Skip to main content

Language Aptitude in Relation to Handedness, Hemispheric Dominance, Cognitive Learning Strategies and Non-verbal IQ: A Combined Quantitative and Qualitative Study

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Exploring Language Aptitude: Views from Psychology, the Language Sciences, and Cognitive Neuroscience

Part of the book series: English Language Education ((ELED,volume 16))

  • 1154 Accesses

Abstract

Individuals vary greatly in their aptitude for language, a phenomenon especially visible in the diverging degree of proficiency present in second language learners. This study uses a combined quantitative and qualitative approach to test individual differences in language aptitude. It explores the impact of handedness and hemispheric brain dominance on language performance by testing participants’ cognitive flexibility in verbal and non-verbal domains. The test battery, consisting of the fifth part of the Modern Language Aptitude Test (MLAT), part F of the LLAMA language aptitude test, and Raven’s Progressive Matrices, was administered to 16 right- and 16 left-handed native German speakers (n = 32) studying art, languages, or natural sciences. These tests serve to evaluate the participants’ aptitude for vocabulary learning, grammatical inferencing, and abstract reasoning. Additionally, two (non-validated) complementary questionnaires enquired about the participants’ preference for either verbal or non-verbal games. The results confirm that handedness does not have any traceable influence on language aptitude, and the groups of art students, language students, and science students did not produce significantly different results. Correlations between scores on the language and reasoning tests indicate that verbal and non-verbal abilities draw on similar mental resources. An additional finding shows that participants opting for non-verbal games scored significantly higher on the language tests than participants who preferred verbal games. These findings lead to the conclusion that handedness and hemispheric dominance have no measurable effect on language performance. Results further suggest that good visuospatial skills can present a considerable advantage in second language learning.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 149.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, E. A. (1952). Analysis of raven’s matrices scores: Preliminary report. Surrey, UK: Surrey Educational Research Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Armour, J., Davison, A., & McManus, I. C. (2014). Genome-wide association study of handedness excludes simple genetic models. Heredity (Edinb.), 112(3), 221–225. https://doi.org/10.1038/hdy.2013.93

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beimer, N. J., Buchtel, H. A., & Glynn, S. M. (2015). One centre’s experience with complications during the Wada test. Epilepsia, 56(8), 110–113. https://doi.org/10.1111/epi.13046

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernat, E., & Lloy, R. (2007). Exploring the gender effect on EFL learners’ beliefs about language learning. Australian Journal of Educational & Developmental Psychology, 7, 79–91. https://doi.org/10.21512/lc.v8i2.453

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biedrón, A., & Pawlak, M. (2016). New conceptualizations of linguistic giftedness. Language Teaching, 49(2), 151–185. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0261444815000439

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Binder, J. R. (2011). Functional MRI is a valid non-invasive alternative to wada testing. Epilepsy & Behaviour, 20(2), 214–222. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2010.08.004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyle, J. P. (1987). Sex differences in listening vocabulary. Language Learning, 37(2), 273–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Briscoe, J., Chilvers, R., Baldeweg, T., & Skuse, D. (2012). A specific cognitive deficit within semantic cognition across a multi-generation family. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 279(1742), 3652–3661. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2012.0894

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. (1981). Twenty-five years of research in foreign language aptitude. In K. C. Diller (Ed.), Individual differences and universals in language learning aptitude (pp. 83–118). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. B., & Stanley, M. (1959). Modern language aptitude test (MLAT). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christiner, M., & Reiterer, S. M. (2015). A Mozart is not a Pavarotti: Singers outperform instrumentalists on foreign accent imitation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 482. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00482

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corballis, M. C. (2014). Left brain, right brain: Facts and fantasies. PLoS Biology, 12(1). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1001767

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corey, D., Hurley, M., & Foundas, A. (2001). Right and left handedness defined: A multivariate approach using hand preference and hand performance measures. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 14(3), 144–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dediu, D. (2008). The role of genetic biases in shaping the correlations between languages and genes. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 254(2). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtbi.2008.05.028

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Dahaene, S., & Hertz-Pannier, L. (2002). Functional neuroimaging of speech perception in infants. Science, 298(5600), 2013–2015. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1077066

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeKeyser, R. (2012). Interaction between individual differences, treatment, and structures in SLA. A Journal of Research in Language Studies, 62(2), 189–200. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2012.00712.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dörnyei, Z., & Skehan, P. (2003). Individual differences in second language learning. In C. J. Doughty & M. H. Long (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 589–630). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dorsaint-Pierre, R., Penhune, V. B., Watkins, K. E., Neelin, P., Lercch, J., Bouffard, M., & Zatorre, R. J. (2006). Asymmetries of the planum temporale and Heschl’s gyrus: Relationship to language lateralization. Brain, 129, 1164–1176. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awl055

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dronkers, N. F., Plaisant, O., Iba-Zizen, M. T., & Cabanis, E. A. (2007). Paul Broca’s historic case: High resolution MR imaging of the brains of Leborgne and Lelong. A Journal of Neurology, 130, 1432–1441. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awm042

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset: The new psychology of success. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golestani, N. (2012). Brain structural correlates of individual differences at low to high-levels of the language processing hierarchy: A review of new approaches to imaging research. International Journal of Bilingualism, 18(1), 6–34. https://doi.org/10.1177/136700691245658

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golestani, N., Paus, T., & Zatorre, R. J. (2002). Anatomical correlates of learning novel speech sounds. Neuron, 35, 997–1010. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00862-0

