Skip to main content
Log in

The search for subtypes of specific reading disability: Reflections from a cognitive perspective

  • Part II Searching For Subtypes
  • Published:
Annals of Dyslexia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

An overview of the literature on developmental dyslexia is offered, moving historically from attempts to identify a single underlying deficit to more recent efforts to define different subtypes of this developmental disorder. Two approaches to the subtyping problem are illustrated and briefly discussed. It is suggested that many of the remaining questions in dyslexia research could be more profitably addressed within the context of a coherent model of reading behaviour. In the latter half of this review, the potential implications of some aspects of the interactive reading theories for the study of reading dysfunction are explored.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allington, R. L., and Fleming, J. T. 1978. The misreading of high frequency words.Journal of Special Education 12:417–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, J. 1979. Orthographic and word-specific mechanisms in children’s reading of words.Child Development 50:60–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, J., and Strawson, C. 1976. Use of orthographic and word-specific knowledge in reading words aloud.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2:386–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barron, R. W. 1980. Visual and phonological strategies in reading and spelling.In U. Frith (ed.).Cognitive Processes in Spelling. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barron, R. W. 1981. Reading skill and reading strategies.In C. A. Perfetti and A. M. Lesgold (eds.).Interactive Processes in Reading. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biemiller, A. 1977. Relationships between oral reading rates for letters, words and simple text in the development of reading achievement.Reading Research Quarterly 13:223–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blank, M. 1968. Cognitive processes in auditory discrimination in normal and retarded readers.Child Development 39:1091–1101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boder, E. 1973. Developmental dyslexia: A diagnostic approach based on three atypical reading-spelling patterns.Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 15:663–687.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L., and Bryant, P. 1981. Visual memory and phonological skills in reading and spelling backwardness.Psychological Research 43:193–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, R. L., and Netley, C. 1978. Cognitive deficits, learning disabilities, and WISC verbal-performance consistency.Developmental Psychology 14:624–634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Critchley, M. 1970.The Dyslexic Child. Springfield, Ill.: Charles C. Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Curtis, M. E. 1980. Development of components in reading skill.Journal of Educational Psychology 72:656–669.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daneman, M., and Carpenter, P. A. 1980. Individual differences in working memory and reading.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 19:450–466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M. B. 1977. Minimal brain dysfunction and dyslexia: Beyond diagnosis by exclusion.In M. E. Blaw, I. Rapin, and M. Kinsbourne (eds.).Topics in Child Neurology. New York: Spectrum Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M. B. 1979. Childhood learning disabilities.In K. M. Heilman and E. Valenstein (eds.).Clinical Neuropsychology. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M. B., and Rudel, R. G. 1976. Rapid “automatized” naming (R.A.N.): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities.Neuropsychologia 14:417–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M. B., Rudel, R. G., and Broman, M. 1981. Tests that discriminate between dyslexic and other learning disabled boys.Brain and Language 13:118–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doehring, D. G. 1976. Acquisition of rapid reading responses.Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 41: Serial Number 165.

  • Doehring, D. G., and Hoshko, I. M. 1977. Classification of reading problems by the Q-technique of factor analysis.Cortex 13:281–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doehring, D. G., Trites, R., and Patel, P. 1980. Neuropsychological and linguistic correlates of three types of reading disability. Paper presented to the 8th annual meeting of the International Neuropsychological Society, San Francisco.

