Skip to main content
Log in

Enhancement of continuous performance task reaction time by smoking in non-deprived smokers

  • Original Investigations
  • Published:
Psychopharmacology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In a test of thewithdrawal-deficit hypothesis of the cognitive effects of cigarette smoking, non-deprived smokers participated in two sessions held on consecutive days. In both sessions, subjects performed two 20-min continuous performance tasks (CPTs). The CPT was a relatively “easy” version designed to require minimal practice (digit 0 target response; digits 1–9 nontarget response; 19% of stimuli targets). In one session, subjects smoked a cigarette prior to each CPT; in the other session they did not smoke (session order counterbalanced). Reaction time (RT) was significantly faster in the smoking session than in the non-smoking session with no difference in number of incorrect responses, a finding incompatible with the withdrawal-deficit hypothesis. Further, light inhalers (as assessed by pre-smoking to post-task increase in expired air carbon monoxide) appeared to process nontarget stimuli faster than deep inhalers, especially in the no smoking session. The results also indicated that, at least during the first CPT of each session, the performance of females in the no smoking session was poorer than in the smoking session and poorer than males regardless of session. In the latter part of the first CPT, the performance of males in the smoking session was better than their performance in the no smoking session. No clear pattern emerged for the second CPT.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Benowitz NL (1986) The human pharmacology of nicotine. In: Cappell HD et al (eds) Research advances in alcohol and drug problems, vol 9. Plenum Press, New York, pp 1–52. Tollison RD (ed) Smoking and society: toward a more balanced assessment. Heath, Lexington MA, pp 89–134

    Google Scholar 

  • Edwards JA, Wesnes K, Warburton DM, Gale A (1985) Evidence of more rapid stimulus evaluation following cigarette smoking. Addict Behav 10:113–126

    Google Scholar 

  • Frearson W, Barrett P, Eysenck HJ (1988) Intelligence, reaction time, and the effects of smoking. Person Individ Diff 9:497–517

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert DG, Robinson JH, Chamberlin CL, Spielberger CD (1989) Effects of smoking/nicotine on anxiety, heart rate, and lateralization of EEG during a stressful movie. Psychophysiology 26:311–320

    Google Scholar 

  • Golding J, Mangan GL (1982) Arousing and dearousing effects of cigarette smoking under conditions of stress and mild sensory isolation. Psychophysiology 19:449–456

    Google Scholar 

  • Hasenfratz M, Michel C, Nil R, Bättig K (1989a) Can smoking increase attention in rapid information processing during noise? Electrocortical, physiological, and behavioral effects. Psychopharmacology 98:75–80

    Google Scholar 

  • Hasenfratz M, Pfiffner D, Pellaud K, Bättig K (1989b) Postlunch smoking for pleasure seeking or arousal maintenance? Pharmacol Biochem Behav 34:631–639

    Google Scholar 

  • Heishman SJ, Snyder FR, Henningfield JE (1990) Effects of repeated nicotine administration. In: Harris L (ed) Problems of drug dependence 1990: Proceedings of the 52nd annual meeting of the committee on problems of drug dependence (NIDA Research Monograph 105). National Institute of Drug Abuse, Rockville, MD, pp 314–315

    Google Scholar 

  • Herning RI, Brigham J, Stitzer ML, Glover BJ, Pickworth WB, Henningfield JE (1990) The effects of nicotine on information processing: medicating a deficit. Psychophysiology 27:S2

  • Hindmarch I, Kerr JS, Sherwood N (1990) Effects of nicotine gum on psychomotor performance in smokers and non-smokers. Psychopharmacology 100:535–541

    Google Scholar 

  • Kerr JS, Sherwood N, Hindmarch I (1991) Separate and combined effects of the social drugs on psychomotor performance. Psychopharmacology 104:113–119

    Google Scholar 

  • Landers DM, Lindholm E, Crews DJ, Koriath JJ (1990) Cigarette smoking and smokeless tobacco facilitate information processing and performance. Psychophysiology 27:S47

  • Leigh G, Tong JE, Campbell JA (1977) Effects of ethanol and tobacco on divided attention. J Stud Alcohol 38:1233–1239

    Google Scholar 

  • Michel Ch, Bättig K (1988) Separate and joint effects of cigarette smoking and alcohol consumption on mental performance and physiological functions. Act Nerv Super 30:107–109

    Google Scholar 

  • Michel Ch, Bättig K (1989) Separate and combined psychophysiological effects of cigarette smoking and alcohol consumption. Psychopharmacology 97:65–73

    Google Scholar 

  • Michel Ch, Hasenfratz M, Nil R, Bättig K (1988) Cardiovascular, electrocortical, and behavioral effects of nicotine chewing gum. Klin Wochenschr 66 [Suppl XI]:72–79

    Google Scholar 

  • Nil R, Woodson PP, Michel C, Bättig K (1988) Effects of smoking on mental performance and vegetative functions in high and low CO absorbing smokers. Klin Wochenschr 66 [Suppl XI]:66–71

