Skip to main content
Log in

The Maudsley reactive and nonreactive strains: A North American perspective

  • Inbred Strains as Model Systems
  • Published:
Behavior Genetics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The Maudsley experiment involving bidirectional selection for differences in open-field defecation (OFD) is briefly reviewed. Attention is focused on the North American stocks of these strains since problems have been encountered with regard to the open-field behavior and continued viability of the British stocks. The open-field behavior of the colonies of the Maudsley strains at the National Institutes of Health and at the University of Northern Iowa closely resembles that originally exhibited by the British strains, provided they are compared under standard conditions. On more complex behaviors, such as escape-avoidance conditioning, serious discrepancies have emerged between British and North American findings. These may be resolved when attempts are made to replicate more closely the procedures used in the British studies. Particular emphasis has been placed on the potential of a second nonreactive strain, the MNRA/HAR strain, to serve as a replicate of selection for decreased emotionality. The value of the RXNRA group of recombinant-inbred strains, developed and maintained by the author, for examining the reliability of associations between traits is also stressed. A correlation between OFD and biochemical aspects of sympathetic function has been found in the Maudsley strains. This finding lends credibility to Hall's claim that the expression of emotionality is linked to autonomic arousal.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Archer, J. (1975). The Maudsley Reactive and Non-reactive strains of rats: The need for an objective evaluation.Behav. Genet. 5:411–415.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bailey, D. W. (1971). Recombinant-inbred strains; An aid to finding identity, linkage and function of histocompatibility and other genes.Transplantation 11:419–422.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blizard, D. A. (1981). Unpublished observations.

  • Blizard, D. A., and Liang, B. (1979). Central serotonergic function and behavior in the Maudsley Reactive and Non-reactive strains: A revaluation.Behav. Genet. 9:413–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blizard, D. A., Liang, B., and Emmel, D. K. (1980). Blood pressure, heart rate, and plasma catecholamines under resting conditions in rat strains selectively bred for differences in response to stress.Behav. Neur. Biol. 29:487–492.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blizard, D. A., Hansen, C., and Freedman, L. S. (1981). Open-field behavior of the Maudsley strains maintained at the National Institutes of Health, submitted.

  • Broadhurst, P. L. (1957a).Emotionality in the rat: A Study of its Determinants, Inheritance and Relation to Some Aspects of Motivation, Ph.D. thesis, University of London, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadhurst, P. L. (1957b). Determinants of emotionality in the rat. I. Situational factors.Br. J. Psychol. 48:1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadhurst, P. L. (1958). Determinant of emotionality in the rat. II. Antecedent factors.Br. J. Psychol. 49:56–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadhurst, P. L. (1960). Experiments in psychogenetics. In Eysenck, H. J. (ed.),Experiments in Personality, Psychogenetics and Psychopharmacology, Vol. 1, Routledge and Kegan Paul, London, pp. 3–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadhurst, P. L. (1961). Analysis of maternal effects in the inheritance of behavior.Anim. Behav. 9:129–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadhurst, P. L. (1962). A note on further progress in a psychogenetic selection experiment.Psychol. Rep. 10:65–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadhurst, P. L. (1975). The Maudsley Reactive and Non-reactive strains of rats: A survey.Behav. Genet. 5:299–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadhurst, P. L. (1979). Availability of behaviorally selected strains of rats.Behav. Genet. 9:323–324.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadhurst, P. L., and Levine, S. (1963). Behavioral consistency in strains of rats selectively bred for emotional elimination.Br. J. Psychol. 56(2):121–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeFries, J. C. (1967). Quantitative genetics and behavior: overview and perspective. In Hirsch, J. (ed.),Behavior-Genetic Analysis, McGraw-Hill, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeFries, J. C. (1980). Selective breeding to behavioral and pharmacological responses in laboratory mice. In Gershon, E. G., Matthysse, S., Breakfield, X. O., and Ciaranello, R. D. (eds.),Genetic Research Strategies for Psychobiology and Psychiatry, Boxwood Press.

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1967).The Biological Basis of Personality, Charles C Thomas, Springfield, Ill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J., and Broadhurst, P. L. (1964). Experiments with animals: Introduction. In Eysenck, H. J. (ed.),Experiments in Motivation, Pergamon, Oxford, pp. 285–291.