  • Doehring, D. G., Trites, R., Patel, P., and Fiedorowicz, C.A.M. 1981.Reading Disabilities: The Interaction of Reading, Language, and Neuropsychological Deficits. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. 1979. Linguistic insight: Threshold of reading acquisition. In T. G. Waller and G. E. MacKinnon (eds.).Reading Research: Advances in theory and practice, Vol. I. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliott, D. E., and Needleman, R. M. 1976. The syndrome of hyperlexia.Brain and Language 3: 339–349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fillmore, C. J., Kempler, D., and Wang, W. S.-Y. 1979.Individual Differences in Language Ability and Language Behavior. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, J. M., Satz, P., and Scholes, R. J. 1981. Developmental changes in the linguistic performance correlates of reading achievement.Brain and Language 13:78–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. G. 1972. One second of reading.In J. P. Kavanagh and I. G. Mattingly (eds.).Language by Ear and by Eye. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graesser, A. C. 1981.Prose Comprehension beyond the Word. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guttentag, R. E., and Haith, M. M. 1978. Automatic processing as a function of age and reading ability.Child Development 49:707–716.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Healy, J. M. 1982. The enigma of hyperlexia.Reading Research Quarterly 17:319–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Healy, J. M., Aram, D. M., Horwitz, S. J., and Kessler, J. W. 1982. A study of hyperlexia.Brain and Language 17:1–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, E. 1976. Varieties of cognitive power.In L. B. Resnick (ed.).The Nature of Intelligence. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, E. Lunneborg, C., and Lewis, J. 1975. What does it mean to be high verbal?Cognitive Psychology 7:194–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huttenlocher, R. R., and Huttenlocher, J. 1973. A study of children with hyperlexia.Neurology 23:1107–1116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, M. D., and McClelland, J. 1979. Processing determinants of reading speed.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 108: 151–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, D. J., and Myklebust, H. R. 1967.Learning Disabilities: Educational principles and practices. New York: Grune and Stratton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R. V., Chi, M. T. H., Ingram, A. L., and Danner, F. W. 1977. Constructive aspects of children’s reading comprehension.Child Development 48:684–688.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R. V., and Marshall, C. V. 1978. Reading skill and memory scanning.Journal of Educational Psychology 70:808–814.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, R. M. 1973. A general syntactic processor.In R. Rustin (ed.).Natural Language Processing. New York: Algorithmics Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kastner, S. B., and Rickards, C. 1974. Mediated memory with novel and familiar stimuli in good and poor readers.Journal of Genetic Psychology 124:105–113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kavale, K., and Schreiner, R. 1979. The reading processes of above average and average readers: A comparison of the use of reasoning strategies in responding to standardized comprehension measures.Reading Research Quarterly 15:102–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M., and Warrington, E. K. 1963. Developmental factors in reading and writing backwardness.British Journal of Psychology 54:145–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kolers, P. A. 1972. Some problems of classification.In J. F. Kavanagh and I. G. Mattingly (eds.).Language by Ear and by Eye. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D. 1973. Attention and the measurement of perceptual learning.Memory and Cognition 1:268–276.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D., and Samuels, S. J. 1974. Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading.Cognitive Psychology 6:293–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lesgold, A. M., and Perfetti, C. A. 1978. Interactive Processes in reading comprehension.Discourse Processes 1:323–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lesgold, A. M., and Perfetti, C. A. 1981.Interactive Processes in Reading. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, L. 1975. Susceptibility to interference effects in short-term memory of normal and retarded readers.Perceptual and Motor Skills 40:791–794.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lesser, V. R., Fennell, R. D., Erman, L. D., and Reddy, D. R. 1974. Organization of the HEARSAY II speech understanding system. (Working papers in Speech Recognition III.) Pittsburgh, Pa.: Carnegie-Mellon University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. 1980. Attention and automaticity in stroop and priming tasks: Theory and data.Cognitive Psychology 12:523–553.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lovell, K., Gray, E. A., and Oliver, D. E. 1964. A further study of some cognitive and other disabilities in backward readers of average non-verbal reasoning scores.British Journal of Educational Psychology 34:255–279.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovett, M. W. 1979. The selective encoding of sentential information in normal reading development.Child Development 50:897–900.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lovett, M. W. 1981a. Reading skill and its development: Theoretical and empirical considerations.In G. E. MacKinnon and T. G. Waller (eds.).Reading Research: Advances in theory and practice, Vol. III. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovett, M. W. 1981b. An information processing approach to the classification of reading disabilities. Paper presented to the 89th Annual Meeting of the American Psychological Association, Los Angeles, California.