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, R. S., & Michel, G. F. (1981). Head orientation position during birth and in infant neonatal period, and hand preference at nineteen weeks. Child Development, 52(3), 819–826.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Groen, M. A., Whitehouse, A. J., Badcock, N. A., & Bishop, D. V. (2013). Associations between handedness and cerebral lateralization for language: A comparison of three measures in children. PLoS One, 8(5), e64876. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0064876

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hepper, P. G., Shahidullah, S., & White, R. (1991). Handedness in the human foetus. Neuropsychologia, 29, 1101–1111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, D., Nicholls, M., Shah, M., & Shields, M. (2009). Nature’s experiment? Handedness and early childhood development. Demography, 46(2), 281–301. https://doi.org/10.1353/dem.0.0053

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knecht, S., Dräger, B., Deppe, M., Bobe, L., Lohmann, H., Flöel, A., & Henningsen, H. (2000). Handedness and hemispheric language dominance in healthy humans. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 123(12), 2512–2518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kratzmeier, H., & Horn, R. (1980). Manual: Raven-Matrices test. Advanced progressive matrices. Weinheim, Germany: Beltz Test.

    Google Scholar 

  • Language Learning and Testing Foundation. (2014). Modern language aptitude test. http://lltf.net/aptitude-tests/language -aptitude-tests/modern-language-aptitude-test-2/. Accessed 13 May 2016.

  • Long, M. (1990). Maturational constraints on language development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 12, 251–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meara, P. (2005). LLAMA language aptitude tests the manual. Swansea, UK: Lognostics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mercer, S. (2012). Dispelling the myth of the natural-born linguist. ELT Journal, 66(1), 22–29. https://doi.org/10.1093/elt/ccr022

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papadatou-Pastou, M. (2011). Handedness and language laterization: Why are we right-handed and left-brained? Hellenic Journal of Psychology, 8, 248–265.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pelletier, I., Sauerwein, H. C., Lepore, F., Saint-Amour, D., & Lassonde, M. (2007). Non-invasive alternatives to the Wada test in the presurgical evaluation of language and memory functions in epilepsy patients. Epileptic Disorders, 9(2), 111–126. https://doi.org/10.1684/epd.2007.0109

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Propper, R. E., O’Donnell, L. J., Whalen, S., Tie, Y., Norton, I. H., Suarez, R., & Golby, A. J. (2010). A combined fMRI and DTI examination of functional language lateralization and arcuate fasciculus structure: Effects of degree versus direction of hand preference. Brain and Cognition, 73(2), 85–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2010.03.004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pujol, J., Deus, J., Losilla, J. M., & Capdevilla, A. (1999). Cerebral lateralization of language in normal left-handed people studied by functional MRI. Neurology, 52(5), 1038–1043.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J. (2000). The Raven’s progressive matrices: Change and stability over culture and time. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 1–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J. C. (1941). Standardisation of progressive matrices, 1938. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 19(1), 137–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reiterer, S. M. (2009). Brain and language talent: A synopsis. In G. Dogil & S. M. Reiterer (Eds.), Language talent and brain activity (pp. 155–192). Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, P. (2007). Aptitude, abilities, contexts, practices. In R. DeKyser (Ed.), Practice in a second language: Perspectives from applied linguistics and cognitive psychology (pp. 256–286). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Saville-Troike, M. (2006). Introducing second language acquisition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scerri, T. S., Brandler, W. M., Paracchini, S., Moris, A. P., & Ring, S. M. (2011). PCSK6 is associated with handedness in individuals with dyslexia. Human Molecular Genetics, 20(3), 608–614. https://doi.org/10.1093/hmg/ddq475

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skehan, P. (1998). A cognitive approach to language learning. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, R., & Ganschow, L. (2001). Aptitude for learning a foreign language. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 21, 90–111. https://doi.org/10.1017/S026719050100006X

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. (2002). The theory of successful intelligence and its implications for language aptitude testing. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Individual differences and instructed language learning (pp. 13–44). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Szaflarski, J., Holland, S. K., Schmithorst, V. J., & Byars, A. W. (2006). An fMRI study of language laterization in children and adults. Human Brain Mapping, 27(3), 202–212. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20177

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehouse, A. J., & Bishop, D. (2009). Hemispheric division of function is the result of independent probabilistic biases. Neuropsychologia, 47(8–9), 1938–1943. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Willems, R. M., Toni, I., Hagoort, P., & Casasanto, D. (2009). Body-specific motor imagery of hand actions: Neural evidence from right- and left-handers. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 3(39). https://doi.org/10.3389/neuro.09.039.2009

  • Witkin, H. (1962). Psychological differentiation. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Xiang, H., Dediu, D., Roberts, L., Norris, D., & Hagoort, P. (2012). The structural connectivity underpinning language aptitude, working memory, and IQ in the perisylvian language network. Language Learning, 62(2), 110–130. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2012.00708.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Klara Kager .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Kager, K. (2018). Language Aptitude in Relation to Handedness, Hemispheric Dominance, Cognitive Learning Strategies and Non-verbal IQ: A Combined Quantitative and Qualitative Study. In: Reiterer, S. (eds) Exploring Language Aptitude: Views from Psychology, the Language Sciences, and Cognitive Neuroscience. English Language Education, vol 16. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-91917-1_9

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-91917-1_9

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-91916-4

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-91917-1

  • eBook Packages: EducationEducation (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics