of Psychology, 38, 299–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, W.H. (1990). Influences of internal and external frames of reference on the formation of math and English self-concepts.Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 107–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McLaughlin, B., Rossman, T., & McLeod, B. (1983). Second-language learning: An information-processing perspective.Language Learning, 33, 135–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Monteil, J.M. (1988). Comparaison sociale, stratégies individuelles et médiation socio-cognitives. Un effet de différenciations comportementales dans le champ scolaire.European Journal of Psychology of Education, 3, 3–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monteil, J.M. (1991). Social regulations and individual cognitive function: Effects of individuation on cognitive performance.European Journal of Social Psychology, 21, 225–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Monteil, J.M. (1992). Toward a social psychology of cognitive functioning: Theoretical outline and empirical illustrations. In M. Von Cranach, W. Doise, & G. Mugny (Eds.),Social representations and the social bases of knowledge (pp. 42–55). Berne: Hubert.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monteil, J.M., & Castel, P. (1989). Modes d’insertions sociaux, attribution de sanctions et comparaisons sociales: Une contribution expérimentale à l’explication de conduites scolaires. In J.L. Beauvois, R.V. Joule, & J.M. Monteil (Eds.),Perspectives cognitives et conduites sociales (vol. 2, pp. 299–310). Fribourg: Delval.

    Google Scholar 

  • Musser, L.M., Graziano, W.G., & Moore, J.S. (1987). Developmental and contextual influences on the processing of information about peers.The Journal of Psychology, 121, 387–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nurius, P., & Markus, H. (1990). Situational variability in the self-concept: Appraisals, expectancies, and asymmetries.Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 9, 316–333.

    Google Scholar 

  • Orne, M.T. (1962). On the social psychology of the psychological experiment: With particular reference to demand characteristics and their implications.American Psychologist, 17, 776–783.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pica, T. (1987). Second-language acquisition, social interaction, and the classroom.Applied Linguistics, 8, 3–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poulisse, N. (1987). Problems and solutions in the classification of compensatory strategies.Second Language Research, 3/2, 141–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rijsman, J.B. (1974). Factors in social comparison of performance influencing actual performance.European Journal of Social Psychology, 4, 279–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rijsman, J.B. (1983). The dynamics of social competition in personal and categorical comparison-situations. In W. Doise & S. Moscovici (Eds.),Current issues in European Social Psychology (pp. 279–312). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruvolo, A., & Markus, H. (1992). Possible selves and performance: The power of self-relevant imagery.Social Cognition, 10, 95–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Selinker, L., & Douglas, D. (1985). Wrestling with “context” in interlanguage theory.Applied Linguistics, 6, 190–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spolsky, B. (1989). Communicative competence, language proficiency, and beyond.Applied Linguistics, 10, 138–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Takahashi, T. (1989). The influence of the listener in L2 speech. In S. Gass, C. Madden, D. Preston, & L. Selinker (Eds.),Variation in second language acquisition: psycholinguistic issues (pp. 245–279). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tzelgov, J., Henik, A., & Leiser, D. (1990). Controlling stroop interference: Evidence from a bilingual task.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 16, 760–771.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, J.V. (1989). Theory and research concerning social comparisons of personnal attributes.Psychological Bulletin, 106, 231–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, J.V. (1996). What is social comparison and how should we study it?Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 520–537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woken, M.D., & Swales, J. (1989). Expertise and authority in native/nonnative conversations: The need for a variable account. In S. Gass, C. Madden, D. Preston, & L. Selinker (Eds.),Variation in second language acquisition: Psycholinguistic issues (pp. 211–227). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zuengler, J. (1989). Performance variation in NS-NNS interactions: Ethnolinguistic difference, or discourse domain? In S. Gass, C. Madden, D. Preston, & L. Selinker (Eds.),Variation in second language acquisition: Psycholinguistic issues (pp. 228–244). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Chambres, P., Marescaux, P.J. Fictitious social position of competence, and performance in a foreign-language interaction situation: An experimental approach. Eur J Psychol Educ 13, 411–430 (1998). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03172954

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03172954

Key words

Navigation