  • Lovett, M. W. 1984a. A developmental perspective on reading dysfunction: Accuracy and rate criteria in the subtyping of dyslexic children.Brain and Language 22:67–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lovett, M. W. 1984b. A developmental approach to reading disability: Defining dyslexia on a continuum with normal reading skill. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Lovett, M. W., Ransby, M. J., and Barron, R. W. 1984. Treatment, subtype, and word type effects in dyslexic children’s response to remediation. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Lyle, J. G. 1968. Performance of retarded readers on the memory-for-designs test.Perceptual and Motor Skills 26:851–854.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mason, M. 1980. Reading ability and the encoding of item and location information.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 6:89–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mattis, S. 1978. Dyslexia syndromes: A working hypothesis that works.In A. L. Benton and D. Pearl (eds.).Dyslexia: An appraisal of current knowledge. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mattis, S., French, J. H., and Rapin, I. 1975. Dyslexia in children and young adults: Three independent neuropsychological syndromes.Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 17:150–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. L., and Rumelhart, D. E. 1981. An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings.Psychological Review 88:375–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mehegan, C. C., and Dreifus, R. E. 1972. Hyperlexia — exceptional reading ability in brain damaged children.Neurology 22:1105–1111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J. 1964. The effects of context upon speed of reading, eye movements and the eye-voice span.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 16:340–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J. 1969. Interaction of information in word recognition.Psychological Review 76:165–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J. 1970. A functional model for memory.In D. A. Norman (ed.).Models of Human Memory. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nunnally, J. C. 1967.Psychometric Theory. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., and Roth, S. F. 1981. Some of the interactive processes in reading and their role in reading skill.In A. M. Lesgold and C. A. Perfetti (eds.).Interactive Processes in Reading. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petrauskas, R. J., and Rourke, B. P. 1979. Identification of subtypes of retarded readers: A neuropsychological, multivariate approach.Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology 1:17–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., and Snyder, C. R. R. 1975. Attention and cognitive control.In R. L. Solso (ed.).Information Processing and Cognition: The Loyola symposium. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richman, L. C., and Kitchell, M. M. 1981. Hyperlexia as a variant of developmental language disorder.Brain and Language 12:203–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rudel, R. G., Denckla, M. B., and Broman, M. 1981. The effect of varying stimulus context on word-finding ability: Dyslexia further differentiated from other learning disabilities.Brain and Language 13:130–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rudel, R. G., Denckla, M. B., and Spalten, E. 1976. Paired associate learning of Morse Code and Braille letter names by dyslexic and normal children.Cortex 12:61–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E. 1977. Toward an interactive model of reading.In S. Dornic (ed.).Attention and Performance VI. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E., and McClelland, J. L. 1981. Interactive processing through spreading activation.In A. M. Lesgold and C. A. Perfetti (eds.).Interactive Processes in Reading. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E., and McClelland, J. L. 1982. An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 2. The contextual enchancement effect and some tests and extensions of the model.Psychological Review 89:60–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schvaneveldt, R., Ackerman, B. P., and Semlear, T. 1977. The effect of semantic context on children’s word recognition.Child Development 48:612–616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schvaneveldt, R., and Meyer, D. E. 1973. Retrieval and comparison processes in semantic memory.In S. Kornblum (ed.).Attention and Performance IV. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smiley, S., Oakley, D., Worthen, D., Campione, J., and Brown, A. 1977. Recall of thematically relevant material by adolescent good and poor readers as a function of written versus oral presentation.Journal of Educational Psychology 69:381–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, F. 1971.Understanding Reading. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, F. 1973.Psycholinguistics and Reading. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. 1981. Phonemic deficits in developmental dyslexia.Psychological Research 43:219–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spiro, R. J., and Tirre, W. C. 1980. Individual differences in schema utilization during discourse processing.Journal of Educational Psychology 72:204–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. 1980. Toward an interactive-compensatory model of individual differences in the development of reading fluency.Reading Research Quarterly 16:32–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. 1982. Individual differences in the cognitive processes of reading: I. Word decoding.Journal of Learning Disabilities 15:485–493.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndyke, P. W., and Stasz, C. 1980. Individual differences in procedures for knowledge acquisition from maps.Cognitive Psychology 12:137–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J., and Goldman, T. 1977. Verbal rehearsal and short-term memory in reading disabled children.Child Development 48:56–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E., and Gold, C. 1963. Stimulus information and contextual information as determinants of tachistoscopic recognition of words.Journal of Experimental Psychology 66:319–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. R. 1978. Toward an understanding of dyslexia: Psychological factors in specific reading disability.In A. L. Benton and D. Pearl (eds.).Dyslexia: An appraisal of current knowledge. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vogel, S. A. 1975.Syntactic Abilities in Normal and Dyslexic Children. Baltimore: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vogel, S. A. 1977. Morphological ability in normal and dyslexic children.Journal of Learning Disabilities 10:35–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wanat, S. F. 1971.Linguistic Structure and Visual Attention in Reading. Newark, Del.: International Reading Association Research Reports.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wepman, J. M. 1961. The interrelationships of hearing, speech and reading.The Reading Teacher 14:245–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • West, R. F., and Stanovich, K. E. 1978. Automatic contextual facilitation in readers of three ages.Child Development 49:717–727.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • West, R. F., and Stanovich, K. E. 1979. The development of automatic word recognition skills.Journal of Reading Behavior 11:211–219.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

Preparation of this review was supported by funds from the Medical Research Council of Canada, the Ontario Mental Health Foundation/COMSOC Provincial Lottery Grants Program, and The Hospital for Sick Children Foundation.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lovett, M.W. The search for subtypes of specific reading disability: Reflections from a cognitive perspective. Annals of Dyslexia 34, 153–178 (1984). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02663618

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02663618

Keywords

Navigation