    Google Scholar 

  • Falconer, D. S. (1960).Introduction to Quantitative Genetics, Ronald Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Festing, M. F. W., and Staats, J. (1973). Standardized Nomenclature for inbred strains of rats.Transplantation 16:221–245.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feuer, G. (1963). Adrenal cortical activity and stress response in rats selectively bred for differences in behavior.J. Physiol. 169:43P-44P.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feuer, G., and Broadhurst, P. L. (1962a). Thyroid function in rats selectively bred for emotional elimination. I. Differences in thyroid hormones.J. Endocrinol. 24:127–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feuer, G., and Broadhurst, P. L. (1962b). Thyroid function in rats selectively bred for emotional elimination. II. Differences in thyroid activity.J. Endocrinol. 24:253–262.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feuer, G., and Broadhurst, P. L. (1962c). Thyroid function in rats selectively bred for emotional elimination. III. Behavioral and physiological changes after treatment with drugs acting on the thyroid.J. Endocrinol. 24:385–396.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuller, J. L., and Thompson, W. R. (1978).Foundations of Behavior Genetics, C. V. Mosby, St. Louis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. A. (1971).The Psychology of Fear and Stress, McGraw-Hill, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gupta, B. D., and Holland, H. C. (1969). An examination of the effects of stimulant and depressant drugs on escape/avoidance conditioning in strains of rats selectively bred for emotionality/non-emotionality: Inter-trial activity.Int. J. Neuropharmacol. 8:227–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, C. S. (1934). Emotional behavior in the rat. I. Defecation and urination as measures of individual differences in emotionality.J. Comp. Psychol. 18:385–403.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, C. S. (1936). Emotional behavior in the rat. III. The relationship between emotionality and ambulatory activity.J. Comp. Psychol. 22:345–352.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, G. M. (1972). Strain differences in open-field behavior of the rat.Psychonom. Sci. 27:51–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, G. M. (1979a). Strain differences in open-field behavior of the rat. II.Bull. Psychonom. Soc. 13:85–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, G. M. (1979b). Strain differences in shuttle avoidance conditioning in the rat.Bull. Psychonom. Soc. 13:161–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, G. M. (1981). The Har strains of rats: Origins and characteristics.Behav. Genet. 11:445–468.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joffe, J. M. (1964). Avoidance learning and failure to learn in two strains of rats selectively bred for emotionality.Psychonom. Sci. 1:185–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levine, S., and Broadhurst, P. L. (1963). Genetic and ontogenetic influences on later emotionality in the rat.J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 56:423–428.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liang, B., and Blizard, D. A. (1978). Central and peripheral norepinephrine concentrations in rat strains selectively bred for differences in response to stress: confirmation and extension.Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 8:75–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maas, J. W. (1962). Neurochemical differences between two strains of mice.Science 137:621.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maas, J. W. (1963). Neurochemical differences between two strains of mice.Nature 197:255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Owen, S. (1962). The effect on avoidance response extinction in rats of CS continuation and emotional constitution.J. Genet. Psychol. 103:147–151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rick, J. T., and Fulker, D. W. (1972). Some biochemical correlates of inherited behavioral differences. In Bradley, P. B., and Brimblecombe, R. W. (eds.),Progress in Brain Research—Biochemical and Pharmacological Mechanisms Underlying Behavior, Vol. 36, Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp. 105–113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Satinder, K. P. (1971). Genotype-dependent effects of d-amphetamine sulphate and caffeine on escape-avoidance behavior of rats.J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 76:359–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Satinder, K. P. (1972). Effects of inter-trial crossing, punishment and d-amphetamine sulfate on avoidance and activity in four selectively bred rat strains.Psychonom. Sci. 29:291–293.

    Google Scholar 

  • Satinder, K. P. (1976). Sensory responsiveness and avoidance learning in rats.J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 90(10):946–957.

    Google Scholar 

  • Satinder, K. P. (1980). Personal communication.

  • Savage, R. D., and Eysenck, H. J. (1964). The definition and measurement of emotionality. In Eysenck, H. J. (ed.),Experiments in Motivation, Pergamon, Oxford, pp. 292–314.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singh, S. D. (1959). Conditioned emotional response in the rat. I. Constitutional and situational determinants.J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 52:574–578.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singh, S. D., and Eysenck, H. J. (1960). Conditioned emotional response in the rat. III. Drug antagonism.J. Gen. Psychol. 63:275–285.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slater, J., Blizard, D. A., and Pohorecky, L. A. (1977). Central and peripheral norepinephrine metabolism in rat strains selectively bred for differences in response to stress.Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 6:511–520.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sudak, H. S., and Maas, J. W. (1964). Behavioral-neurochemical correlation in Reactive and Non-reactive strains of rats.Science 46:418–420.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, B. A. (1971). Recombinant-inbred lines. A new approach to genetic analysis. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Institute of Biological Sciences, Fort Collins, Colo.

  • Wahlsten, D. (1978). Behavioral genetics and animal learning. In Anisman, H., and Bignami, G. (eds.),Psychopharmacology of Aversively Motivated Behavior, Plenum Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, R. H. J. (1960). Constitutional differences between 2 strains of rats with different behavioral characteristics. In Karger, S. (ed.),Advances in Psychosomatic Medicine, Basel and New York, pp. 160–165.

  • Womack, J. E. (1981). The linkage map of the rat.Behav. Genet. 11:437–444.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

Preparation of this article was supported by grant GM 28874 from the National Institute of General Medical Sciences.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Blizard, D.A. The Maudsley reactive and nonreactive strains: A North American perspective. Behav Genet 11, 469–489 (1981). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01070004

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01070004

Key Words

Navigation