    Google Scholar 

  • Parrott AC, Winder G (1989) Nicotine chewing gum (2 mg, 4 mg) and cigarette smoking: comparative effects upon vigilance and heart rate. Psychopharmacology 99:257–261

    Google Scholar 

  • Petrie RXA, Deary IJ (1989) Smoking and human information processing. Psychopharmacology 99:393–396

    Google Scholar 

  • Pritchard WS (1990) Cigarette smoking and the human EEG. Psychophysiology 27:S1

  • Pritchard WS (1991a) Electroencephalographic effects of cigarette smoking. Psychopharmacology 104:485–490

    Google Scholar 

  • Pritchard WS (1991b) The link between smoking and P: a serotonergic hypothesis. Person Individ Diff 12:1187–1204

    Google Scholar 

  • Pritchard WS, Robinson JH, de Bethizy JD, Davis RA (1991) Effects of smoking and caffeine on EEG, heart rate, performance, and anxiety/mental alertness/muscular tension. Psychophysiology 28:S44

    Google Scholar 

  • Provost SC, Woodward R (1991) Effects of nicotine gum on repeated administration of the Stroop test. Psychopharmacology 104:536–540

    Google Scholar 

  • Revell AD (1988) Smoking and performance — a puff-by-puff analysis. Psychopharmacology 96:563–565

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson JH, Pritchard WS, Davis RA (1992) Psychopharmacological effects of smoking a cigarette with typical “tar” and carbon monoxide yields but minimal nicotine. Psychopharmacology 108:466–472

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherwood N, Kerr JS, Hindmarch I (1990) No differences in the psychomotor response of heavy, light and non-smokers to the acute administration of nicotine. Med Sci Res 18:839–840

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherwood N, Kerr JS, Hindmarch I (1991) Effects of nicotine on short-term memory. In: Adlkofer F, Thurau K (eds) Effects of nicotine on biological systems. Birkhäuser, Basel, pp 531–535

    Google Scholar 

  • Snyder FR, Davis FC, Henningfield JE (1989) The tobacco withdrawal syndrome: performance decrements assessed on a computerized test battery. Drug Alcohol Depend 23:259–266

    Google Scholar 

  • Spielberger CD (1986) Psychological determinants of smoking behavior. In: Tollison RD (ed) Smoking and society: toward a more balanced assessment. Heath, Lexington MA, pp 89–134

    Google Scholar 

  • Warburton DM (1988) The functional use of nicotine. In: Wald NJ, Froggatt P (eds) Nicotine, smoking and the low tar programme. Oxford, London, pp 182–199

  • Warburton DM, Revell A, Walters AC (1988) Nicotine as a resource. In: Rand M, Turau K (eds) The pharmacology of nicotine. IRL, London, pp 359–373

    Google Scholar 

  • Wesnes K, Revell A (1984) The separate and combined effects of scopolamine and nicotine on human information processing. Psychopharmacology 84:5–11

    Google Scholar 

  • Wesnes K, Warburton DM (1978) The effects of cigarette smoking and nicotine tablets upon human attention. In: Thornton RE (ed) Smoking behaviour. Physiological and psychological influences. Churchill Livingstone, New York, pp 131–147

    Google Scholar 

  • Wesnes K, Warburton DM (1983) Effects of smoking on rapid information processing performance. Neuropsychobiology 9:223–229

    Google Scholar 

  • Wesnes K, Warburton DM (1984a) Smoking, nicotine, and human performance. In: Balfour DJK (ed) Nicotine and the tobacco smoking habit. Pergamon Press, Oxford, pp 133–152

    Google Scholar 

  • Wesnes K, Warburton, DM (1984b) The effects of cigarettes of varying yield on rapid information processing performance. Psychopharmacology 82:338–342

    Google Scholar 

  • Wesnes K, Warburton DM (1984c) Effects of scopolamine and nicotine on human rapid information processing performance. Psychopharmacology 82:147–150

    Google Scholar 

  • Wesnes K, Warburton DM, Matz, B (1983) Effects of nicotine on stimulus sensitivity and response bias in a visual vigilance task. Neuropsychobiology 9:41–44

    Google Scholar 

  • West RJ (1990) Nicotine pharmacodynamics: Some unresolved issues. In Bock G, Marsh J (eds) The biology of nicotine dependence. Wiley, Chichester, pp 210–224

    Google Scholar 

  • West RJ, Hack S (1991) Effects of cigarettes on memory search and subjective ratings. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 38:281–286

    Google Scholar 

  • West RJ, Jarvis MJ (1986) Effects of nicotine on finger tapping rate in non-smokers. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 25:727–731

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams DG (1980) Effects of cigarette smoking on immediate memory and performance in different kinds of smoker. Br J Psychol 71:83–90

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams DG, Tata PR, Miskella J (1984) Different “types” of cigarette smokers received similar effects from smoking. Addict Behav 9:207–210

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Pritchard, W.S., Robinson, J.H. & Guy, T.D. Enhancement of continuous performance task reaction time by smoking in non-deprived smokers. Psychopharmacology 108, 437–442 (1992). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02247417

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02247417

Key words

Navigation