Advertisement

Motivation and Emotion

, Volume 8, Issue 4, pp 343–353 | Cite as

Effects of induced mood on causal attributions for success and failure

  • Jonathon Brown
Article

Abstract

The present research examined the influence of induced mood on causal perception and goal expectancies for an achievement-oriented activity. Prior to the administration of a laboratory task, positive and negative moods were experimentally induced in student volunteers. It was found that elated subjects were initially more confident of success than subjects temporarily made to feel depressed. Furthermore, following the receipt of bogus success/failure feedback, individuals in a positive mood perceived the causes of success as more stable than subjects in a negative mood. In addition, the judgments of elated subjects appear to have been biased in a self-enhancing direction following success, but outcome had no effect on the causal attributions of subjects temporarily induced to feel depressed. The findings indicate that prevailing affective state is an important determinant of causal perception, and suggest that mood may play a central role in the accurate or biased perception of valenced outcomes.

Keywords

Social Psychology Important Determinant Affective State Negative Mood Positive Mood 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Abramson, L. Y., & Alloy, L. B. (1981). Depression, nondepression, and cognitive illusions: Reply to Schwartz.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110 436–447.Google Scholar
  2. Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87 49–74.Google Scholar
  3. Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1979). Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder but wiser?Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108 441–485.Google Scholar
  4. Alloy, L. B., Abramson, L. Y., & Viscusi, D. (1981). Induced mood and the illusion of control.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41 1129–1140.Google Scholar
  5. Beck, A. T. (1967).Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  6. Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory.American Psychologist, 36 129–148.Google Scholar
  7. Brown, J., & Taylor, S. E. (1984).Affect and the processing of personal information. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  8. Brown, J., & Weiner, B. (1984). Affective consequences of ability versus effort: Controversies, resolutions, and quandaries.Journal of Educational Psychology, 76 146–158.Google Scholar
  9. Coyne, J. C., & Gotlib, I. H. (1983). The role of cognition in depression: A critical appraisal.Psychological Bulletin, 94 472–505.Google Scholar
  10. Feather, N. T., & Simon, J. G. (1971a). Causal attributions for success and failure in relation to expectations of success based on selective or manipulative control.Journal of Personality, 39 527–544.Google Scholar
  11. Feather, N. T., & Simon, J. G. (1971b). Attribution of responsibility and valence of outcome in relation to initial confidence and success and failure for self and other.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18 173–188.Google Scholar
  12. Frost, R. O., & Green, M. L. (1982). Velten mood induction procedure effects: Duration and post-experimental removal.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 8 341–347.Google Scholar
  13. Garber, J., & Hollon, S. D. (1980). Universal versus personal helplessness in depression. Belief in uncontrollability or incompetence?Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89 56–66.Google Scholar
  14. Goodwin, A. M., & Williams, J. M. G. (1982). Mood induction research: Its implications for clinical depression.Behavior Research and Therapy, 20 373–382.Google Scholar
  15. Klein, D. C., Fencil-Morse, E., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1976). Learned helplessness, depression, and the attribution of failure.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33 508–516.Google Scholar
  16. Kuiper, N. A., & MacDonald, M. R. (1982). Self and other perception in mild depressives.Social Cognition, 1 223–239.Google Scholar
  17. Lazarus, R. S. (1984). On the primacy of cognition.American Psychologist, 39 124–129.Google Scholar
  18. Levine, M. (1966). Hypothesis behavior by humans during discrimination learning.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71 331–338.Google Scholar
  19. Mukherji, B. R., Abramson, L. Y., & Martin, D. J. (1982). Induced depressive mood and attributional patterns.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 6 15–21.Google Scholar
  20. Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1984). Causal explanations as a risk factor for depression. Theory and evidence.Psychological Review, 91 347–374.Google Scholar
  21. Ronis, D. L., Hansen, R. D., & O'Leary, V. E. (1983). Understanding the meaning of achievement attributions: A test of derived locus and stability scores.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44 702–711.Google Scholar
  22. Teasdale, J. D. (1983). Negative thinking in depression: Cause, effect, or reciprocal relationship? In L. Joyce-Moniz, F. Lowe, & P. Higson (Eds.),Theoretical issues in cognitive-behavioral therapy. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  23. Teasdale, J. D., & Fogarty, S. J. (1979). Differential effects of induced mood on the retrieval of pleasant and unpleasant events from episodic memory.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88 248–257.Google Scholar
  24. Velten, E. (1967).The induction of elation and depression through the reading of structured sets of mood-statements. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Southern California.Google Scholar
  25. Velten, E. (1968). A laboratory task for induction of mood states.Behavior Research and Therapy, 6 473–483.Google Scholar
  26. Weiner, B. (1979). A theory of motivation for some classroom experiences.Journal of Educational Psychology, 71 3–25.Google Scholar
  27. Weiner, B. (1982). The emotional consequences of causal ascriptions. In M. S. Clark & S. T. Fiske (Eds.),Affect and cognition (pp. 185–209). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  28. Weiner, B. (1983). Some methodological pitfalls in attributional research.Journal of Educational Psychology, 75 530–543.Google Scholar
  29. Weiner, B., Frieze, I. H., Kukla, A., Reed, L., Rest, S., & Rosenbaum, R. M. (1971).Perceiving the causes of success and failure. Morristown, New Jersey: General Learning Press.Google Scholar
  30. Weiner, B., Russell, D., & Lerman, D. (1978). Affective consequences of causal ascriptions. In J. H. Harvey, W. J. Ickes, & F. R. Kidd (Eds.),New directions in attribution research (Vol. 2, pp. 59–90). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  31. Weiner, B., Russell, D., & Lerman, D. (1979). The cognition-emotion process in achievement-related contexts.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37 1211–1220.Google Scholar
  32. Wright, J., & Mischel, W. (1982). Influence of affect on cognitive social learning person variables.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43 901–914.Google Scholar
  33. Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences.American Psychologist, 35 151–175.Google Scholar
  34. Zajonc, R. B. (1984). On the primacy of affect.American Psychologist, 39 117–123.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1984

Authors and Affiliations

  • Jonathon Brown
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of CaliforniaLos Angeles

Personalised recommendations